Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
22 June 2017 Sexual Isolation between Two Sympatric Desmognathus in the Gulf Coastal Plain
Jennifer Y. Lamb
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

A substantial body of work exists examining courtship in lungless salamanders (family Plethodontidae), but there are many species and population pairs that have not been tested for sexual isolation. Spotted (Desmognathus conanti sensu lato [SL]) and Southern Dusky Salamanders (D. aff. auriculatus) occur sympatrically within drainages in South Mississippi. I crossed individuals from three populations of D. aff. auriculatus (n = 6 individuals) and seven populations of D. conanti (SL) (n = 10) occurring in the Pascagoula and Pearl River basins. I alternated homospecific (n = 23) and heterospecific (n = 23) trials across nights in the spring and summer of 2014 and 2015, and I used GoPro™ HERO3 cameras and time-lapse photography to record the behaviors of a random subset of these pairs, as well as to determine the stage of courtship reached. Seventeen of the 23 homospecific trials resulted in spermatophore deposition and 14 in insemination. None of the 23 heterospecific trials (D. aff. auriculatus ♂ x D. conanti [SL]♀; or D. conanti [SL]♂ x D. aff. auriculatus ♀) reached this stage of courtship, but ten of the 14 photographed, heterospecific pairs exhibited pursuit and/or persuasive behaviors (i.e., pheromone transfer). These results suggest that D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) are completely sexually isolated and that isolation is likely chemically mediated. I used a total of eight polymorphic microsatellite loci to genotype offspring from two clutches oviposited by a single female D. aff. auriculatus and one clutch oviposited by a female D. conanti (SL). Strict exclusion suggested that each clutch was sired by a single male, and that female D. aff. auriculatus and female D. conanti (SL) were capable of storing sperm for at least 4 mo 5 days and 3 mo 3 days, respectively. The data presented here further our understanding of the reproductive ecologies of Desmognathus in the historically understudied Gulf Coastal Plain.

REPRODUCTIVE isolation—one line of evidence that can be used in species delineation (de Queiroz, 2005)—occurs when populations are unable to successfully interbreed and create fertile offspring. This phenomenon can occur prior to (i.e., pre-zygotic) or after (i.e., post-zygotic) a sperm fertilizes an egg cell (Coyne and Orr, 2004). There are many examples of incomplete reproductive isolation among Caudata (Voss and Shaffer, 1996; Mead and Verrell, 2002; Tilley, 2016), and there has been an impressive amount of work investigating sexual incompatibility among the woodland (genus Plethodon) and dusky (genus Desmognathus) salamanders (family Plethodontidae; Arnold et al., 1993; Mabry and Verrell, 2004). Sexual incompatibility, also known as sexual isolation (Dobzhansky, 1935), is a type of pre-zygotic reproductive isolation encompassing scenarios in which the act of insemination does not occur (Dobzhansky, 1935; Arnold et al., 1993; Mabry and Verrell, 2004). The family Plethodontidae is an interesting group with which to examine a variety of hypotheses pertaining to sexual isolation. This family boasts of a large number of species that are distributed both allopatrically and sympatrically (Lannoo, 2005), and many species can easily be maintained and will readily court in the laboratory (Arnold et al., 1993). Courtship behaviors appear to be remarkably similar across congeneric taxa (Verrell, 1999); therefore, we can ask whether sexual isolation is maintained by behavioral and/or chemical cues (Arnold et al., 1993). Finally, because the degree of reproductive isolation between not only different species, but also among conspecific populations, varies with geographic and genetic distance (Mabry and Verrell, 2004; Tilley, 2016) there are opportunities to examine the role of both distances in the evolution and maintenance of sexual isolation.

Species that are broadly sympatric may exhibit a greater degree of sexual isolation than those with narrow overlap along the peripheral portions of their ranges. Complete sexual isolation has been observed between multiple pairs of sympatric species of plethodontids, but pre- and post-zygotic barriers can be leaky (Mabry and Verrell, 2004; Tilley, 2016). For example, in situ hybridization has been documented between Northern (D. fuscus) and Allegheny Mountain Dusky (D. ochrophaeus) salamanders (Karlin and Guttman, 1981; Sharbel et al., 1995). Fewer studies examining sexual isolation have been undertaken for taxa which occur in the Gulf Coastal Plain (Verrell, 1990a; Kozak, 2003; Mabry and Verrell, 2004).

Both the Southern Dusky (D. auriculatus; Holbrook, 1838) and the Spotted Dusky (D. conanti; Rossman, 1958) salamanders have widely overlapping ranges within the Gulf Coastal Plain (Means, 2005; Means and Bonett, 2005). Populations attributed to D. auriculatus in Mississippi and Louisiana are genetically (Lamb, 2016) and morphologically distinct from topotypic D. auriculatus in Georgia and Florida, and proposed taxonomic revisions for this historically confounding group are pending (Means et al., in press). Hereafter I refer to these populations of salamanders in Mississippi and Louisiana as D. aff. auriculatus. Desmognathus conanti also includes multiple, divergent genetic lineages (Karlin and Guttman, 1986; Bonett, 2002; Kozak et al., 2005; Beamer and Lamb, 2008; Lamb, 2016), but their taxonomic status is uncertain. Hereafter populations of this species are referred to as D. conanti sensu lato (SL).

