Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2009 Developmental Stages in the Howler Monkey, Subspecies Alouatta Palliata Mexicana: A New Classification Using Age-Sex Categories
Cristina Domingo Balcells, Joaquím José Veà Baró
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

We present a new classification of age-sex categories for the mantled howler monkey Alouatta palliata. This classification includes only those physical and behavioral characteristics that can be distinguished under field conditions, with the goal of being able to infer the approximate age of monkeys in the wild. Our classification is based on data collected ad libitum during monthly censuses of 8 groups of A. p. mexicana in a fragmented landscape on the “Los Tuxtlas” Biosphere Reserve at the northern limit of this species' distribution. Our new classification system contains 10 categories that can be compared directly to existing classification schemes to facilitate cross-site studies. We compare the results of our study to the currently used classification system which was based on more southern populations of A. palliata in Panamá and Costa Rica.

Presentamos una nueva clasificación de categorías de edad-sexo para los monos aulladores Alouatta palliata. Esta clasificación incluye solamente aquellas características físicas y comportamentales que pueden ser distinguidas en condiciones de campo, con el propósito de poder inferir la edad aproximada de los monos en estado silvestre. Nuestra clasificación está basada en datos colectados ad libitum durante censos mensuales de 8 grupos de A. p. mexicana en un paisaje fragmentado el la Reserva de la Biósfera “Los Tuxtlas” en el límite norte de distribución de esta especie. Nuestro nuevo sistema de clasificación contiene 10 categorías que pueden ser comparadas directamente con esquemas de clasificación existentes para facilitar lo estudios de diferentes sitios. Comparamos los resultados de nuestro estudio con el sistema de clasificación actualmente utilizado el cual se basaba en poblaciones de A. palliata más sureñas en Panamá y Costa Rica.

Introduction

The study of ontogeny is essential to evolutionary anthropology and primatology since it allows for the comparison of developmental and growth strategies among species. Within a given species, establishing life stages allows one to understand the degree of variability in the development and maturation patterns found in different populations (Neville et al., 1988). Furthermore, for a wide range of primatological studies it is crucial to be able to determine the main developmental stages of the individuals of a species and estimate their age based on their characteristics. For this purpose, the sequence of dental eruption and the length of the long bones are considered to provide the most accurate information (baboons: Phillips-Conroy and Jolly, 1988; howlers: DeGusta and Milton 1998; De Gusta et al., 2003; vervet monkeys: Bolter and Zihlman, 2003; chimpanzees: Zihlman et al., 2004). However, for the analysis of hard tissue it is necessary to examine the individual directly and in the majority of field studies this may not be feasible, may be costly, or may even be harmful to the primates. As such, standardized classifications for each species are useful as they allow the ages of the individuals and their developmental stage to be estimated based only on morphological and behavioral characteristics that are observable from a certain distance.

In demographic and ethological studies these classifications can be used to describe the group composition, age at sexual maturity or migration, morphological and behavioral changes associated with these processes or to examine the influence of ecological constraints on the different developmental stages in a primate population (Bolter and Zihlman, 2003). On the other hand, depending on the physical and behavioral criteria that are applied, the assessment of these stages can vary within a species or even within a population (Bramblett, 1980). Additionally, genetic variation within and between populations has been frequently identified as a cause of differences in the development of individuals (Arendt, 1997). The habitat niche can also modify the growth and development pattern of different populations of the same species (Bolter and Zihlman, 2003). Geographic variation as well as altitudinal and latitudinal, temperature and precipitation gradients, often create genetic clines in growth and development rates (Arendt, 1997). In contrast, when and how to grow, mature and reproduce depend on the state of the organism, including its physiological condition and the associated ecological costs (Gotthard, 2001), which in turn depend on the environment, the risks of predation and resource availability.

For the mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata) there are few published classifications that define developmental stages by age based on observable characteristics. The most used systems differentiate between immature individuals and adults, or between a few different stages of development (Table 1) and are based on data for the southern populations of howlers from Barro Colorado Island (BCI), Panamá (Carpenter, 1934, 1965) and from Costa Rica (Glander, 1980; Clarke, 1990). These classifications have been used extensively to classify wild howler monkeys by their age throughout their wide distribution. Mantled howlers are found from southern Mexico through Central America and part of Colombia and Ecuador. Despite being one of the most studied New World primate genera, Alouatta species' systematic relationships remain unclear (Cortés-Ortiz et al., 2003), and subsequently, it is unknown whether this genetic differentiation has any influence on the development of individuals or the relation between age and external changes. Here we present a new system of classification in age-sex categories for the mantled howler monkey in its most northern distribution, in the region of Los Tuxtlas, Mexico. We compare and discuss our results with the current classification systems from Panamá and Costa Rica.

