Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
11 April 2019 Bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila) Functional Traits in Relation to Sampling Methodology in a Restored Tallgrass Prairie
Kenneth W. McCravy, Ray K. Geroff, Jason Gibbs
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Analysis of functional trait composition can provide insights into effects of habitat alteration on ecological functioning of particular taxa. However, assessments of functional trait composition may be affected by the sampling methodology used. We assessed functional trait composition of bees (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila) collected using 4 sampling methods (elevated bowl traps, ground-level bowl traps, Malaise traps, and vane traps). Functional diversity, as measured by functional dispersion (mean distance between individual species and the community centroid in multidimensional trait space), did not vary among sampling methods. Fourth corner analysis revealed statistically greater representation of solitary species and above-ground nesting species than expected in ground-level bowl traps. Polylecty was strongly associated with Malaise traps. Body length was negatively associated with Malaise traps and positively associated with vane traps. Our results suggest that sampling of bee functional traits can be method-dependent, and this should be considered in assessments of functional trait composition.

Functional traits are characteristics of individuals that affect fitness, and can determine species persistence and contribution to ecosystem functioning, the processes that sustain ecosystems (Violle et al. 2007). Species diversity does not directly determine ecosystem functioning, but rather indirectly determines functioning through the cumulative contributions of traits present in these species (Cadotte et al. 2011). Loss of biodiversity may result in decline of ecological functioning (Hooper et al. 2012), particularly if these losses are concentrated in particular functional groups with similar ecological niches (Flynn et al. 2009). Whereas identification of important functional traits has progressed most rapidly for plants (Díaz et al. 2007), trait analyses also have played important roles in studies of certain animal taxa, including hover flies (Schweiger et al. 2007), ground beetles (Barber et al. 2017), birds (Newbold et al. 2013), fish (Pont et al. 2006), and bees (Moretti et al. 2009; Williams et al. 2010; Forrest et al. 2015; Blitzer et al. 2016; Bartomeus et al. 2018).

Bees play an ecologically essential role as pollinators, and functional diversity may increase the proportion of flowers pollinated, seed set, and other measures of pollination success. For instance, Blitzer et al. (2016) showed that bee functional group diversity (based on nesting habits, sociality, and body size) was associated with increased seed set, and decreased pollen limitation in commercial apple farms. Several studies also have suggested that bee species composition and associated functional traits respond to habitat alteration. Forrest et al. (2015) showed that farmland supports lower bee functional diversity than do natural habitats, and Moretti et al. (2009) found that bee functional composition shifted in response to fire in temperate forest. Functional traits have been shown to be useful as predictors of bee species responses to a variety of habitat alterations (Williams et al. 2010), although bee traits were weak and inconsistent predictors of response to variation in land use practices in 49 sites across 3 crop systems (watermelon, cranberry, and blueberry) in New Jersey and eastern Pennsylvania, USA (Bartomeus et al. 2018).

A variety of sampling methods are available for assessing bee assemblages, including bowl traps (sometimes called “pan traps”), Malaise traps, and vane traps. Bowl traps used in bee sampling are usually plastic bowls, with blue, white, and yellow colors typically used. Bowl traps are generally placed on the ground in insect surveys, but are sometimes elevated to match surrounding vegetation (Leong & Thorp 1999; Hopwood 2008; Tuell & Isaacs 2009; Droege et al. 2010; Geroff et al. 2014; Shapiro et al. 2014). Both Malaise traps (Townes 1972; Bartholomew & Prowell 2005; Ngo et al. 2013; Geroff et al. 2014; McCravy et al. 2016; McCravy & Ruholl 2017) and vane traps (Stephen & Rao 2005; Kimoto et al. 2012; Geroff et al. 2014; Buchanan et al. 2017; McCravy & Ruholl 2017) are flight interception traps that also have been used effectively in collecting bees. There is evidence that elevated and ground-level bowl traps, Malaise traps, and vane traps vary in the abundance and species composition of bees collected (Geroff et al. 2014; Joshi et al. 2015). Because species richness and diversity are major foci in assessments of bee conservation status, it is important that such methodological variation be recognized. However, as with more traditional measures such as species richness and diversity, particular sampling methodologies may produce biased estimates of functional trait composition of bee communities as well. For instance, Gonzalez et al. (2016) found that bowl trap height can produce biased estimates of bee body size in unmanaged grass and wild carrot-dominated areas in northwestern Turkey, and bee body size and proportions of oligolectic and parasitic species varied with the sampling method in northwestern USA bunchgrass and forb-dominated prairie (Rhoades et al. 2017). There is evidence that eusocial bees are more attracted to blue and white bowl traps, whereas non-eusocial bees are more attracted to yellow (Sircom et al. 2018). In the desert southwestern USA, native bee species associated with creosote bush, Larrea tridentata (DC) Coville (Zygophyllaceae), were poorly represented in bowl traps compared to aerial netting (Cane et al. 2000). In Michigan and Pennsylvania, USA, apple and sour cherry orchards, blue vane traps collected an assemblage of largely non-crop visiting bees that was different from that collected via aerial netting on crop flowers (Gibbs et al. 2017b).