Desmognathus aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) have the opportunity to interact in situ. These species occur within the same tributaries in the Pearl and Pascagoula rivers in South Mississippi, and they can occasionally be found in the same microhabitats at lotic sites (Valentine, 1963; Means et al., in press). Individuals, especially older males (Valentine, 1963), are grossly phenotypically similar and reproductive adults overlap in body size (e.g., male D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti [SL] mean snout-to-vent lengths [SVL] are 56±6 mm and 49±4 mm, respectively; female mean SVLs are 53±5 mm and 48±9 mm, respectively; Means et al., in press). Our limited knowledge of the reproductive phenologies of both species in this part of their ranges suggests that their breeding seasons partially coincide. For example, I encountered a female D. conanti (SL) from a Pascagoula population (Jones Co.) with a sperm cap in her cloaca in May, and I have observed both of these species tending eggs in the late summer (i.e., D. conanti [SL] from a Pearl River population, Marion Co.), and eggs or very recent hatchlings in the fall (i.e., D. conanti [SL] from Jackson Co. and D. aff. auriculatus from Forrest Co. in October; unpubl. data). Males with prominent mental glands can be found throughout most of the year for both taxa (unpubl. data). Histological studies of populations of D. conanti (SL) elsewhere in the Gulf Coastal Plain suggest that these glands do not fully atrophy and that males may be capable of breeding throughout the year (Rupp, 2016). Given the capacity for female plethodontids to store sperm for anywhere from two to greater than nine months (Marynick, 1971; Sever, 2003; Eddy et al., 2015) as well as the prolonged breeding season in some species, it is feasible that receptive adults of both D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) co-occur spatially and temporally.

I completed multiple crosses in captivity of D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) from several populations in South Mississippi to test whether these salamanders were sexually isolated within the sympatric portions of their ranges. I used GoPro HERO3 cameras (GoPro, Inc., San Mateo, CA) and time-lapse photography to identify the stage of courtship reached as well as to compare behaviors between species. Upon the completion of courtship trials, I allowed females to oviposit naturally and then used multiple microsatellite loci (Lamb et al., 2015) to genotype any resulting offspring. This allowed me to identify the successful sire(s) for each clutch and to determine the minimum period of time over which females were capable of storing viable sperm.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Collection and maintenance of animals.—Desmognathus aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) can be difficult to distinguish in situ; however, there are discrete phenotypic differences that genetic studies have confirmed are reliable (Means et al., in press). Between 3 April 2014 and 2 February 2015 I collected six D. aff. auriculatus and ten D. conanti (SL) from nine localities in the Pascagoula and Pearl river drainages in South Mississippi (Fig. 1). I measured snout-to-vent lengths (SVL) and sexed salamanders in the field using the presence of an obvious mental gland to identify males (SVL three D. aff. auriculatus: 55 mm, range = 48–63 mm; SVL six D. conanti [SL]: 51 mm, range = 42–59 mm) and ovarian follicles visible through the ventral skin to identify females (SVL three D. aff. auriculatus: 60 mm, range = 58–62 mm; SVL four D. conanti [SL]: 51 mm, range = 47–55 mm). I transported salamanders in individual, re-sealable plastic bags to laboratory facilities at the University of Southern Mississippi where I maintained each in separate, lidded plastic containers (21 × 15 x 10 cm) containing moist paper towels. I kept containers in an environmental chamber set to a 12 hr day:night cycle and constant 20°C. This temperature was within the range of what these species would experience in small streams during the summer months in situ (unpubl. data). Once or twice per week, I changed paper towels and fed each salamander between five and seven small crickets. I occasionally supplemented their diets with fruit flies (Drosophila spp.) and calcium dusted crickets.

Fig. 1. 

Collection localities in the Pascagoula and Pearl river drainages. The inset includes a portion of the Gulf Coastal Plain with state names abbreviated and the study area enclosed within a gray square. White and gray circles mark locations from which only D. aff. auriculatus or D. conanti (SL) were collected, respectively. The black square marks a locality from which both species were collected syntopically.

i0045-8511-105-2-261-f01.tif

Courtship encounters.—I staged courtship encounters from June–September 2014 and April–May 2015. Encounters took place during the nocturnal cycle, from 2000–0600 h, in lidded plastic shoe boxes (35 × 18 x 12 cm) lined with moist paper towels and under Mood-lites® Passion Red bulbs. I obscured the view between adjacent boxes by wrapping each box in brown paper towels. Homospecific encounters (n = 23 total) always preceded heterospecific encounters (n = 23 total), and individuals were typically given three nights between each encounter to recuperate (range = 2–5 nights; Verrell, 1988a, 1988b; Arnold et al., 1993). All but one pairing of salamanders was unique. A single male and female D. aff. auriculatus were paired in both 2014 and 2015 in order to ensure that each male met a homospecific prior to a heterospecific female. I allowed each female to acclimate for 30 min in the shoe box before adding a single male and beginning the trial. On the following morning, I visually inspected the paper towel and the bottom of the enclosure for remnants of spermatophores. I also inspected the cloaca of each female for the presence of a sperm cap, which can remain visible for ca. 24 hours post-insemination (Verrell, 1991).

GoPro HERO cameras have recently been used in behavioral studies with amphibians (Pašukonis et al., 2014; Greene et al., 2016; Lanctôt et al., 2016) and reptiles (Holding et al., 2015). The small size of these cameras and their ability to capture high resolution, wide-angle photos makes them well suited for documenting behavior in small, dimly lit arenas. I used GoPro HERO3 cameras and 30-second time-lapse photography (photo resolution = 1080 p) to record the full duration of 12 homospecific (seven D. aff. auriculatus; five D. conanti) and 14 heterospecific (six D. aff. auriculatus male x D. conanti [SL] female; eight D. conanti [SL] male x D. aff. auriculatus female) encounters. I chose the couples randomly, and I photographed each from directly above the shoe box via a small hole cut out of the center of the lid. I used the behavioral scale in Verrell and Arnold (1989) to identify the stage of courtship reached by each couple (i.e., 0. no interest; 1. pursuit; 2. persuasion; 3. sperm transfer) with specific behaviors scored using a modified ethogram (Table 1) informed by descriptions in Verrell (1995, 1997), Mead and Verrell (2002), and Plenderleith and Forester (2011). Since 30-second time-lapse photography cannot capture rapid behaviors with short durations such as “jerk,” “nudge,” or “butterfly” (Verrell, 1995, 1997), a comparison of the complete suite of courtship behaviors exhibited by these populations and species is beyond the scope of the present study and the ethogram presented is limited.