Table 1.

Classification of A. palliata by age-sex category.

t01_01.gif

Methods

Study site

This study was carried out in a fragmented landscape of disturbed tropical rainforest, located in southern Veracruz, Mexico. The area is within the buffer zone of the Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (18°34′ 18°37′N and 95°02′, 95°08′W; elevation in the study area: 25–400 m a.s.l.). The climate is warm and humid (Soto and Gama, 1997) with a mean annual temperature of 25° C and annual precipitation of 4710 mm (EBT, Los Tuxtlas Biology Station, 1996–2005). The dry season occurs from January or February to May and the rainy season is from June to December or January (EBT, 1996–2005), with strong winds from the north from November to February (Soto and Gama, 1997). Currently, there are no large predators, such as jaguar or harpy eagle, in the zone, although there have been reports of attempted attacks by tayra on a troop of howlers (Asensio and Gómez-Marín, 2002).

Data collection

Eight groups of howler monkeys were monitored over a total of 23 non-consecutive months in daily sessions of 6 to 8 hours starting at sunrise. Each group was visited several times each month to ensure reliable observations. Data were collected from October 2003 to November 2005 by two teams, each with a researcher and a field assistant, who made every effort to apply homogeneous criteria, as described below. At the beginning of the study there were a total of 20 immature in the eight groups and over the observation period there were 20 births that could be assigned to a specific month. All the animals observed were individually recognized by their natural markings typical of the subspecies. Their identification cards were kept up-to-date throughout the study, and complemented with drawings, photographs and videos. To follow individual development, during each session the observation date and the particular characteristics of the individual at that moment were recorded on each card ad libitum. Most of the morphological and behavioral characteristics used in previously established classification systems were included. Morphology: color and length of fur, relative body structure and size, appearance of the genitals, head shape. Behavior: relationship with the mother, presence/absence of sexual behavior, type of locomotion, feeding and play. The characteristics are listed in Table 2 of the Results section.

Estimating the age corresponding to different developmental stages in howlers

Once the field work had been completed, “time lines” were drawn for each individual that was born during the study and therefore of known age. We call the representation of the age in months of each monkey its “time line”, and used these together with morphological and behavioral changes that occurred as the study progressed to describe each stage. Using the characteristics recorded, a life stage was assigned to each individual for each observation, applying each of the existing classification systems presented in Table 1. This made it possible to compare the degree of agreement between these classification systems with respect to the assigned stage and in comparison with the animal's real age. Owing to discrepancies detected, the classifications were modified and a new one was created that homogenized the criteria and fit the data of our study. Then, this new system was used to estimate the age of the 20 immature that were present in the first census of the study. Using our pilot classification system, the life stage was assigned to each individual for each observation based solely on the animal's characteristics. In one column the age range estimated using this classification was noted, and in another column the age range estimated at the first observation of the individual according to its characteristics was noted, and to this age we added the elapsed months for the subsequent observations. Finally, agreement between the columns was compared to test whether the classification and the estimated ages fit. Applying this method systematically, we were able to estimate the age ranges for the life stages leading up to maturity.

Figure 1.

Time lines for the 40 immature individuals studied (see text). The category corresponding to the age in months is shown for each individual, applying the classification scheme proposed in this study. Time lines starting at zero (individuals 21–40) correspond to the 20 individuals that were born during the observation period. Lines 1–20 represent immature already present at the beginning of the study.

f01_01.eps

Results

Using the time lines, we present the development of the 40 immature studied (Fig. 1). The category corresponding to the age in months is shown for each individual throughout the study, from the first observation to end of the last observation at the end of the study or when the animal disappeared, either because it died or emigrated. To simplify interpretation, adult categories are not included in the figure and the time lines are drawn as continuous, even though all individuals were not observed during all the months (e.g. sample gaps or emigration-immigration events, see Balcells, 2008 for further details on the groups and individuals history). The complete classification by age-sex categories fit to our Los Tuxtlas population is summarized in Table 2 where the distinguishing characteristics for each stage and the estimated age range are listed. The classification we propose is constructed based on those listed in Table 1. There are 10 categories; more than those proposed by other authors, but they can easily be grouped together when required because of the data or the study. We suggest that the maximum number of categories be used to obtain a more accurate estimate of age based on characteristics that are easy to record in the field over a reasonable amount of observation time. On the other hand, any classification of a continuous temporal process is arbitrary and because of this, under field conditions we frequently find individuals that are difficult to assign to one or another of two adjacent categories; these i–ii transition stages (e.g. Infant 2–3) can be read in Table 2 as n/n+1.

