Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2009 Anthropogenic Change and Primate Predation Risk: Crested Caracaras (Caracara plancus) Attempt Predation on Mantled Howler Monkeys (Alouatta palliata)
Tracie McKinney
Author Affiliations +

Introduction

Anthropogenic change in primate habitats may be an important factor in predation risk. Predation is often considered a principle selective pressure in primate evolution, and thus an important determining factor for grouping behavior, travel patterns, and choice of sleeping sites (van Schaik, 1983; Isbell, 1994; Treves, 2002; Shultz et al., 2004). Predation events are rarely witnessed in the wild due to their rarity, short duration, and the effects of observer presence. Reports of failed predation attempts or even the absence of predation events (Boinski et al., 2000) will broaden our understanding of this important issue. With our currently limited scope on primate predators and risk factors, we may be missing the impact of human habitat alteration on predation risk and rates. This paper reports a presumed predation attempt by two crested caracaras (Caracara plancus) on infant mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in Costa Rica. Crested caracaras are generally associated with human activity (Rodríguez-Estrella et al., 1998; Morrison & Humphrey, 2001), suggesting that habitat disturbance at this site may alter predation risks for these monkeys.

Large-bodied, arboreal howler monkeys should have fewer potential predators than many other primate genera (Di Fiore, 2002). Researchers have confirmed predation by jaguar (Peetz et al., 1992; Chinchilla, 1997), puma, Puma concolor (Chinchilla, 1997; Ludwig et al., 2007), ocelot, Leopardus pardalis (Miranda et al., 2005; Blanchi & Mendes, 2007) and harpy eagles, Harpia harpyja (Sherman, 1991) on wild howler monkeys. Predation attempts by black hawk-eagles, Spizaetus tyrannus (Miranda et al., 2006), crested eagles, Morphnus guianensis (Julliot, 1994), and tayras, Eira barbara (Phillips, 1995; Asensio & Gómez-Martín., 2002) are also reported in the literature. While incidences of snake predation on Ateline primates have not been documented, large snakes may also be considered a potential threat to juvenile howlers because they prey on many other Neotropical primates (Di Fiore, 2002). Birds are the most common predators for many monkey species, both in the Old and New Worlds. Only a few Neotropical birds, including the Harpy eagle (Harpia harpyja), the crested eagle (Morphnus guianensis), and the hawk eagle (Spizaetus spp.), are believed large enough to capture adult Atelines (Julliot, 1994; Miranda et al., 2006). Infant howler monkeys, however, are certainly reasonably sized prey for many Neotropical raptors (Di Fiore, 2002).

In the observed incident, an apparent predation attempt on infant howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) was made by two crested caracaras (Caracara plancus). The crested caracara ranges from the southern United States to Tierra del Fuego and the Faulkland Islands (Travaini et al., 2001; Henderson, 2002; Vargas et al., 2007). It is a medium-sized raptor in the falcon family (Falconidae), weighing approximately 1 kg and with a wingspan of 120–130 cm (Henderson, 2002). While they are mostly known as a scavenging species, caracaras are opportunistic hunters that feed on a variety of invertebrate and vertebrate prey (Travaini et al., 2001; Vargas et al., 2007), and may in fact consume more fresh meat than carrion (Richmond, 1976). Mammals account for nearly one quarter of crested caracara prey (Vargas et al., 2007). Although the majority of mammalian preys are rodents, caracaras are known to prey on larger animals, such as skunks and young rabbits (Henderson, 2002). While caracaras are not confirmed predators on even small monkeys, a taxidermized caracara did elicit alarm responses from captive marmosets (Callitrhix penicillata) in an experimental setting (Barros et al., 2002). Caracaras are particularly suited to act as active predators of monkeys in anthropogenic environments yet pose relatively little risk in more forested habitats. As open-habitat species (Morrison & Phillips, 2000), caracaras thrive in areas with little tree cover, such as natural clearings and manmade pastures. They are highly tolerant of human activity, and are often found hunting and scavenging on farms or along roadways (Rodríguez-Estrella et al., 1998; Morrison & Humphrey, 2001). Like many open-area raptors, caracaras seek out fragmented habitats, using the forest cover for nesting while hunting in the fields (Rodriguez-Estrella et al., 1998; Sánchez-Zapata & Calvo, 1999). Habitat changes that favor caracara activity may place local wildlife confined to narrow forest fragments at risk of heavy predation (Morrison & Phillips, 2000).

Study Population and Observation

The observed event took place at the Refugio Nacional de Vida Silvestre Curú, a 1,500 ha private “hacienda” and wildlife refuge in western Costa Rica (9°47′43″N, 84°55′15″W) (for a complete description of the site see Baker & Schutt, 2005). The incident was witnessed during data collection of a long-term project concerning the impact of human disturbance on mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata) and white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) ecology and social behavior. Curú is an ideal site for such studies, as it incorporates a variety of man-made and natural habitats including primary and advanced secondary forest, mangrove swamp, mango, banana, coconut, and African oil palm plantations, and open pastures with living fences. The refuge has a high density of howler monkeys, with some troops found in entirely anthropogenic habitats, others completely removed from human interaction, and many troops living in territories between these two extremes. The study troop of mantled howlers presented here ranges through riparian forest, living fences, mango and banana plantations, and secondary forest. The troop is fully habituated to human observers, and regularly encounters tourists, vehicles, and domestic animals. At the time of this observation, the troop consisted of 30 individuals—5 adult males, 16 adult females, and 9 immatures. Comparable troop sizes are common at Curú; the relevance of such large groups at this site is not yet clear. The events were recorded through ad libitum observation and continuous focal animal samples (Altmann, 1974) and were witnessed by two researchers and two volunteers.