Table 1. 

Modified behavioral ethogram used to score time-lapse photography.

i0045-8511-105-2-261-t01.tif

Oviposition and tissue collection.—Each summer, after the conclusion of all courtship trials, I placed ovigerous females in individual shoe boxes designed to mimic nesting microhabitats and encourage oviposition. I maintained nesting enclosures under the same light and temperature regime as previously described. Each enclosure contained one or two cover items (i.e., pieces of wood or flat rocks) and clumps of moss (Sphagnum sp.) atop a substrate of approximately 1–2 cm of washed sand. I checked shoe boxes for clutches of eggs and provided females with a few small crickets no more frequently than once per week. I added small amounts of water or misted enclosures during these brief disturbances to keep the substrate very moist. When I discovered a clutch, I photographed it as found so that I could estimate the minimum number of eggs produced. I also photographed clutches opportunistically throughout their development. In most cases I allowed females to remain with their clutches until hatching. I had previously collected tissue samples from each adult for other studies. I collected tissue from offspring either by snipping the tail tips of hatchlings or by preserving eggs whole in 100% ethanol. I euthanized hatchlings and adults not involved in other studies with a 20% benzocaine solution (Brown et al., 2004) and preserved them in a 10% formalin solution. Adults were transferred to a 70% solution of ethyl alcohol and ultimately deposited at the Mississippi Museum of Natural Science to serve as locality vouchers.

Paternal exclusion.—I used the conditions specified in Lamb et al. (2015) to amplify six polymorphic microsatellite loci per species (D. aff. auriculatus: Dcon05, Dcon14, Dcon16, Dcon26, Dcon34, and Dcon36; D. conanti [SL]: Dcon12, Dcon14, Dcon26, Dcon34, Dcon36, and Dcon40) to genotype offspring, known mothers, and any males that had successfully inseminated those females during courtship trials. When individuals or loci amplified inconsistently, I adjusted PCR conditions to include a greater concentration of template (1.5–2 μL per reaction) and Taq (1.5X) and/or I added bovine serum albumin (1–2X). I visualized genotypes on a polyacrylamide gel using a LICOR 4300 DNA analyzer, and I sized alleles using GeneProfiler ver. 4.05 (LICOR Co.).

Multiple paternity has been documented in other Desmognathus (Tilley and Hausman, 1976; Houck et al., 1985; Adams et al., 2005), and clutches produced in this study could have been sired by more than one male during trials, or by males that a female encountered in the field prior to being collected. I was able to use complete exclusion to determine which captive male(s) were or were not the father(s) because the females that oviposited were only inseminated by a small number of males in captivity, and because the microsatellites being used were highly polymorphic (Lamb et al., 2015). Complete or strict exclusion is a conservative method of assigning paternity in which a male cannot be the father of a particular offspring if his genotype mismatches that individual at a single locus (Tennessen and Zamudio, 2003). Offspring with alleles not shared by any of the captive potential fathers are assumed to have been the result of sperm stored by the female after successful courtship in the field. This method of paternity analysis assumes that there has been no mutation or genotyping error. To avoid the latter issue, I based each individual's allele scores on the results from at least two successful, independent amplification attempts. I used GenAlEx 6.5 (Peakall and Smouse, 2012) to calculate allelic diversity and other summary statistics for each clutch.

RESULTS

Courtship encounters.—Insemination occurred in 54% of homospecific encounters involving D. aff. auriculatus and in 67% of homospecific encounters involving D. conanti (SL), and nearly all encounters between D. conanti (SL) resulted in the deposition of at least one spermatophore by the male (Table 2). In most encounters a male deposited a single spermatophore. However, two males, one of each species, deposited two spermatophores during an encounter, and a different male D. conanti (SL) deposited three spermatophores in one night.

Table 2. 

Proportion of encounters in which insemination and spermatophore deposition occurred. Six D. aff. auriculatus (three males and three females) and ten D. conanti (SL) (six males and four females) were used. The number of inseminations is given first and the number of encounters in which spermatophores were deposited is bracketed. Parentheses enclose the number of encounters of that type (23 homospecific and 23 heterospecific trials in total).