Table 2.

Age-sex classification of the Los Tuxtlas howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata mexicana).

t02a_01.gif

continued

t02b_01.gif

continued

t02c_01.gif

Some important behavioral events in the lives of primates are of interest when comparing individual's age with external appearance. Howlers usually leave their natal group before reaching maturity (Glander, 1992). Accordingly, there is notable migratory activity in our study population at the group level (Balcells, 2008), and the individuals in the juvenile and subadult categories are often solitary and secretive. In Los Tuxtlas natal emigration generally occurs when the sex of the individual is not externally detectable under field conditions (see also Clarke et al., 2007, for evidence of juvenile monomorphism in A. palliata). As such any difference in migration age and behavior between the sexes is not easily distinguishable without later long term follow-up. During our study, we were able to observe only two individuals and determine their sex after they had emigrated. Individual 10 (Fig. 1) returned to his group and was identified as a Subadult male (according to our classification system), having abandoned the group at 27–29 months of age. Individual 15 emigrated at 20–22 months old and later was identified as a small solitary Subadult female behaving evasively. Our data suggest that emigration can occur in any of the three juvenile stages (25% Juvenile 1, 37.5% Juvenile 2 and 37.5% Juvenile 3, n=8 observed disappearances of immatures in age of non-dependency from mother) and 3 to 4 of the migrations coincided with the birth of a sibling.

In addition to migration, weaning and first reproduction are also key periods in the lives of howlers. During our study, we were only able to infer the age of one female (individual 19, Fig.1) when she first gave birth, at between 39 and 42 months of age; as calculated for a gestation period of 6 months (Cortés-Ortiz et al., 1993; Glander, 1980), and that first estrus and copulations occur at around 35 months. For males, the beginning of reproductive activity probably depends on social aspects that give them access to the females, but during our study we were not able to observe this process. With respect to feeding, complete weaning in Los Tuxtlas was observed at the end of the Juvenile 1 stage, at 18–20 months of age. It is common to observe a Juvenile 1 (from 15 to 19 months old) still suckling opportunistically when the mother is resting, although during this stage it appears that individuals are no longer dependent on their mother for food, because they can survive as solitaries and some individuals leave the natal group before reaching 15 months of age.

Discussion

Comparison of classification systems

The classification systems analyzed here for age-sex categories of Alouatta palliata coincide with each other to differing degrees. On the one hand, Carpenter's (1965) classes have wide age ranges and very advanced ages for the accompanying physical and behavioral characteristics when compared with other systems, including the one we propose for Los Tuxtlas and an earlier one by the same author from 1934. The age assigned for individuals with similar characteristics can differ by as much as 15 months, in spite of being classified as the same morphological stage (e.g. Juvenile 1 is 3–6 months old according to Glander, 1980 and 20–30 months according to Carpenter, 1965). Glander (1980) also proposes a wide range of ages for Juvenile 2 (6–30 months), that would include Carpenter's (1934) Juvenile 1 and 2 stages (in both characteristics and age), and from the end of Infant 3 to Juvenile 3 for the system we propose for Los Tuxtlas. In general, our classification system is similar, with certain differences, to the systems of Carpenter (1934) and Clarke (1990). Carpenter (1934) classifies as Juvenile 3 those individuals with characteristics similar to those of the Subadult stage in the Los Tuxtlas classification system. Clarke's (1990) classification proposes age ranges that are very similar to those of our system, although she assigns an age of 20 to 36 months to Juvenile 2, and includes individuals that we would classify as Juvenile 3 and Subadult in this stage. However, for some studies it might be interesting to differentiate between individuals that begin to exhibit sexual traits and the juveniles that do not yet show any external evidence of their sexual identity given that these stages are associated with physiological and social changes. Additionally, differentiating between these stages allows a closer fit to the estimated range of ages for the individuals in the pre-maturation stages that are critical to group dynamics (Glander, 1992).