In this study, we compare the performance of 4 bee-sampling methods (elevated bowl traps, ground-level bowl traps, Malaise traps, and vane traps) with respect to 4 functional traits: body length (as a measure of body size), lecty, sociality, and nest location. Body size and lecty are associated with dispersal capabilities and foraging behavior in bees (Greenleaf et al. 2007), and these traits have been used to assess responses of bees to habitat changes. Nest location is also habitatdependent and thus can be affected by habitat disturbance. Information on potential sampling bias associated with these traits could be important when considering the experimental design of studies aiming to assess bee communities using these functional traits.

Materials and Methods

This study was done in an approximately 12 ha restored tallgrass prairie at the Alice L. Kibbe Field Station (40.3660°N, 91.4070°W), Western Illinois University in Hancock County, Illinois, USA. As part of an assessment of bee sampling methods by Geroff et al. (2014), bees were collected from 9:00 AM to 6:00 PM on 6 d (based on weather conditions) during each of the 4 sampling periods in 2010: early Jun, mid-Jul, late Aug, and late Sep-early Oct. Four sampling methods were employed: bowl traps (elevated 1 m using wooden posts), bowl traps (ground level), Malaise traps, and vane traps. Bowl traps were 354 mL colored plastic bowls (Hallmark, Kansas City, Missouri, USA). Malaise traps were standard Townes-style (Sante Traps, Lexington, Kentucky, USA). Vane traps (SpringStar, Inc., Woodinville, Washington, USA) were suspended from shepherd hooks with the top of the collection jar 1 m above the ground. Traps were deployed in a transect of 20 plots, with 5 plots per trap type. Each plot consisted of 3 elevated bowl traps (1 each of blue, white, and yellow), or 3 ground-level bowl traps (1 each of blue, white, and yellow), or 1 Malaise trap, or 4 vane traps (2 each of blue and yellow). Within plots with multiple traps, traps were located 5 m apart, and plots were separated by 25 m. Trap types were randomly assigned to plots, and trap color positions were randomly assigned within plots. Geroff et al. (2014) provided further details on the study site and design.

Before analysis, males (10.1% of total captures) were excluded from the data set because of sex-related differences in body lengths, and because females are the primary foraging and nest-building sex. Three data matrices were constructed: (1) a main matrix, with bee species and their counts as columns, and plots as rows; (2) a second matrix, specifying a sampling method for each plot; and (3) a trait matrix, with traits as columns and bee species as rows. Four traits were used that reflect important aspects of bee ecology, behavior, and natural history, and for which information is widely available for most bee species in the literature: body length (a quantitative variable, expressed in mm), sociality (categorical: social or solitary), nest location (categorical: aboveground or belowground), and lecty (categorical: oligolectic or polylectic). Communal bees were classified as ‘solitary,’ so sociality is actually a measure of division of labor. Information on these traits was obtained from Mitchell (1960, 1962), LaBerge (1967, 1971, 1973, 1977, 1989), Bouseman and LaBerge (1979), Michener (2007), Gibbs (2010), and Gibbs et al. (2017a), or predicted based on phylogeny (Gibbs et al. 2012; Danforth et al. 2013). The mean value was used where a range was given for body length.

For each of the 4 sampling methods, functional dispersion, a measure of functional diversity, was calculated for each plot. Functional dispersion is the mean distance between individual species (weighted by relative abundances) and the community centroid in multidimensional trait space (Laliberté & Legendre 2010). Because the traits include categorical variables, the species X trait matrix was represented as a series of principal coordinate axes based on a Gower distance matrix, with correction for negative eigenvalues (Legendre & Anderson 1999). The Kruskal-Wallis test was performed to compare functional dispersion among sampling methods.

Fourth corner analysis was used to examine relationships between sampling methods and traits of the bee species collected. This analysis allows analysis of bivariate linear associations between quantitative and categorical species traits and explanatory variables (sampling method), based on the relative abundances of the species that link these traits and variables. For pairs of categorical variables, fourth corner analysis calculates a log-likelihood ratio G-test. For mixed pairs of categorical and quantitative variables, fourth corner analysis produces an R-value (equivalent to a correlation value) and D-value. The D-value is a measure of the degree of clustering of the quantitative variable within levels of the categorical variable (Peck 2016).