i0045-8511-105-2-261-t02.tif

In homospecific encounters, D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) exhibited similar behaviors, but they differed in 1) the proportion of couples across which behaviors were observed (Table 3), 2) the frequency of occurrence for certain behaviors within individual encounters, and 3) the duration of behaviors. There is insufficient power to complete statistical significance tests due to the small sample sizes used herein, but it is still useful to compare the behaviors observed across encounter types. The C-Position and Waltz (see Table 1), both of which occur during the persuasion stage (Stage 2), varied most notably between taxa. The C-Position may be a precursor to the “snap” (Mead and Verrell, 2002) or the Waltz (Verrell, 1997; pers. obs.). I observed this behavior in a larger proportion of encounters between D. conanti (SL) than D. aff. auriculatus (Table 3). Of those encounters in which I observed the C-Position, it was performed more frequently per encounter by male D. conanti (SL) (mean = 7.8 times per encounter, range = 1–16 times) than by male D. aff. auriculatus (mean = 4.3 times per encounter, range = 1–13 times). Further, on multiple occasions and in several encounters, I observed male D. conanti (SL) performing the C-Position across sequential photos but alternating the side to which they arched backwards, giving them the appearance of swinging from side to side as they led a female slowly forwards. This swinging C-Position sequence was not observed in D. aff. auriculatus. A larger proportion of couples of D. aff. auriculatus exhibited the Waltz behavior (Table 3). When the Waltz occurred, male D. aff. auriculatus led females for slightly longer periods of time (mean = 2.5 min, range = 1–7 min) and initiated the waltz more frequently per encounter (mean = 12.0 times, range = 4–29 times) than did D. conanti. Only a single male D. conanti in a single encounter exhibited the Waltz (mean duration = 1.5 min, range = 1–2 min; frequency = 2 times).

Table 3. 

Proportion of couples exhibiting each behavior. A total of 12 homospecific encounters were photographed (seven between D. aff. auriculatus and five between D. conanti [SL]). Fourteen heterospecific encounters were also photographed (eight between a male D. conanti [SL] and female D. aff. auriculatus, and six between a male D. aff. auriculatus and female D. conanti [SL]).

i0045-8511-105-2-261-t03.tif

In stark contrast, no heterospecific encounters reached the sperm transfer stage of courtship. Joint Isolation (JI), or the proportional difference between the number of successful homospecific versus heterospecific encounters, is a mating index commonly used to describe the overall breakdown in courtship between populations or species (Arnold et al., 1996). Success can be judged using the proportion of inseminations as well as that for spermatophore deposition. In this study, JIINSEMINATION = 1.21, whereas JISPERMATOPHORE = 1.46. Males of both species did exhibit pursuit (Stage 1) and at least initial persuasive behaviors (Stage 2; i.e., Head Rubbing; Table 3) in heterospecific crosses. Male D. aff. auriculatus initiated Head Rubbing slightly more frequently (mean = 1.5 times, range = 1–5) than did male D. conanti (mean = 0.876, range = 1–3) across photographed heterospecific encounters. Interestingly, I observed what appeared to be Head Rubbing behaviors initiated by two different female D. aff. auriculatus in two heterospecific encounters. In one of these encounters, the female D. aff. auriculatus approached the male D. conanti from behind and slid her body beneath his until their heads were aligned, at which point she pressed the top of her head upwards into his chin and throat. The male then pressed his rostrum downwards and briefly rubbed his head against the top and sides of the female's head. I did not observe female D. aff. auriculatus in homospecific trials, or any female D. conanti (SL), initiate Head Rubbing.

I observed aggressive behaviors in one homospecific encounter between D. aff. auriculatus, as well as in two heterospecific encounters between male D. conanti and a female D. aff. auriculatus. The same female D. aff. auriculatus was involved in each of these three unique pairings and was the aggressor in the heterospecific encounters, but not in the homospecific encounter. In the homospecific encounter, the male D. aff. auriculatus Gaped (Table 1) in front of the female's head. In each of the heterospecific encounters the female D. aff. auriculatus Gaped and then subsequently Bit the male D. conanti on the rostrum after he oriented towards and approached her head. I did not observe aggression between D. conanti (SL) in homospecific encounters, nor aggression initiated by male or female D. conanti (SL) in heterospecific encounters. However, if aggressive behaviors lasted for less than 30 seconds, or if they occurred during the interval not photographed by the GoPro cameras, they may have been missed.

Oviposition and insemination.—Females of each species oviposited a single clutch in both 2014 and 2015 (n = 4 clutches total). The same female D. aff. auriculatus (JYL #160) produced one clutch in each year. The first clutch contained 31 eggs and was oviposited on 7 October, while the second contained ≥27 eggs and was oviposited on 15 September. All 31 individuals from the first clutch hatched within 56–59 days, but tissue samples were only collected from 29 individuals. A total of nine hatchlings were produced from the second clutch after 46 days. I collected female D. aff. auriculatus #160 from the field on 2 June 2014. She was inseminated by a single male D. aff. auriculatus (JYL #170) during courtship trials in 2014, and by two male D. aff. auriculatus in 2015 (JYL #169 and #170).

A single female D. conanti (SL) oviposited a clutch of at least 14 eggs on 28 October, but by approximately two weeks later had consumed her entire clutch. At least ten of her eggs had been fertilized (i.e., nerulation visible in photos, pre-Stage 16 [Marks and Collazo, 1998]). In 2015 a different female D. conanti (SL) (JYL #328) produced a clutch of at least 16 eggs on 22 July. By 7 August a few eggs had turned brown and shrunk in size, but many others were clearly fertilized and developing (i.e., fore and hind limb buds visible, Stage 18 or 19 [Marks and Collazo, 1998]). Only six eggs remained by 15 August, the others presumably consumed by the female. These remaining eggs included two in which no development was visible and four containing embryos that were at approximately Stage 22–24 (i.e., at least some pigmentation visible in the eye [Marks and Collazo, 1998]). To prevent any further loss, I preserved all six eggs in ethanol on 15 August. I used the entirety of each egg to extract genomic DNA for microsatellite genotyping. I collected female D. conanti (SL) #328 from the field on 14 February 2015. She was successfully inseminated by a single male D. conanti (SL) (JYL# 329) on 19 April 2015.