As regards key events during the development of howler monkeys, there is greater consensus. From our data, sexual maturity in females occurs at approximately 36 months and the first birth occurs at 43 months. This agrees with reports by Glander (1980) and Clarke (1990). According to Clarke (1990) weaning occurs during Juvenile 1 (18 months) in females, while males become independent earlier, both in locomotion and feeding. Carpenter (1934) agrees with this, and although in 1965 he said weaning occurs during Juvenile 1, a much older age is assigned (20–30 months). Glander (1980) does not specify any age for weaning. In Los Tuxtlas, complete weaning was observed at the end of Juvenile 1, at 18–20 months old when individuals can emigrate and survive without suckling, and this coincides with the findings of Carpenter (1934) and Clarke (1990). Finally, the emigration age in males is 12–20 months according to Clarke (1990), while females emigrate at 22–24 months. Our data suggest that the birth of a sibling is an important factor for triggering emigration, and emigration tends to occur before the sex of the individual can be identified and so any difference associated with the sex of the individual is difficult to discern at the time of emigration. More data are required to determine whether there are differences between the sexes or individuals in emigration age, and to broaden our understanding of the factors that might produce any differences observed.

Genetics, environment and development

Although growth rates are genetically imprinted, they are phenotypically plastic (Laurila et al., 2001), and a function of the adaptive balance between the costs and benefits associated with development (Gotthard, 2001). In two populations of captive macaques, differences were found in growth rates that could be attributed to their different origins, the different climates of the centers, different social structure or genetic profile (Vancata et al., 2000). Also, in langurs the socio-ecology of the species interacts with its development patterns (Washburn, 1942). In baboons, as a result of improved nutrition, growth rate varies among groups during the same year and for the same group in different years (Strum, 2005). In vervets, reproductive maturity is early compared to that of baboons and is considered an adaptation to unpredictable food sources (Rowell, 1977). On the other hand, the early consumption of solid food in indris could reduce maternal investment (Godfrey et al., 2004). However, in some Platyrrhini (Aotus, Callicebus, Saimiri, Callimico, Saguinus, Leontopithecus, Cebuella and Callithrix), ecological and social factors, such as predation and competition for food do not appear to adequately explain the differences observed in infant development and growth rates (Garber and Leigh, 1997). In howlers, the lack of consistency in the age classifications within each geographic area prevents us from asserting that environmental differences significantly affect the development strategies of the populations. Although the forage available to the different populations studied might offer differing nutritional value owing to differences in habitat and vegetation (Table 1), this factor probably does not have a significant influence on development (at least for field observations). Howler monkeys can restrict their energetic expenditure behaviorally (Milton, 1980) and, owing to their highly folivore and generalized diet, are capable of using different resources, even in disturbed habitats (Rodríguez-Luna et al., 2003; Cristobal and Arroyo, 2007). For the same reason, the effects of intra- or interspecific competition in habitats with limited resources are mitigated (Van Schaik, 1989).

On the other hand, howler monkeys are not subject to severe predation pressure in any of the study sites because of their large size and the decrease in the populations of potential predators; hence this factor is also not likely to exert a significant influence at present. Finally, latitude does not appear to have an important effect on the rate of development of A. palliata, given that there are no consistent differences between the Mexican and more southern populations, in spite of the fact that latitude might be important in fish and amphibians (Duponchelle and Legendre, 2000). It is, however, worth mentioning that in a study using 7 microsatellites for different populations of A. palliata in Mesoamerica, Cortés-Ortiz (2003) found a marked decrease in the genetic variation of the populations from south to north where A. p. mexicana (from Mexico) was the least variable population, followed by A. p. palliata (from Costa Rica), and the population of A. p. aequetorialis (from Panamá) had the greatest variation. Even though populations of A. p. mexicana do not appear to be facing any detrimental effects owing to the reduction in the genetic variation, this fact could cause differences between populations and should be considered when making comparisons between different populations (subspecies). The incorporation of molecular data in demographic-behavioral studies of different populations would help to reveal the effect of the loss of genetic variation in the ontogenetic development of howler monkeys.

The consequences of using different criteria

According to Neville et al. (1988), the use of different criteria for classifying wild Alouatta palliata individuals can interfere with the comparison and interpretation of the results obtained by different authors over a wide variety of studies. In demographic studies, not considering subadults as a separate class could result in an underestimation of the number of juveniles or reproductive adults in the censuses. Similarly, certain individuals would be considered as infants or juveniles depending on the classification system. Therefore, it is necessary to keep in mind which classification system has been applied to evaluate the evolution of populations for censuses that use the number of immatures per reproductive female (Heltne et al., 1976). Even purely behavioral and socio-ecological studies (e.g. daily activity pattern, diet, play, socialization) that use distinct age classifications to compare behavior patterns between categories of individuals can lead to contradictory conclusions as a result of the criteria applied. The same problem occurs in studies for which it is necessary to infer the age of the individuals in order to prepare life tables, evaluate relative mortality for each stage and age, infer the birth month of the individuals, time of weaning, reproduction or migration.

