Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 December 2004 Aspects of the Behavioral and Endocrine Ontogeny of Six Moustached Tamarins, Saguinus mystax (Callitrichinae)
Maren Huck, Petra Löttker, Eckhard W. Heymann, Michael Heistermann
Author Affiliations +

Introduction

In order to recognize differences in ontogeny between species and between their life history trajectories, it is necessary to have an understanding of the timing of milestone events in behavior and physiology (Pereira and Leigh, 2003). Assessing when and how individuals become independent from their mother and other caregivers is necessary to evaluate costs of varying parental strategies, which in turn allows broader issues to be addressed, such as the evolution of non-maternal care in anthropoid primates (Ross and Maclarnon, 2000). Patterns of ontogeny may also explain aspects of individual differences in behavior, especially, for example, when samesexed adults become competitors. Ventura and Buchanan-Smith (2003) have stressed that a good understanding of the species' development and behavior in the wild is essential to assess and ensure the well-being of maturing individuals in captivity. Observations are often anecdotal, but the difficulties and imprecision in obtaining data of this sort under field conditions can be overcome by repeated studies of groups of the same or related species.

The ontogeny of immature primates is tied to endocrinological changes. Typically the gonads are only partially developed at birth. The production of sex hormones such as testosterone increases during puberty until adult levels are reached (Plant, 1988). In adult males, testosterone and other androgens are important in the control of spermatogenesis (Wickings et al., 1986) and seem to play a role in the expression of aggressive behavior and in the achievement of social status, though this latter relationship might be less pronounced in primates (Dixson, 1980; Bouissou, 1983). In three callitrichine species (Callithrix jacchus, Callithrix kuhlii, and Saguinus oedipus), however, newborn males show relatively high testosterone levels that drop after two to six months and rise slowly again thereafter (Abbott, 1984; French and Schaffner, 1995; Ginther et al., 2002). Ginther et al. (2002) speculated that this neonatal elevation might be related to the sexual maturation of the gonads, endocrine system, and behavior, but it is not known how representative this pattern may be of other callitrichine species.

During a field study on moustached tamarins (Saguinus mystax), we were able to observe some aspects of the development of six immature individuals (five males and one female). For some of them we were able to record, for the first time, the onset and final appearance of a number of behaviors — in particular, the first observations of feeding on solid food, foraging, social play, and marking behavior, and the last days they were carried and successfully begged for food.

We expected that the patterns of testosterone and androgen in the five male immatures would be similar to those of the three callitrichines mentioned above. In addition, the immigration of a new female into one of the groups on 5 December 2001 provided the opportunity to study whether this caused endocrinological changes in the immature males. Given that one subadult male was evicted from the group following the new female's arrival, we also tested whether he and his twin differed in their testosterone levels, and whether their values differed from those of adult males in general. If co-twins differ in their sexual maturation, it might be supposed that the more mature of the two would be forced to leave the group in order to remove a potential competitor. If this is true, it might be expected that the evicted twin had higher testosterone levels than the remaining one. Different testosterone levels due to different aggression levels in the twins are unlikely, since we were able to show previously that adult moustached tamarins do not differ in their testosterone levels despite different breeding and “dominance” status (Huck et al., 2005b; note: aggression occurs too rarely to establish hierarchical relationships). These results should be considered preliminary owing to our small sample size, but they nevertheless present the first data on the behavioral ontogeny of Saguinus mystax and the first endocrine data for immature males of any callitrichine in the wild.

Methods

The study took place in 2001 (January to December) at the Estación Biológica Quebrada Blanco, in northeastern Amazonian Peru (for further details of the study site, see Heymann, 1995). We collected fecal samples and behavioral data from two habituated groups of S. mystax, each with two sets of offspring, one born in 2000 and the other in 2001. Group W contained one adult female, three adult males, male twins born in February 2000 (WM4 and WM5), and a single male offspring born in February 2001 (WM6). Group E consisted of three adult females and three adult males, a single male born in May 2000 (EM4), and twins — one male (EM6) and one female (EF4) — born in January 2001. Based on physical characteristics, Soini and Soini (1990) classified moustached tamarins as infants from birth until about three months of age, as juveniles between 4–12 months, as subadults between 13–18 months, and as adults thereafter. Following these criteria, three of the immature males (WM4, WM5, EM4) reached adulthood at the end of the study in December 2001. Two males (WM6, EM6) and one female (EF4) were born near the beginning of the investigation, developing from newborns to relatively independent juveniles over the course of the year (see Table 1). In Group W the breeding female WF1, mother of WM6 and (probably) aunt of WM4 and WM5 (relationships were evaluated by microsatellite analysis as described by Huck et al., 2005a), died near the end of the study (3 December, 2001). Two days later an unrelated female (WF3) immigrated to Group W, and one of the male subadults (WM4) was evicted on 5 December 2001. The composition of the groups and major demographic events are described in more detail by Löttker et al. (2004).