Excluding potential fathers.—All loci successfully amplified for known mothers, as well as across all but one potential father (i.e., Dcon34 for D. aff. auriculatus #169; Table 4). None of the offspring mismatched the genotype of their mothers. I observed between one and four alleles in each clutch produced by the female D. aff. auriculatus; however, some offspring did not amplify at locus Dcon26 in the first clutch and at loci Dcon05, Dcon26, and Dcon34 in the second clutch (Table 4). Mean observed heterozygosity (± standard error) across loci for offspring in the first clutch was 0.580±0.160, and that for the second was 0.625±0.171. Despite having successfully inseminated female D. aff. auriculatus #160 in both 2014 and 2015, male D. aff. auriculatus #170 mismatched all 29 offspring in each clutch at loci Dcon16, as well as all genotyped offspring across multiple other loci (Table 4). The alleles of male D. aff. auriculatus #169 are represented in the genotypes of each of the nine offspring in the 2015 clutch with no mismatches (Table 4).

Table 4. 

Genotypes of known mothers, potential fathers, and offspring from each clutch. The number of offspring successfully genotyped for each locus is included in parentheses. A = locus failed to amplify consistently for this individual across two or more independent amplification attempts. Female D. aff. auriculatus #160 mated with male #170 prior to ovipositing Clutch #1, and both male #169 and #170 prior to Clutch #2. Female D. conanti (SL) #328 only mated with male #329 in trials.

i0045-8511-105-2-261-t04.tif

Of the six eggs collected from the single clutch from female D. conanti (SL) #328, two failed to amplify across four or more loci, and therefore I excluded them from this analysis. The remaining four offspring were genotyped for at least five of the six microsatellite loci, and I observed between two and four alleles across loci in this clutch (Table 4). Mean observed heterozygosity was 0.944±0.056. The genotypes for all four offspring were concordant with alleles found in male #329 with no unaccounted for alleles.

DISCUSSION

Comparing JI values for this study (JIINSEMINATION = 1.21; JISPERMATOPHORE = 1.46) with those of other, similar studies highlights the extreme degree of sexual isolation that I observed between D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) (Fig. 2). It is feasible that these JI values will differ if a larger number of individuals from each population and species are crossed in future studies. However, given that none of the heterospecific couples reached advanced stages of courtship (i.e., Stage 3, sperm transfer; Verrell and Arnold, 1989), it is not likely that isolation estimates will be dramatically lower.

Fig. 2. 

Joint isolation coefficients from published studies of crosses between species of plethodontids. Joint isolation (JI) coefficients for insemination from crosses between allopatric (black) and sympatric (gray) taxa are represented as a histogram. Results from this study (JI = 1.21) between sympatric D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) are indicated with a patterned fill. Previously published studies represented in this figure include Verrell (1989b, 1990a, 1990b), Verrell and Tilley (1992), Uzendoski and Verrell (1993), Arnold et al. (1996), Herring and Verrell (1996), Kozak (2003), and Mabry and Verrell (2004).

i0045-8511-105-2-261-f02.tif

The method of time-lapse photography implemented in this study was able to document multiple interesting behaviors, including both female-initiated courtship and aggression. Female-initiated behaviors have been observed in other studies with Desmognathus (e.g., nudging [Mabry and Verrell, 2004]), and female small-bodied Plethodon perform a number of behaviors which increase the likelihood of pheromone transfer and reception, including Head Rubbing (Dyal, 2006). I observed two different female D. aff. auriculatus initiating Head Rubbing in two heterospecific encounters, but larger sample sizes and continuous filming or observation are needed to verify the occurrence and frequency of these behaviors. Aggression has also been documented among species of plethodontids, but, with respect to courtship, any “overt fighting,” including threats, biting, and chasing, appears to occur more often between competing males (Verrell, 1989a). I noted Gaping and Biting (Table 1) between males and females in three different encounters (i.e., one homospecific and two heterospecific), and in each case the aggressor was either a male or a female D. aff. auriculatus. In the homospecific encounter, the male D. aff. auriculatus performed Gape in front of the female after having initiated multiple instances of Body Contact. This encounter did not progress beyond initial pheromone transfer behaviors. Territorial defense is an unlikely explanation for these behaviors because neither individual was a permanent resident of the enclosure. It would be useful to stage trials between D. aff. auriculatus and other species of Desmognathus to determine whether the former is generally more aggressive.

I suspect that the Waltz is an important precursor to the Tail-Straddle Walk (Table 1) in D. aff. auriculatus, and this may also be the case for other Desmognathus. I observed the Waltz in each photographed encounter for D. aff. auriculatus that resulted in insemination (n = 4). Couples often performed more than one complete rotation, and I observed them to perform the Waltz throughout the enclosure (i.e., this behavior was not necessarily triggered by proximity to a confining wall). Verrell (1997) first described the Waltz in a courtship study involving populations collected from Aiken and Barnwell counties in South Carolina, which he identified as D. auriculatus. Beamer and Lamb's (2008) survey of lineages of Desmognathus across the Coastal Plain suggests that Verrell's populations are not synonymous with D. auriculatus from the topotype locality and therefore should be referred to as D. cf. auriculatus. Sequence data (Lamb, 2016; Means et al., in press) demonstrate that populations of D. cf. auriculatus from South Carolina are also not synonymous with D. aff. auriculatus in Mississippi and Louisiana. It therefore appears that the Waltz occurs in at least two divergent lineages of Desmognathus in the Coastal Plain, and that this behavior should be given equal consideration as a discrete courtship behavior in comparative courtship studies. Interestingly, a very similar behavior, the Circular Tail-Straddling-Walk (c-TSW), has been described in Aneides (Sapp and Kiemnec-Tyburczy, 2011), a closely related genus of salamanders. Like the Waltz, c-TSW precedes the linear Tail-Straddle Walk that is typical of plethodontids, but, unlike the Waltz, in c-TSW the male's chin is in contact with the base of the female's tail (see fig. 1 in Sapp and Kiemnec-Tyburczy, 2011).