For fourth corner analysis, species with fewer than 3 non-zero cells were excluded to reduce sparsity, and values for each species were then relativized by the maximum value to reduce heterogeneity and the effect of dominant species. To eliminate decimal values for analysis of categorical variables, these values were then transformed via multiplication by 100 and rounded to the nearest whole number (Peck 2016). To test the null hypothesis that species were distributed according to preference for sampling method but independently of their traits, randomization by entire columns (species) was done (Dray & Legendre 2008), with the Benjamini and Hochberg (1995) P-adjustment for false discovery rate.

All analyses were done using PC-Ord, version 7 (MjM Software Design, Gleneden Beach, Oregon, USA; McCune & Mefford 2016), except for the Kruskal-Wallis test, which was done using Sigma Plot, version 13 (Systat Software, Inc., San Jose, California, USA).

Results

One hundred eleven bee species were collected during the study. See Geroff et al. (2014) for a complete list of species and abundances per sampling method. After exclusion of males, 99 species remained. Median functional dispersion was 0.906 for elevated bowl traps, 0.890 for ground-level bowl traps, 0.882 for Malaise traps, and 0.951 for vane traps. There was no significant difference in functional dispersion among the 4 methods (H = 4.451; df = 3; P = 0.217).

After exclusion of species with fewer than 3 non-zero cells, 58 species remained for fourth corner analysis. See Table 1 for a list of these species and their functional traits. Globally, there was a significant relationship between the sampling method and sociality (G = 910.02; P = 0.038). Among individual methods, there was a significant association between ground-level bowl traps and sociality (solitary: P = 0.040; social: P = 0.043), with greater representation of solitary bees (and lower representation of social bees) than expected (Table 2). This result was driven to a great extent by the relatively low incidence of social Lasioglossum (Hymenoptera: Halictidae) in ground-level bowl traps. Of 15 species of social Lasioglossum, only 6 (40%) were represented in ground-level bowl traps, vs. 15 in elevated bowl traps, 15 in Malaise traps, and 10 (66.7%) in vane traps. Bombus (Hymenoptera: Apidae) also were poorly represented in ground-level bowl traps. Of the 4 species of Bombus combined, ground-level bowl traps accounted for only 3 of 35 trap plot occurrences (Bombus griseocollis [DeGeer] in 2 ground-level bowl trap plots and Bombus impatiens Cresson in 1 ground-level bowl trap plot). Overall, 20 of 32 species (62.5%) collected in ground-level bowl traps were solitary, vs. 29 of 51 species (56.9%) in elevated bowl traps, 24 of 45 species (53.3%) in Malaise traps, and 23 of 41 species (56.1%) in vane traps.

There was a significant global relationship between sampling method and nest location (G = 1144.21; P = 0.039). A significant association between ground-level bowl traps and nest location was found (belowground: P = 0.024; aboveground: P = 0.024), with greater representation of above-ground nesting species (and lower representation of below-ground species) than expected (Table 3). Again, Lasioglossum and Bombus were important drivers of this pattern. Only 6 of 15 belowground nesting Lasioglossum were represented in ground-level bowl traps. Only 1 of these 6 species was solitary, indicating substantial overlap of results for nest location and sociality. Likewise, the below-ground nesting Bombus were poorly represented in ground-level bowl traps as described above. Melissodes (Hymenoptera: Apidae) also contributed to the significant result for ground-level bowl traps, as well as the relatively low P-values for Malaise traps. All 5 Melissodes (all solitary below-ground nesters) were absent from ground-level bowl traps and Malaise traps, but all were present in elevated bowl traps, as well as vane traps. Overall, 13 of 32 species (40.6%) collected in ground-level bowl traps were above-ground nesters, vs. 15 of 51 species (29.4%) in elevated bowl traps, 17 of 45 species (37.8%) in Malaise traps, and 13 of 41 species (31.7%) in vane traps.

There was a significant global relationship between sampling method and lecty (G = 1076.62; P = 0.029). There was a significant association between Malaise traps and lecty (polylectic: P = 0.024; oligolectic: P = 0.024), with greater representation of polylectic species (and lower representation of oligolectic species) than expected (Table 4). All 3 oligolectic Melissodes, as well as Peponapis pruinosa (Say) and Svastra obliqua (Say) (both Hymenoptera: Apidae), were absent from Malaise traps, but present in 2 of the 3 other trap types. Overall, 1 of 45 species (2.2%) collected in Malaise traps were oligolectic, vs. 2 of 32 species (6.3%) in ground-level bowl traps, 6 of 51 species (11.8%) in elevated bowl traps, and 5 of 41 species (12.2%) in vane traps.