Our results indicate the need to unify the criteria for classification by age-sex categories for mantled howler monkeys, and to identify consistent differences between populations with different characteristics. Further long term studies are needed in Los Tuxtlas, Mexico and the other places where this species is found.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to B. Hervier and S. Vegas for providing data and to C. Pérez for participating in long hours of field work. We thank the Los Tuxtlas Biological Station-UNAM for climate information. For valuable comments on a previous version of the manuscript we thank L. Cortés-Ortiz, P.A.D. Dias and J. Cristóbal-Azkarate. CD Balcells was supported by Floquet de Neu scholarship 1st ed. and from the Ministry of External Affairs in Mexico. For infrastructure support we thank the Ministry of Education and Science of Spain BSO2002-03340 and SESJ2005 1562/ PSIC, and E. Rodríguez-Luna of Universidad Veracruzana, Mexico. The research complied with protocols approved by the appropriate institutional animal care committee of SEMARNAT-the Mexican Office for the Environment and Natural Resources, and adhered to the legal requirements of Mexico.

References

1.

J. D. Arendt 1997. Adaptive intrinsic growth rates: An integration across taxa. Q. Rev. Biol. 72: 149–177. Google Scholar

2.

N. Asensio and F. Gómez-Marín 2002. Interspecific interaction and predator avoidance behavior in response to tayra (Eira barbara) by mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Primates 43: 339–341. Google Scholar

3.

C. D. Balcells 2008. Efectos de la fragmentación del habitat en la evolución demográfica del mono aullador (Alouatta palliata mexicana) . Doctoral thesis, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain. Google Scholar

4.

D. R. Bolter and A. L. Zihlman 2003. Morphometric analysis of growth and development in wild-collected vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops), with implications for growth patterns in Old World monkeys, apes and humans. J. Zool. Lond. 260: 99–110. Google Scholar

5.

C. A. Bramblett 1980. Model for development of social behavior in vervet monkeys. Dev. Psychol. 13: 205–223. Google Scholar

6.

C. R. Carpenter 1934. A field study of the behavior and social relations of howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Comp. Psychol. Monogr. 10 (2): 1–168. Google Scholar

7.

C. R. Carpenter 1965. The howlers of Barro Colorado Island. In: Primate Behavior , I. De Vore (ed.), pp. 250–291. Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York. Google Scholar

8.

M. R. Clarke 1990. Behavioral development and socialization of infants in a free-ranging group of howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Folia Primatol. 54: 1–15. Google Scholar

9.

M. R. Clarke , E. L. Zucker , R. T. Ford , and R. M. Harrison 2007. Behavior and endocrine concentrations do not distinguish sex in monomorphic juvenile howlers (Alouatta palliata). Am. J. Primatol. 69: 477–484. Google Scholar

10.

L. Cortés-Ortiz , E. Bermingham , C. Rico , E. Rodríguez-Luna , I. Sampaio , and M. Ruiz-García 2003. Molecular systematics and biogeography of the Neotropical monkey genus, Alouatta. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 26: 64–81. Google Scholar

11.

L. Cortés-Ortiz 2003. Evolution of Howler Monkeys, Genus Alouatta. Dissertation, University of East Anglia, Norwich, England. Google Scholar

12.

J. Cristóbal-Azkarate and V. Arroyo-Rodríguez 2007. Diet and Activity Pattern of Howler Monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in Los Tuxtlas, Mexico: Effects of Habitat Fragmentation and Implications for Conservation. Am. J. Primatol. 69: 1–17. Google Scholar

13.

D. De Gusta and K. Milton 1998. Skeletal pathologies in a population of Alouatta palliata: behavioral, ecological, and evolutionary implications. Int. J. Primatol. 19(3): 615–650. Google Scholar

14.

D. De Gusta , M. A. Everett , and K. Milton 2003. Natural selection on molar size in a wild population of howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). P. Roy. Soc. Lon. B. Bio. 270: 15–17. Google Scholar

15.

F. Duponchelle and M. Legendre 2000. Oreochromis niloticus (Cichlidae) in Lake Ayame, Côte d'Ivoire: life history traits of a strongly diminished population. Cybium 24(2): 161–172. Google Scholar

16.