Microsatellites (Adams et al., 2005) and allozymes (Tilley and Hausman, 1976; Houck et al., 1985) have been used to determine paternity among species of Desmognathus within the ochrophaeus complex, but this study is the first to do so for Desmognathus in the Gulf Coastal Plain. The genotypes belonging to the clutch produced by female D. aff. auriculatus #160 in 2014 suggest that an unknown male must have fertilized this female in the field prior to her capture in June 2014, making these offspring the result of sperm which was stored for at least 127 days (4 mo 5 days). The 2015 clutch produced by the female D. aff. auriculatus appears to have been sired by the captive male #169 (Table 4), and therefore she must have stored sperm for at least 149 days (4 mo 27 days). Although it is possible that female #160 stored viable sperm (i.e., capable of fertilizing eggs) between seasons from an unknown male with whom she mated prior to capture (Houck and Schwenk, 1984; Eddy et al., 2015), this outcome seems improbable given the close match between paternal alleles and the ones found in male #169. Similarly, the four offspring in the clutch produced by female D. conanti (SL) #328 also appear to be sired by a single captive male, D. conanti (SL) #329 (Table 4), and this female must have stored sperm for at least 94 days (3 mo 3 days). There were four or fewer alleles per locus per clutch (Table 4) and the probability of genotyping error was low (i.e., allele sizes were confirmed across at least two amplification attempts). Therefore, assuming that there were no mutations or null alleles (Tennessen and Zamudio, 2003), it is unlikely that any of the three clutches were sired by more than one male.

The behaviors described herein, along with the results of the paternity exclusion analyses, further our understanding of the reproductive ecologies and behaviors of Desmognathus in the Gulf Coastal Plain. Desmognathus aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) from the Pearl and Pascagoula rivers exhibit a high degree of sexual isolation and only appear to exchange initial pheromone transfer behaviors in laboratory courtship trials. Offspring that were the result of homospecific matings indicate that female D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL) are capable of storing viable sperm for similar minimum durations, 4 mo and 5 days and 3 mo 3 days, respectively. These minimum estimates are similar to the three-month sperm storage duration suggested by Marynick (1971) for populations of Desmognathus from Louisiana. Although no hybridization occurred between D. aff. auriculatus and D. conanti (SL), the microsatellite loci used here could be applied in other studies to examine later stages of both pre-zygotic (i.e., sperm competition or exclusion) and post-zygotic isolation (i.e., reduced offspring fitness) among other species and populations of Desmognathus.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was funded by a National Science Foundation (NSF) Graduate Research Fellowship under Grant No. 0940712 and an NSF “Molecules to Muscles” GK-12 Fellowship Award No. 0947944 through the University of Southern Mississippi. I thank D. Beamer, G. Brown, C. Huntzinger, R. Jones, J. Lee, K. Narum, W. Selman, and A. Sinclair for their help in the field or in identifying potential collection localities. I am grateful to the J. Schaefer Lab for use of their GoPro™ HERO3 cameras. I also thank B. Morris, B. Kreiser, and C. Qualls for their editorial assistance, S. Arnold for our discussions pertaining to this manuscript, and T. Herman for his advice with respect to salamander husbandry. Scientific collection permits were provided by the Mississippi Department of Wildlife, Fisheries and Parks (Permits 0422131 and 0422141). All work was conducted in accordance with the appropriate institutional animal care guidelines (Permit 11061301).

LITERATURE CITED

1.

Adams, E. M., A. G. Jones, and S. J. Arnold. 2005. Multiple paternity in a natural population of a salamander with long-term sperm storage. Molecular Ecology 14:1803–1810. Google Scholar

2.

Arnold, S. J., N. L. Reagan, and P. A. Verrell. 1993. Reproductive isolation and speciation in plethodontid salamanders. Herpetologica 49:216–228. Google Scholar

3.

Arnold, S. J., P. A. Verrell, and S. G. Tilley. 1996. The evolution of asymmetry in sexual isolation: a model and a test case. Evolution 50:1024–1033. Google Scholar

4.

Beamer, D. A., and T. Lamb. 2008. Dusky salamanders (Desmognathus, Plethodontidae) from the Coastal Plain: multiple independent lineages and their bearing on the molecular phylogeny of the genus. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 47:143–153. Google Scholar

5.

Bonett, R. M. 2002. Analysis of the contact zone between the dusky salamanders Desmognathus fuscus fuscus and Desmognathus fuscus conanti (Caudata: Plethodontidae). Copeia 2002:344–355. Google Scholar

6.

Brown, H. H., H. K. Tyler, and T. A. Mousseau. 2004. Orajel as an amphibian anesthetic: refining the technique. Herpetological Review 35:252. Google Scholar

7.

Coyne, J. A., and H. A. Orr. 2004. Speciation. Sinaur Associates, Inc., Sunderland, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

8.

Dobzhansky, T. 1935. A critique of the species concept in biology. Philosophy of Science 2:344–355. Google Scholar

9.

Dyal, L. A. 2006. novel courtship behaviors in three small eastern Plethodon species. Journal of Herpetology 40:55–65. Google Scholar

10.

Eddy, S. L., E. A. Vaccaro, C. L. Baggett, K. M. Kiemnec-Tyburczy, and L. D. Houck. 2015. Sperm mass longevity and sperm storage in the female reproductive tract of Plethodon shermani (Amphibia: Plethodontidae). Herpetologica 71:177–183. Google Scholar

11.