Globally, there was a significant association between sampling method and body length (F = 986.85; P = 0.001), with significant clustering within Malaise trap (D = 0.206; P < 0.001), i.e., low variation of data points around the mean. Malaise traps showed a significant negative association (R = −0.290; P < 0.001), and vane traps a significant positive association (R = 0.306, P < 0.001) with body length, i.e., relative to the other sampling methods, body length of collected bees is less for Malaise traps and greater for vane traps. Bombus were relatively poorly represented in Malaise traps. Bombus auricomus (Robertson) was absent from Malaise traps but appeared in 6 other trap plots, whereas Bombus bimaculatus Cresson appeared in 1 Malaise trap of 5 total plots, B. griseocollis in 3 Malaise traps of 14 total plots, and B. impatiens in 2 Malaise traps of 10 total plots. Eucerine bees (Eucera hamata (Bradley), Melissodes spp., P. pruinosa, and S. obliqua) also were absent from Malaise trap collections. Eucera hamata (Bradley) (Hymenoptera: Apidae) was collected in all 3 of the other trap types. All 5 species of Melissodes were collected exclusively in elevated bowl traps and vane traps, as was P. pruinosa, whereas S. obliqua were collected only in elevated and ground-level bowl traps. On the other hand, Bombus were well represented in vane traps, as were the eucerines with the exception of S. obliqua. Among the 4 species of Bombus combined, vane traps accounted for 16 of 35 trap plot occurrences, and among the 5 species of Melissodes, 15 of 26 trap plot occurrences. Among the very short-bodied (< 5 mm) species Hylaeus mesillae (Cockerell), Lasioglossum illinoense (Robertson), Lasioglossum imitatum (Smith), and Lasioglossum weemsi (Mitchell) (all Hymenoptera: Apidae), vane traps accounted for only 3 of 24 trap plot occurrences.

Discussion

Results of this study suggest that overall trait diversity of bees (as measured by functional dispersion) in this restored tallgrass prairie habitat does not vary substantially with the sampling method. However, fourth corner analysis indicates that particular traits are strongly associated with individual sampling methods. In particular, ground-level bowl traps, probably the most commonly used bee trapping method, were significantly associated with relatively high representation of solitary bees and above-ground nesting bees. Somewhat surprisingly, these patterns were driven largely by the absence of over half of Lasioglossum species from this trap type. Lasioglossum have been collected in great abundance in ground-level bowl traps in several studies (Toler et al. 2005; Wilson et al. 2008; Droege et al. 2010). Geroff et al. (2014) collected 189 Lasioglossum in ground-level bowl traps, but 167 (88.4%) of these were the very abundant Lasioglossum versatum (Robertson) that also was collected in large numbers (> 2,000) in Malaise traps and vane traps. The low incidence of Bombus in ground-level bowl traps is not surprising and has been noted by Roulston et al. (2007).

Gonzalez et al. (2016) found that bowl traps that were elevated 70 cm collected larger bees (based on intertegular distance) than did ground-level bowl traps, and Rhoades et al. (2017) found that groundlevel bowl traps collected smaller bees (again based on intertegular distance) than did blue vane traps and net collecting (elevated bowl traps were not used in the study). We found no evidence of an effect of bowl trap height on collected bee body size (based on body length).

Malaise traps were significantly associated with polylecty and were negatively associated with body length, whereas vane traps were positively associated with body length. Of the 4 sampling methods in this study, Malaise traps appear to be the least dependent on active colorbased attraction of bees and may collect relatively fewer specialist bees that are attracted to particular flower colors. All 14 indicator species for Malaise traps in the study of Geroff et al. (2014) were polylectic. It is possible that larger bees, that are presumably stronger, more active fliers, are more adept at avoiding the Malaise traps altogether, or escaping from them before entering the collection container. The positive association of vane traps with body length was not surprising. Vane traps, particularly blue vane traps, are known to be especially effective in collecting larger bees, such as bumble bees and eucerine bees (Stephen & Rao 2005; Kimoto et al. 2012; Geroff et al. 2014; Buchanan et al. 2017; Gibbs et al. 2017b; McCravy & Ruholl 2017), particularly long-tongued bumble bees such as B. auricomus (Gibbs et al. 2017b). The light reflectance of blue vane traps may attract these bees by mimicking that of their preferred host plants (Joshi et al. 2015; Gibbs et al. 2017b).

Table 1.

Traits of bee species collected using 4 sampling methods (elevated bowl traps, ground-level bowl traps, Malaise traps, and vane traps) in a west-central Illinois restored tallgrass prairie. See Gibbs et al. (2017a) for citations for species traits.

t01a_134.gif

Continued

t01b_134.gif

Table 2.