Table 1.

Immatures of Saguinus mystax study groups W and E, with mean testosterone values of the males and number of fecal samples analyzed (N).

i1413-4705-12-3-131-t01.gif

We followed each group daily from the time they left their sleeping site (about 05:45) until they entered their sleeping site in the afternoon (about 15:45 h). From January to December 2001 we accumulated a total of 3004 and 3257 contact hours over 330 and 351 days for groups W and E, respectively. The average visibility of individuals — measured as the percentage of hourly scans in which each individual could be seen — was 18.0 and 18.6% for adults and immatures in group W, respectively, and 11.3 and 10.9% for adults and immatures in group E. Following the protocols for behavior sampling of Martin and Bateson (1993), we recorded the first observation of an infant independent of a carrier (being “off ”), eating solid food (without distinguishing between prey or fruit), engaging in social play, and foraging for prey. We also noted the last observations of an infant being carried and of successfully begging for food. Except for the data on food-begging, the records were taken from the younger litter in the two groups (i.e., WM6 in group W, and EM6 and EF4 in group E). We were unable to distinguish the Group E twins, EM6 and EF4, until they were 81 days old; until then, data of the first occurrences of either one of the two infants are used. Data on food-begging were recorded from the older immatures (WM4 and WM5 in group W, EM4 in group E).

For hormonal analyses, we collected a total of 151 fecal samples from immature males between January and December 2001 (see Table 1 for the number of samples per individual). Immediately after an animal was seen defecating, the feces were collected and immersed in 96% ethanol. (The protocols for treatment and storage of the samples are given in Huck et al. [2005b], as are techniques for hormone extraction and measurement of immunoreactive testosterone by enzyme immunoassay.) Hormone concentrations are given in ng/g fecal dry weight and were log-10-transformed for parametric statistics.

In order to examine changes in testosterone excretion over time, we calculated Pearson's correlation coefficient (r) between age and mean monthly testosterone values. As in Callithrix jacchus, Callithrix kuhlii and Saguinus oedipus, testosterone levels in newborns drop only after two to six months and rise slowly again thereafter (cf. Abbott, 1984; French and Schaffner, 1995; Ginther et al., 2002). The data depicted in Figure 1 appear to show a drop in testosterone levels between months 4 and 5 (around day 140). Therefore we also calculated the coefficient r for the younger males WM6 and EM6, excluding the potentially high values before their 140th day of life, which seems to correspond to the turning point in testosterone production described for Callithrix jacchus, Callithrix kuhlii, and Saguinus oedipus at similar ages (Abbott, 1984; French and Schaffner, 1995; Ginther et al., 2002). Three samples were collected from WM6 before he was 140 days old and one for EM6.

Figure 1.

Log-10-transformed fecal hormone concentrations for all individuals related to relative age (in days). Curves give the distanceweighted least-squares fit for each male. Only the month, not the exact date of birth was known for EM4, so it was taken to be 15 May.

i1413-4705-12-3-131-f01.gif

Unfortunately we could not obtain enough samples for statistical analysis after WF1's death (3 December 2001) and the subsequent emigration of WM4 (5 December 2001); we collected only one sample each for WM4 and WM6. Instead, we evaluated whether the mean hormone concentration of these samples collected after the new female had immigrated lay above the 95% upper boundary of the group average when WF1 was still alive. In addition, we compared testosterone levels between the twin brothers in group W and in relation to mean testosterone levels of the adult males in the two study groups W and E (values for adult males appear in Huck et al., 2005b).

Results

Our first and last observations of milestone events in behavior are listed in Table 2. Nursing was observed only rarely and was not included. Apart from “last time seen successfully begging”, the data report on the second litters only (WM6 in group W, and EM6 and EF4 in group E). Table 1 shows the testosterone concentrations of all samples by individual and the distance-weighted least-squares fit for each. No immature showed a significant increase in testosterone levels during the year, and none of the correlations between age and mean monthly testosterone values were significant (WM4: r = 0.06, p = 0.86; WM5: r = 0.37, p = 0.23; WM6: r = −0.03, p = 0.94; EM4: r = 0.21, p = 0.50; EM6: r = 0.11, p = 0.83).

Table 2.