Greene, K. M., S. E. Pittman, and M. E. Dorcas. 2016. The effects of conspecifics on burrow selection in juvenile spotted salamanders (Ambystoma maculatum). Journal of Ethology 34:309–314. Google Scholar

12.

Herring, K., and P. A. Verrell. 1996. Sexual incompatibility and geographical variation in mate recognition systems: a test in the salamander Desmognathus ochrophaeus. Animal Behaviour 52:279–287. Google Scholar

13.

Holbrook, J. E. 1838. North American Herpetology; Or, a Description of the Reptiles Inhabiting the United States. First edition. J. Dobson and Son, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania. Google Scholar

14.

Holding, M. L., E. H. Kern, R. D. Denton, and H. L. Gibbs. 2015. Fixed prey cue preferences among Dusky Pigmy Rattlesnakes (Sistrurus miliarius barbouri) raised on different long-term diets. Evolutionary Ecology 30:1–7. Google Scholar

15.

Houck, L. D., and K. Schwenk. 1984. The potential for long-term sperm competition in a plethodontid salamander. Herpetologica 40:410–415. Google Scholar

16.

Houck, L. D., S. G. Tilley, and S. J. Arnold. 1985. Sperm competition in a plethodontid salamander: preliminary results. Journal of Herpetology 19:420–423. Google Scholar

17.

Karlin, A. A., and S. I. Guttman. 1981. Hybridization between Desmognathus fuscus and Desmognathus ochrophaeus (Amphibia: Urodela: Plethodontidae) in northeastern Ohio and northwestern Pennsylvania. Copeia 1981:371–377. Google Scholar

18.

Karlin, A. A., and S. I. Guttman. 1986. Systematics and geographic isozyme variation in the plethodontid salamander Desmognathus fuscus (Rafinesque). Herpetologica 42:283–301. Google Scholar

19.

Kozak, K. H. 2003. Sexual isolation and courtship behavior in salamanders of the Eurycea bislineata species complex, with comments on the evolution of the mental gland and pheromone delivery behavior in the Plethodontidae. Southeastern Naturalist 2:281–292. Google Scholar

20.

Kozak, K. H., A. Larson, R. M. Bonett, and L. J. Harmon. 2005. Phylogenetic analysis of ecomorphological divergence, community structure, and diversification rates in dusky salamanders (Plethodontidae: Desmognathus). Evolution 59:2000–2016. Google Scholar

21.

Lamb, J. Y. 2016. Ecology and genetics of lungless salamanders (family Plethodontidae) in the Gulf Coastal Plain. Unpubl. Ph.D. diss., The University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, Mississippi. Google Scholar

22.

Lamb, J. Y., B. R. Kreiser, and C. P. Qualls. 2015. Characterization of microsatellite loci for the spotted dusky salamander (Desmognathus conanti) using paired-end Illumina shotgun sequencing and cross-amplification in another species of Desmognathus. Herpetological Review 46:343–347. Google Scholar

23.

Lanctôt, C., W. Bennett, S. Wilson, L. Fabbro, F. D. L. Leusch, and S. D. Melvin. 2016. Behaviour, development and metal accumulation in striped marsh frog tadpoles (Limnodynastes peronii) exposed to coal mine wastewater. Aquatic Toxicology 173:218–227. Google Scholar

24.

Lannoo, M. 2005. Amphibian Declines: The Conservation Status of United States Species. University of California Press, Berkeley, California. Google Scholar

25.

Mabry, M., and P. Verrell. 2004. Stifled sex in sympatry: patterns of sexual incompatibility among desmognathine salamanders. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 82:367–375. Google Scholar

26.

Marks, S. B., and A. Collazo. 1998. Direct development in Desmognathus aeneus (Caudata: Plethodontidae): a staging table. Copeia 1998:637–648. Google Scholar

27.

Marynick, S. P. 1971. Long term storage of sperm in Desmognathus fuscus from Louisiana. Copeia 1971:345–347. Google Scholar

28.

Mead, L. S., and P. A. Verrell. 2002. Evolution of courtship behaviour patterns and reproductive isolation in the Desmognathus ochrophaeus complex. Ethology 108:403–427. Google Scholar

29.

Means, D. B. 2005. Desmognathus auriculatus (Holbrook, 1938) Southern Dusky Salamander, p. 700–701. In:Amphibian Declines: The Conservation Status of United States Species. M. Lannoo (ed.). University of California Press, Berkeley, California. Google Scholar

30.

Means, D. B., and R. M. Bonett. 2005. Desmognathus conanti Rossman, 1958. Spotted dusky salamander, p. 705–706. In:Amphibian Declines: The Conservation Status of United States Species. M. Lannoo (ed.). University of California Press, Berkeley, California. Google Scholar

31.

Means, D. B., J. Y. Lamb, and J. Bernardo. In press. A new species of dusky salamander (Amphibia: Plethodontidae: Desmognathus) from the Eastern Gulf Coastal Plain of the United States and a redescription of D. auriculatus. Zootaxa. Google Scholar

32.

Pašukonis, A., M.-C. Loretto, L. Landler, M. Ringler, and W. Hödl. 2014. Homing trajectories and initial orientation in a Neotropical territorial frog, Allobates femoralis (Dendrobatidae). Frontiers in Zoology 11:29. Google Scholar

33.

Peakall, R., and P. E. Smouse. 2012. GenAlEx 6.5: genetic analysis in Excel. Population genetic software for teaching and research—an update. Bioinformatics 28:2537–2539. Google Scholar

34.