Numbers of social and solitary bees (unrelativized and relativized by the maximum) collected using 4 sampling methods in a west-central Illinois restored tallgrass prairie, with P-values. EBT: elevated bowl trap; GBT: ground-level bowl trap; MT: Malaise trap; VT: vane trap. Global G-value = 910.02; P = 0.038.

t02_134.gif

Table 3.

Numbers of aboveground and belowground-nesting bees (unrelativized and relativized by the maximum) collected using 4 sampling methods in a westcentral Illinois restored tallgrass prairie, with P-values. EBT: elevated bowl trap; GBT: ground-level bowl trap; MT: Malaise trap; VT: vane trap. Global G-value = 1,144.21; P = 0.039.

t03_134.gif

Table 4.

Numbers of oligolectic and polylectic bees (unrelativized and relativized by the maximum) collected using 4 sampling methods in a west-central Illinois restored tallgrass prairie, with P-values. EBT: elevated bowl trap; GBT: groundlevel bowl trap; MT: Malaise trap; VT: vane trap. Global G-value = 1,076.62; P = 0.029.

t04_134.gif

Analysis of functional traits is being increasingly used to evaluate effects of land-use changes on bee diversity and conservation. Our results provide additional evidence that functional traits represented in bee samples can vary depending on the sampling method used, and this should be considered in such studies. For instance, studies examining bee foraging ranges and effects of habitat fragmentation on bees should consider the potential body size biases associated with sampling methodology, because bee body size and foraging range are known to be associated (Greenleaf et al. 2007). The quality of these data is dependent on reliable sampling.

Because we have no independent estimate of the true number and species composition of bees in a community, we have no reference with which to compare sampling results (Colwell & Coddington 1994; Droege et al. 2010). Such independent data are needed badly, and can possibly be obtained through habitat manipulation studies and studies incorporating marking of individual bees (Droege et al. 2010). However, the variation in results of different sampling methods documented here and elsewhere suggests that a combination of sampling methods may provide a more complete picture of bee species and trait diversity. Incorporation of additional methods, particularly active netting (search-and-net, and sweep netting) would provide an additional dimension to the data (Cane et al. 2000; Grundel et al. 2011).

It also should be noted that functional traits can be difficult to measure and classify. Intraspecific body size measurements of worker bees can vary geographically (Skandalis et al. 2009), seasonally (Quezada-Euán et al. 2011), and individually over the course of their adult life (León-Contrera et al. 2006). Degrees of sociality range from that found in Apis, with thousands of workers per colony, to Augochloropsis metallica (F.) fulgida (Smith) (Hymenoptera: Halictidae), in which an excavated nest had 1 worker (Gibbs 2017). By manipulating food resources, Ceratina calcarata Robertson (Hymenoptera: Apidae) can produce a dwarf worker, and so is potentially social (Lawson et al. 2017). The concept of lecty also is fraught with complexity, and characterizations of lecty can involve finegrained levels of classification (Cane & Sipes 2006). Further studies are needed to increase our knowledge of bee natural history, clarify the role of trait analysis in studies of bee/habitat relationships, and make informed decisions regarding the most effective methodological approaches to employ.

Acknowledgments

We thank Sam Droege (US Geological Survey, Patuxent Wildlife Research Center), Rob Jean (Environmental Solutions and Innovations, Inc.), Mike Arduser (Missouri Department of Conservation), and Jared Ruholl (USDA, Farm Service Agency) for their contributions to bee identifications; Jennifer Hopwood (The Xerces Society) for advice on bee sampling; Mario Biondini (North Dakota State University) for assistance with interpreting results of fourth corner analysis; and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. We also thank Prairie Biotic Research, Inc., and the Department of Biological Sciences, College of Arts and Sciences, Graduate Student Research and Professional Development Fund, University Research Council, and the Foundation Office of Western Illinois University for providing funds in support of this study.

References Cited

1.

Barber NA, Lamagdeleine-Dent KA, Willand JE, Jones HP, McCravy KW. 2017. Species and functional trait re-assembly of ground beetle communities in restored grasslands. Biodiversity and Conservation 26: 3481-3498. Google Scholar

2.

Bartholomew CS, Prowell D. 2005. Pan compared to malaise trapping for bees (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) in a longleaf pine savanna. Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 78: 390-392. Google Scholar

3.

Bartomeus I, Cariveau DP, Harrison T, Winfree R. 2018. On the inconsistency of pollinator species traits for predicting either response to land-use change or functional contribution. Oikos 127: 306-315. Google Scholar

4.

Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. 1995. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B 57: 289-300. Google Scholar

5.

Blitzer EJ, Gibbs J, Park MG, Danforth BN. 2016. Pollination services for apple are dependent on diverse wild bee communities. Agriculture, Ecosystems and Environment 221: 1-7. Google Scholar

6.