P. A. Garber and S. R. Leigh 1997. Ontogenetic variation in small-bodied New World primates: implications for patterns of reproduction and infant care. Folia Primatol. 68(1): 1–22. Google Scholar

17.

K. E. Glander 1980. Reproduction and population growth in free-ranging mantled howling monkeys. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 53: 25–36. Google Scholar

18.

K. E. Glander 1992. Dispersal patterns in Costa Rican Mantled Howling Monkeys. Int. J. Primatol. 13 (4): 415–435. Google Scholar

19.

L. R. Godfrey , K. E. Samonds , W. L. Jungers , M. R. Sutherland , and M. T. Irwin 2004. Ontogenetic Correlates of Diet in Malagasy Lemurs. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 123: 250–276. Google Scholar

20.

K. Gotthard 2001. Growth strategies of ectothermic animals in temperate environments. In: Animal Developmental Ecology , D. Atkinson and M. Thorndyke (eds.), chapter 15. BIOS Scientific Publishers Ltd, Oxford. Google Scholar

21.

P. G. Heltne , D. C Turner , and N. J. Scott Jr. 1976. Comparison of census data on Alouatta palliata from Costa Rica and Panama. In: Neotropical Primates: Field studies and conservation , R. W. Thorington and P. G. Heltne (eds.), pp. 10–19. National Academy of Science, Washington DC. Google Scholar

22.

A. Laurila , S. Pakkasmaa , and J. Merilä 2001. Influence of seasonal time constraints on growth and development of common frog tadpoles: a photoperiod experiment. Oikos 95: 451–460. Google Scholar

23.

K. Milton 1980. The foraging strategy of howler monkeys. A study in primate economics. Columbia University Press, New York. Google Scholar

24.

M. K. Neville , K. E. Glander , E. Braza , and A. B. Rylands 1988. The howling monkeys: genus Alouatta. In: Ecology and behavior of Neotropical primates , A. M. Russell , R. A. Mittermeier , A. B. Rylands , A. Coimbra-Filho and G. A. B. Fonseca (eds.), pp. 349–453. World Wildlife Fund, Washington DC. Google Scholar

25.

J. E. Phillips-Conroy and C. J. Jolly 1988. Dental eruption schedules of wild and captive baboons. Am. J. Primatol. 15: 17–29. Google Scholar

26.

E. Rodríguez-Luna , L. E. Domínguez-Domínguez , J.E. Morales-Mávil and M. Martínez-Morales 2003. Foraging strategy changes in an Alouatta palliata mexicana troop released on an island. In: Primates in Fragments: Ecology and Conservation , L. K. Marsh (ed.), pp 229–250. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New York. Google Scholar

27.

T. Rowell 1977. Variation in age at puberty in monkeys. Folia Primatol. 27: 284–296. Google Scholar

28.

A. Soto and L. Gama 1997. Climas. In: Historia Natural de Los Tuxtlas , E. González-Soriano , R. Dirzo and R. C. Vogt (eds.), pp. 7–23. UNAM, Mexico DF. Google Scholar

29.

S. C. Strum 2005. Weight and age in wild olive baboons. Am. J. Primatol. 25 (4): 219–237. Google Scholar

30.

V. Vancata , M. A. Vancatová , V G. Chalyan , and N. Meishvilli 2000. Longitudinal study of growth and body mass changes in ontogeny in captive rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) from the Institute of Medical Primatology, Sochi. Variability and Evolution 8: 51–81. Google Scholar

31.

C. P. Van Schaik 1989. The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In: Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals , V. Standen and R. A. Foley (eds.), pp. 195–218. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford. Google Scholar

32.

S. L. Washburn 1942. Skeletal proportions of adult langurs and macaques. Hum. Biol. 14: 444–472. Google Scholar

33.

A. Zihlman , D. Bolter , and C. Boesch 2004. Wild chimpanzee dentition and its implications for assessing life history in immature hominin fossils. P. Natl. Acad. Sci. 101(29): 10541–10543. Google Scholar
Cristina Domingo Balcells and Joaquím José Veà Baró "Developmental Stages in the Howler Monkey, Subspecies Alouatta Palliata Mexicana: A New Classification Using Age-Sex Categories," Neotropical Primates 16(1), 1-8, (1 June 2009). https://doi.org/10.1896/044.016.0101
Published: 1 June 2009
KEYWORDS
A. palliata
Age-sex categories
Categorías sexo-edad
Back to Top