Plenderleith, T. L., and D. C. Forester. 2011. Lack of evidence for the prior residence effect in the Allegheny Mountain Dusky Salamander (Desmognathus ochrophaeus). Ethology 117:565–572. Google Scholar

35.

de Queiroz, K. 2005. A unified concept of species and its consequences for the future of taxonomy. Proceedings of the California Academy of Sciences 56:196–215. Google Scholar

36.

Rossman, D. A. 1958. A new race of Desmognathus fuscus from the south-central United States. Herpetologica 14:158–160. Google Scholar

37.

Rupp, A. 2016. Histology and ultrastructure of seasonal variation of mental glands and caudal courtship glands in three genera of plethodontid salamanders. Unpubl. Master's thesis, Southeastern Louisiana University, Hammond, Louisiana. Google Scholar

38.

Sapp, J. R., and K. M. Kiemnec-Tyburczy. 2011. The circular tail-straddling walk of the clouded salamander, Aneides ferreus: a deviation from the highly conserved linear tail-straddling walk of the Plethodontidae. Amphibia-Reptilia 32:235–243. Google Scholar

39.

Sever, D. M. 2003. Courtship and mating glands, p. 323–381. In:Reproductive Biology and Phylogeny of Urodela. D. M. Sever (ed.). Science Publishers, Inc., Enfield, New Hampshire. Google Scholar

40.

Sharbel, T. F., J. Bonin, L. A. Lowcock, and D. M. Green. 1995. Partial genetic compatibility and unidirectional hybridization in syntopic populations of the salamanders Desmognathus fuscus and D. ochrophaeus. Copeia 1995:466–469. Google Scholar

41.

Tennessen, J. A., and K. R. Zamudio. 2003. Early male reproductive advantage, multiple paternity and sperm storage in an amphibian aggregate breeder. Molecular Ecology 12:1567–1576. Google Scholar

42.

Tilley, S. G. 2016. Patterns of genetic differentiation in woodland and dusky salamanders. Copeia 104:8–20. Google Scholar

43.

Tilley, S. G., and J. S. Hausman. 1976. Allozymic variation and occurrence of multiple inseminations in populations of the salamander Desmognathus ochrophaeus. Copeia 1976:734–741. Google Scholar

44.

Uzendoski, K., and P. A. Verrell. 1993. Sexual incompatibility and mate-recognition systems: a study of two species of sympatric salamanders (Plethodontidae). Animal Behaviour 46:267–278. Google Scholar

45.

Valentine, B. D. 1963. The salamander genus Desmognathus in Mississippi. Copeia 1963:130–139. Google Scholar

46.

Verrell, P. 1999. Bracketing the extremes: courtship behaviour of the smallest- and largest-bodied species in the salamander genus Desmognathus (Plethodontidae: Desmognathinae). Journal of Zoology 247:105–111. Google Scholar

47.

Verrell, P. A. 1988a. Intrinsic male mating capacity is limited in the plethodontid salamander, Desmognathus ochrophaeus. Journal of Herpetology 22:394–400. Google Scholar

48.

Verrell, P. A. 1988b. Mating and female sexual responsiveness in the salamander Desmognathus ochrophaeus. Herpetologica 44:334–337. Google Scholar

49.

Verrell, P. A. 1989a. The sexual strategies of natural populations of newts and salamanders. Herpetologica 45:265–282. Google Scholar

50.

Verrell, P. A. 1989b. An experimental study of the behavioral basis of sexual isolation between two sympatric plethodontid salamanders, Desmognathus imitator and D. ochrophaeus. Ethology 80:274–282. Google Scholar

51.

Verrell, P. A. 1990a. Sexual compatibility among plethodontid salamanders: tests between Desmognathus apalachicolae, and D. ochrophaeus and D. fuscus. Herpetologica 46:415–422. Google Scholar

52.

Verrell, P. A. 1990b. Tests for sexual isolation among sympatric salamanders of the genus Desmognathus. Amphibia–Reptilia 11:147–153. Google Scholar

53.

Verrell, P. A. 1991. Insemination temporarily inhibits sexual responsiveness in female salamanders (Desmognathus ochrophaeus). Behaviour 119:51–64. Google Scholar

54.

Verrell, P. A. 1995. The courtship behaviour of the spotted dusky salamander, Desmognathus fuscus conanti (Amphibia: Caudata: Plethodontidae). Journal of Zoology 235:515–523. Google Scholar

55.

Verrell, P. A. 1997. Courtship in desmognathine salamanders: the southern dusky salamander, Desmognathus auriculatus. Journal of Herpetology 31:273–277. Google Scholar

56.

Verrell, P. A., and S. J. Arnold. 1989. Behavioral observations of sexual isolation among allopatric populations of the mountain dusky salamander, Desmognathus ochrophaeus. Evolution 43:745–755. Google Scholar

57.

Verrell, P. A., and S. G. Tilley. 1992. Population differentiation in plethodontid salamanders: divergence of allozymes and sexual compatibility among populations of Desmognathus imitator and D. ochrophaeus (Caudata: Plethodontidae). Zoological Journal of the Linnean Society 104:67–80. Google Scholar

58.

Voss, S. R., and H. B. Shaffer. 1996. What insights into the developmental traits of urodeles does the study of interspecific hybrids provide? The International Journal of Developmental Biology 40:885–893. Google Scholar
© 2017 by the American Society of Ichthyologists and Herpetologists
Jennifer Y. Lamb "Sexual Isolation between Two Sympatric Desmognathus in the Gulf Coastal Plain," Copeia 105(2), 261-268, (22 June 2017). https://doi.org/10.1643/CE-16-565
Received: 15 December 2016; Accepted: 1 February 2017; Published: 22 June 2017
Back to Top