Bouseman JK, LaBerge WE. 1979. A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part IX. Subgenus Melandrena. Transactions of the American Entomological Society 104: 275-389. Google Scholar

7.

Buchanan AL, Gibbs J, Komondy L, Szendrei Z. 2017. Bee community of commercial potato fields in Michigan and Bombus impatiens visitation to neonicotinoid-treated potato plants. Insects 8: 30. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects8010030 Google Scholar

8.

Cadotte MW, Carscadden K, Mirotchnick N. 2011. Beyond species: functional diversity and the maintenance of ecological processes and services. Journal of Applied Ecology 48: 1079-1087. Google Scholar

9.

Cane JH, Minckley RL, Kervin LJ. 2000. Sampling bees (Hymenoptera: Apiformes) for pollinator community studies: pitfalls of pan-trapping. Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 73: 225-231. Google Scholar

10.

Cane JH, Sipes S. 2006. Characterizing floral specialization by bees: analytical methods and a revised lexicon for oligolecty, pp. 99-122 In Waser NM, Ollerton J [eds.], Plant-Pollinator Interactions: From Specialization to Generalization. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA. Google Scholar

11.

Colwell RA, Coddington JA. 1994. Estimating terrestrial biodiversity through extrapolation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 345: 101-118. Google Scholar

12.

Danforth BN, Cardinal S, Praz C, Almeida EAB, Michez D. 2013. The impact of molecular data on our understanding of bee phylogeny and evolution. Annual Review of Entomology 58: 57-78. Google Scholar

13.

Díaz S, Lavorel S, de Bello F, Quétier F, Grigulis K, Robson TM. 2007. Incorporating plant functional diversity effects in ecosystem services assessments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 104: 20684-20689. Google Scholar

14.

Dray S, Legendre P. 2008. Testing the species traits-environment relationships: the fourth corner problem revisited. Ecology 89: 3400-3412. Google Scholar

15.

Droege S, Tepedino VJ, Lebuhn G, Link W, Minckley RL, Chen Q, Conrad C. 2010. Spatial patterns of bee captures in North American bowl trapping surveys. Insect Conservation and Diversity 3: 15-23. Google Scholar

16.

Flynn DFB, Gogol-Prokurat M, Nogeire T, Molinari N, Trautman Richers B, Lin BB, Simpson N, Mayfield MM, DeClerck F. 2009. Loss of functional diversity under land use intensification across multiple taxa. Ecology Letters 12: 22-33. Google Scholar

17.

Forrest JRK, Thorp RW, Kremen C, Williams NM. 2015. Contrasting patterns in species and functional-trait diversity of bees in an agricultural landscape. Journal of Applied Ecology 52: 706-715. Google Scholar

18.

Geroff RK, Gibbs J, McCravy KW. 2014. Assessing bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) diversity of an Illinois restored tallgrass prairie: methodology and conservation considerations. Journal of Insect Conservation 18: 951-964. Google Scholar

19.

Gibbs J. 2010. Revision of the metallic species of Lasioglossum (Dialictus) in Canada (Hymenoptera, Halictidae, Halictini). Zootaxa 2591: 342-346. Google Scholar

20.

Gibbs J. 2017. Notes on the nests of Augochloropsis metallica fulgida and Megachile mucida in Central Michigan (Hymenoptera: Halictidae, Megachilidae). Great Lakes Entomologist 50: 17-24. Google Scholar

21.

Gibbs J, Ascher JS, Rightmyer MG, Isaacs R. 2017a. The bees of Michigan (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila), with notes on distribution, taxonomy, pollination, and natural history. Zootaxa 4352: 1-160. Google Scholar

22.

Gibbs J, Brady SG, Kanda K, Danforth BN. 2012. Phylogeny of halictine bees supports a shared origin of eusociality for Halictus and Lasioglossum (Apoidea: Anthophila: Halictidae). Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 65: 926-939. Google Scholar

23.

Gibbs J, Joshi NK, Wilson JK, Rothwell NL, Powers K, Haas M, Gut L, Biddinger DJ, Isaacs R. 2017b. Does passive sampling accurately reflect the bee (Apoidea: Anthophila) communities pollinating apple and sour cherry orchards? Environmental Entomology 46: 579-588. Google Scholar

24.

Gonzalez VH, Park KE, Cakmak I, Hranitz JM, Barthell JF. 2016. Pan traps and bee body size in unmanaged urban habitats. Journal of Hymenoptera Research 51: 241-247. Google Scholar

25.

Greenleaf SS, Williams NM, Winfree R, Kremen C. 2007. Bee foraging ranges and their relationship to body size. Oecologia 153: 589-596. Google Scholar

26.