First and last occurrence of behavioral milestone events in two study groups of S. mystax.

i1413-4705-12-3-131-t02.gif

When we calculated the regressions for WM6 and EM6 after their 140th day of life, only WM6 showed a significant increase in testosterone values over the following six months until December 2001 (r = 0.50, p = 0.012, see Fig. 1); we did not find such a correlation for EM6 for the seven months after his 140th day of life until December 2001 (r = −0.27, p = 0.37; see Fig. 1).

We had no samples from WM4 after the death of the breeding female WF1. The samples of WM5 and WM6, collected after immigration of the new female, both fell above the 95% boundary (Table 3). WM5 had significantly lower testosterone values than the adult males of group W, but the values of WM4 did not differ significantly from adult levels (mean and SD for adults: 2782.1 ± 3233.3; for WM4 and WM5: see Table 1. Scheffé post hoc tests: adults vs. WM4: p = 0.72; adults vs. WM5: p = 0.014). The testosterone levels of the twin brothers (WM4 and WM5) did not differ significantly but tended to be lower in WM5 (Scheffé posthoc test: p = 0.30; compare fit curve in Fig. 1).

Table 3.

Mean (untransformed) testosterone values and the upper 95% boundary of juveniles of Group W during the tenure of WF1, and the value of a sample collected after the immigration of a new female.

i1413-4705-12-3-131-t03.gif

Discussion

In general, the two study groups showed consistent results but differed in two ways. First, WM6 began foraging and showing social play later than his peers from group E. This might have been due to his having been raised as a singleton; his sibling was killed by a raptor at the age of 28 days (Oversluijs Vásquez and Heymann, 2001). Other studies have shown that infants prefer to play with same-aged playmates and that singletons play significantly less than twins (e.g., Cleveland and Snowdon, 1984). This holds true even when older siblings are available as potential playmates (Cleveland and Snowdon, 1984). Without further study we cannot say whether our results are representative and, if they are, whether the reduced play retarded the development of motor skills vital to foraging or whether — with only a single infant to care for — caregivers are more indulgent, resulting in less need for an infant to forage for itself.

The only slight (non-significant) increase of testosterone levels with increasing age in all subjects was unexpected. WM6 showed a testosterone excretion pattern similar to those described for other callitrichines (Abbott, 1984; French and Schaffner, 1995), with high levels very early in life that drop after three months and then rise again after the seventh month. The similar-aged EM6 did not show this pattern, but this might be due to the fact that only one fecal sample was analyzed before his 140th day of life. There was a strong increase in testosterone concentrations in the samples of WM5 and the considerably younger WM6 collected after the death of the related WF1 and the subsequent immigration of the unrelated female WF3.

With only one fecal sample in each case, this finding must be regarded with caution, but we suspect that the elevated levels might be related to the fact that incest avoidance no longer had a role to play (see also Baker et al., 1999, for Callithrix jacchus). In addition, WM4, who was later evicted, seems to have been more precocious than his brother. Although the twins in group W showed no differences in time budgets (G-test with William's correction, G6 = 11.6, p = n.s.; unpubl. data), their endocrine levels were different (although non-significantly), and WM4 may have posed more of a threat to the adult males after the arrival of the unrelated female WF3. The testosterone values of WM5 were significantly lower than adult levels, while those of his brother were not. WM4 had apparently reached adult levels before the onset of the study when he was 10.5 months old. In contrast, his twin WM5 reached adult levels only at the end of the study at nearly 23 months old (cf. Ginther et al., 2002, for similar ranges). Age and morphology alone are thus insufficient predictors of sexual maturity, since testosterone levels, which are likely to be related to sperm production and fertility, differ widely during maturation even between twins.

Acknowledgments

We thank our field assistants for their invaluable help in collecting samples. Fieldwork was carried out under a letter of understanding between the Universidad Nacional de la Amazonía Peruana (UNAP), Iquitos, and the Deutsches Primatenzentrum, Göttingen, and complied with Peruvian laws. Financial support was provided by the DFG (HE 1870/10-1,2).

References

1.

D. H. Abbott 1984. Behavioral and physiological suppression of fertility in subordinate marmoset monkeys. Am. J. Primatol 6:169–186. Google Scholar

2.

J. V. Baker, D. H. Abbott, and W. Saltzman . 1999. Social determinants of reproductive failure in male common marmosets housed with their natal family. Anim. Behav 58:501–513. Google Scholar

3.

M-F. Bouissou 1983. Androgens, aggressive behaviour and social relationships in higher mammals. Hormone Res 18:43–61. Google Scholar

4.

J. Cleveland and C. T. Snowdon . 1984. Social development during the first twenty weeks in the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus o. oedipus). Anim. Behav 32:432–444. Google Scholar

5.