Grundel R, Frohnapple KJ, Jean RP, Pavlovic NB. 2011. Effectiveness of bowl trapping and netting for inventory of a bee community. Environmental Entomology 40: 374-380. Google Scholar

27.

Hooper DU, Adair EC, Cardinale BJ, Byrnes JEK, Hungate BA, Matulich KL, Gonzalez A, Duffy JE, Gamfeldt L, O'Connor MI. 2012. A global synthesis reveals biodiversity loss as a major driver of ecosystem change. Nature 486: 105-108. Google Scholar

28.

Hopwood JL. 2008. The contribution of roadside grassland restorations to native bee conservation. Biological Conservation 141: 2632-2640. Google Scholar

29.

Joshi NK, Leslie T, Rajotte EG, Kammerer MA, Otieno M, Biddinger DJ. 2015. Comparative trapping efficiency to characterize bee abundance, diversity, and community composition in apple orchards. Annals of the Entomological Society of America 108: 785-799. Google Scholar

30.

Kimoto C, Debano SJ, Thorp RW, Rao S, Stephen WP. 2012. Investigating temporal patterns of a native bee community in a remnant North American bunchgrass prairie using blue vane traps. Journal of Insect Science 12: 108.  http://www.insectscience.org/12.108  Google Scholar

31.

LaBerge WE. 1967. A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part I. Callandrena (Hymenoptera: Andrenidae). Bulletin of the University of Nebraska State Museum 7: 1-316. Google Scholar

32.

LaBerge WE. 1971. A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part IV. Scrapteropsis, Xiphandrena, and Rhaphandrena. Transactions of the American Entomological Society 97: 441-520. Google Scholar

33.

LaBerge WE. 1973. A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part VI. Subgenus Trachandrena. Transactions of the American Entomological Society 99: 235-371. Google Scholar

34.

LaBerge WE. 1977. A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part VIII. Subgenera Thysandrena, Dasyandrena, Psammandrena, Rhacandrena, Euandrena, Oxyandrena. Transactions of the American Entomological Society 103: 1-144. Google Scholar

35.

LaBerge WE. 1989. A revision of the bees of the genus Andrena of the Western Hemisphere. Part XIII. Subgenera Simandrena and Taeniandrena. Transactions of the American Entomological Society 115: 1-56. Google Scholar

36.

Laliberté E, Legendre P. 2010. A distance-based framework for measuring functional diversity from multiple traits. Ecology 91: 299-305. Google Scholar

37.

Lawson SP, Helmreich SL, Rehan SM. 2017. Effects of nutritional deprivation on development and behavior in the subsocial bee Ceratina calcarata (Hymenoptera: Xylocopinae). Journal of Experimental Biology 220: 4456-4462. Google Scholar

38.

Legendre P, Anderson MJ. 1999. Distance-based redundancy analysis: testing multispecies responses in multifactorial ecological experiments. Ecological Monographs 69: 1-24. Google Scholar

39.

León-Contrera FA, Imperatriz-Fonseca VL, Koedam D. 2006. Age-dependent mass variation in the stingless bee Melipona quadrifasciata (Apidae, Meliponini). Brazilian Journal of Morphological Sciences 23: 321-324. Google Scholar

40.

Leong JM, Thorp RW. 1999. Colour-coded sampling: the pan trap colour preferences of oligolectic and nonoligolectic bees associated with a vernal pool plant. Ecological Entomology 24: 329-335. Google Scholar

41.

McCravy KW, Geroff RK, Gibbs J. 2016. Malaise trap sampling efficiency for bees (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) in a restored tallgrass prairie. Florida Entomologist 99: 321-323. Google Scholar

42.

McCravy KW, Ruholl JD. 2017. Bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) diversity and sampling methodology in a Midwestern USA deciduous forest. Insects 8: 81. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects8030081 Google Scholar

43.

McCune B, Mefford MJ. 2016. PC-ORD: Multivariate Analysis of Ecological Data, Version 7. MjM Software Design, Gleneden Beach, Oregon, USA. Google Scholar

44.

Michener CD. 2007. The Bees of the World, 2nd ed. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, Maryland, USA. Google Scholar

45.

Mitchell TB. 1960. Bees of the eastern United States. I. [Introduction, Andrenidae, Colletidae, Halictidae, Mellitidae]. Technical Bulletin, North Carolina Agricultural Experiment Station 141: 1-538. Google Scholar

46.

Mitchell TB. 1962. Bees of the eastern United States. II. [Megachilidae, Anthophoridae, Apidae s.s.]. Technical Bulletin, North Carolina Agricultural Experiment Station 152: 1-557. Google Scholar

47.

Moretti M, de Bello F, Roberts SPM, Potts SG. 2009. Taxonomical vs. functional responses of bee communities to fire in two contrasting climatic regions. Journal of Animal Ecology 78: 98-108. Google Scholar

48.

Newbold T, Scharlemann JPW, Butchart SHM, Sekercioğlu CH, Alkemade R, Booth H, Purves DW. 2013. Ecological traits affect the response of tropical forest bird species to land-use intensity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280: 20122131.  http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2131  Google Scholar

49.

Ngo HT, Gibbs J, Griswold T, Packer L. 2013. Evaluating bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) diversity using Malaise traps in coffee landscapes of Costa Rica. Canadian Entomologist 145: 435-453. Google Scholar

50.

Peck JE. 2016. Multivariate Analysis for Ecologists: Step-by-Step, 2nd ed. MjM Software Design, Gleneden Beach, Oregon, USA. Google Scholar

51.

Pont D, Hugueny B, Beier U, Goffaux D, Melcher A, Noble R, Rogers C, Roset N, Schmutz S. 2006. Assessing river biotic condition at a continental scale: a European approach using functional metrics and fish assemblages. Journal of Applied Ecology 43: 70-80. Google Scholar

52.

Quezada-Euán JJG, López-Velasco A, Pérez-Balam J, Moo-Valle H, Velazquez-Madrazo A. 2011. Body size differs in workers produced across time and is associated with variation in the quantity and composition of larval food in Nannotrigona perilampoides (Hymenoptera, Meliponini). Insectes Sociaux 58: 31-38. Google Scholar

53.

Rhoades P, Griswold T, Waits L, Bosque-Pérez NA, Kennedy CM, Eigenbrode SD. 2017. Sampling technique affects detection of habitat factors influencing wild bee communities. Journal of Insect Conservation 21: 703-714. Google Scholar

54.

Roulston TH, Smith SA, Brewster AL. 2007. A comparison of pan trap and intensive net sampling techniques for documenting a bee (Hymenoptera: Apiformes) fauna. Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 80: 179-181. Google Scholar

55.

Schweiger O, Musche M, Bailey D, Billeter R, Dickotter T, Hendrickx F, Herzog F, Liira J, Maelfait JP, Speelmans M, Dziock F. 2007. Functional richness of local hoverfly communities (Diptera, Syrphidae) in response to land use across temperate Europe. Oikos 116: 461-472. Google Scholar

56.

Shapiro LH, Tepedino VJ, Minckley RL. 2014. Bowling for bees: optimal sample number for “bee bowl” sampling transects. Journal of Insect Conservation 18: 1105-1113. Google Scholar

57.

Sircom J, Arul Jothi G, Pinksen J. 2018. Monitoring bee populations: are eusocial bees attracted to different colours of pan trap than other bees? Journal of Insect Conservation 1572-9753 (online).  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10841-018-0071-y  Google Scholar

58.

Skandalis DA, Tattersall GJ, Prager S, Richards MH. 2009. Body size and shape of the large carpenter bee, Xylocopa virginica (L.) (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 82: 30-42. Google Scholar

59.

Stephen WP, Rao S. 2005. Unscented color traps for non-Apis bees (Hymenoptera: Apiformes). Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 78: 373-380. Google Scholar

60.

Toler TR, Evans EW, Tepedino VJ. 2005. Pan-trapping for bees (Hymenoptera: Apiformes) in Utah's West Desert: the importance of color diversity. Pan-Pacific Entomologist 81: 103-113. Google Scholar

61.

Townes H. 1972. A light-weight Malaise trap. Entomological News 83: 239-247. Google Scholar

62.

Tuell JK, Isaacs R. 2009. Elevated pan traps to monitor bees in flowering crop canopies. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 131: 93-98. Google Scholar

63.

Violle C, Navas M-L, Vile D, Kazakou E, Fortunel C, Hummel I, Garnier E. 2007. Let the concept of trait be functional! Oikos 116: 882-892. Google Scholar

64.

Williams NM, Crone EE, Roulston TH, Minckley RL, Packer L, Potts SG. 2010. Ecological and life-history traits predict bee species responses to environmental disturbances. Biological Conservation 143: 2280-2291. Google Scholar

65.

Wilson JS, Griswold T, Messinger OJ. 2008. Sampling bee communities (Hymenoptera: Apiformes) in a desert landscape: are pan traps sufficient? Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 81: 288-300. Google Scholar
Kenneth W. McCravy, Ray K. Geroff, and Jason Gibbs "Bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila) Functional Traits in Relation to Sampling Methodology in a Restored Tallgrass Prairie," Florida Entomologist 102(1), 134-140, (11 April 2019). https://doi.org/10.1653/024.102.0122
Published: 11 April 2019
KEYWORDS
functional composition
lecty
trait diversity
trap bias
Back to Top