A. F. Dixson 1980. Androgens and aggressive behavior in primates: A review. Aggressive Behav 6:37–67. Google Scholar

6.

J. A. French and C. M. Schaffner . 1995. Social and developmental influences on urinary testosterone levels in male black tufted-ear marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Am. J. Primatol 36:123. Google Scholar

7.

A. J. Ginther, A. A. Carlson, T. E. Ziegler, and C. T. Snowdon . 2002. Neonatal and pubertal development in males of a cooperatively breeding primate, the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). Biol. Reprod 66:282–290. Google Scholar

8.

E. W. Heymann 1995. Sleeping habits of tamarins, Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis (Mammalia; Primates; Callitrichidae), in north-eastern Peru. J. Zool., Lond 237:211–226. Google Scholar

9.

M. Huck, P. Löttker, U-R. Böhle, and E. W. Heymann . 2005a. Paternity and kinship patterns in polyandrous moustached tamarins (Saguinus mystax). Am. J. Phys. Anthropol 127:449–464. Google Scholar

10.

M. Huck, P. Löttker, E. W. Heymann, and M. Heistermann . 2005b. Characterization and social correlates of fecal testosterone and cortisol excretion in wild Saguinus mystax. Int. J. Primatol 26:159–179. Google Scholar

11.

P. Löttker, M. Huck, and E. W. Heymann . 2004. Group composition and demographic events in wild moustached tamarins (Saguinus mystax). Am. J. Primatol 64:425–249. Google Scholar

12.

P. Martin and P. Bateson . 1993. Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge. Google Scholar

13.

M. R. Oversluijs Vásquez and E. W. Heymann . 2001. Crested eagle (Morphnus guianensis) predation on infant tamarins (Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis, Callitrichinae). Folia Primatol 72:301–303. Google Scholar

14.

M. E. Pereira and S. R. Leigh . 2003. Modes of primate development. In Primate Life Histories and Socioecology. P. M. Kappeler and M. E. Pereira , editors. (eds.), pp. 149–176.The University of Chicago Press. Chicago. Google Scholar

15.

T. M. Plant 1988. Puberty in primates. In The Physiology of Reproduction. E. Knobil, J. D. Neill, and G. S. Greenwald , editors. (eds.), pp. 1763–1788.Raven Press. New York. Google Scholar

16.

C. Ross and A. MacLarnon . 2000. The evolution of non-maternal care in anthropoid primates: A test of the hypotheses. Folia Primatol 71:93–113. Google Scholar

17.

P. Soini and M. Soini . 1990. Distribución geográfica y ecológia poblacional de Saguinus mystax. In La Primatología en el Perú: Investigaciones Primatológicas (1973–1985). N. E. Castro-Rodríguez , editor. (ed.), pp. 272–313.Imprenta Propaceb. Lima. Google Scholar

18.

R. Ventura and H. M. Buchanan-Smith . 2003. Physical environmental effects on infant care and development in captive Callithrix jacchus. Int. J. Primatol 24:399–413. Google Scholar

19.

E. J. Wickings, G. R. Marshall, and E. Nieschlag . 1986. Endocrine regulation of male reproduction. In Comparative Primate Biology. W. R. Dukelow and J. Erwin , editors. (eds.), pp. 149–170.Alan R. Liss. New York. Google Scholar

Notes

[1] Maren Huck, Department of Biology & Environmental Science, John Maynard Smith Building, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QG, Great Britain, e-mail: <maren_huck@hotmail.com>

[2] Petra Löttker, Abteilung Verhaltensökologie & Soziobiologie, Deutsches Primatenzentrum, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany, e-mail: <ploettker@dpz.gwdg.de> and Institut für Neuro- und Verhaltensbiologie, Abteilung Verhaltensbiologie, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität, Münster, Germany

[3] Eckhard W. Heymann, Abteilung Verhaltensökologie & Soziobiologie, Deutsches Primatenzentrum, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany, e-mail: <eheyman@gwdg.de>

[4] Michael Heistermann, Abteilung Reproduktionsbiologie, Deutsches Primatenzentrum, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany, e-mail: <mheiste@gwdg.de>.

Maren Huck, Petra Löttker, Eckhard W. Heymann, and Michael Heistermann "Aspects of the Behavioral and Endocrine Ontogeny of Six Moustached Tamarins, Saguinus mystax (Callitrichinae)," Neotropical Primates 12(3), 131-135, (1 December 2004). https://doi.org/10.1896/1413-4705.12.3.131
Published: 1 December 2004
Back to Top