The interaction with the caracaras took place at approximately 8:25 am on December 12, 2007. The monkeys were traveling and foraging through a strip of riparian forest between a cattle pasture and a dirt road. Two females with small infants crossed a gap in the canopy on an exposed horizontal branch 12–15 meters in height and approximately 5 meters long. Such exposed areas are risky for arboreal primates (Isbell, 1994), and these females were traveling quickly and in the presence of an adult male. Both infants were riding dorsally and appeared 4–6 weeks of age. The two raptors approached across the open pasture as the females were in the center of the branch, and swooped at the monkeys. The birds did not make contact or land on the branch. Both females ducked as the birds passed overhead, then hurried along the branch to cover. The entire interaction lasted less than 30 seconds.

There were few overt anti-predator behaviors from the females or other troop members. Anti-predator behaviors in howler monkeys typically include descent and dispersion, vocalization, and grouping quietly high in the trees (Eason, 1989; Julliot, 1994; Phillips, 1995; Miranda et al., 2006). In this incident, the nearest adult male howled for about 15 seconds shortly after the event, but a truck was passing at the same time, so it is unclear whether the vocalization was in response to the birds or to the motor. After the interaction with the caracaras, the troop continued to forage and settled down in the same area for their midday rest. A second observer recorded a single caracara flying through the trees at 12:36 pm. In this case, there were no interactions or responses from the monkeys. This is the only predation-related event observed for this monkey troop over a period of 491.25 hours of data collection. This represents a predation-related event rate of 0.002 events/hour. Even though the young howlers survived this interaction, caracaras should now be considered and monitored as a potential threat to infant monkey survival in edge habitats.

Discussion

Anthropogenic change means much more than habitat loss for non-human primates. The effects of human disturbance are pervasive, impacting parameters like foraging patterns, social interactions, and predation. Seidensticker (1983) proposed a consideration of predation when discussing disturbance, noting that primate predation by big cats depends largely on the availability of livestock. In the 25 years since, little has been made of human alteration and predation, and the few reports that have considered the idea are contradictory. Some authors suggest that predation rates decrease with close proximity to humans (Isbell & Young, 1993; Stanford, 2002), while others point out that certain types of predation may be favorable in anthropogenic habitats (chimpanzees: Grieser Johns & Grieser Johns, 1995; leopards: Ludwig et al., 2007). Domestic dogs are now reported in the literature as effective monkey predators (De Oliveira et al., 2008; Raguet-Schofield, 2008), and some wildlife may seek out anthropogenic habitats as well. Certainly human factors impact predation, but the nature of the change will be site specific. While we cannot account for every impact human disturbance places on a primate community, it is important to realize that wild primates face different challenges than they would in the absence of human encroachment. Predation rates may be impacted in a number of ways, namely: (1) predators may be adverse to human altered habitats, reducing predation risk; (2) anthropogenic habitats may encourage certain predators, increasing predation risk; or (3) the predator assemblage of a habitat changes, with potentially drastic effects on endemic wildlife that lack the experience and selection-driven behavioral patterns necessary to avoid these predators. As primate habitats become increasingly anthropogenic, their predation risks—and potential predators—are bound to change.

Acknowledgements

Many thanks are due to the Schutt-Valle family of Refugio de Vida Silvestre Curú, for permission to work on their land and for their continued friendship. Volunteers from Earthwatch Institute, Carolina Orozco Zamora, and Caspar Harris provided excellent field assistance. Dr. Scott McGraw offered helpful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. This research was funded by generous support from Earthwatch Institute, Conservation International, and The Ohio State University.

References

1.

J. Altmann 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–267. Google Scholar

2.

N. Asensio and F. Gómez-Martín 2002. Interspecific interaction and predator avoidance behavior in response to tayra (Eira Barbara) by mantled howler monkeys (Alouattapalliata). Primates 43(4): 339–341. Google Scholar

3.

M. Baker and A. Schutt 2005. Managing monkeys and mangos. In: Commensalism and Conflict: The HumanPrimate Interface , J. D. Paterson and J. Wallis (eds.), pp.444–463. American Society of Primatologists, Norman, Oklahoma. Google Scholar

4.

M. Barros , V. Boere, E. L. Mello Jr. , and C. Tomaz 2002. Reactions to potential predators in captive-born marmosets (Callithrix penicillata). Int. J. Primatol. 23(2): 443–454. Google Scholar

5.

R. C. Bianchi and S. L. Mendes 2007. Ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) predation on primates in Caratinga Biological Station, Southeast Brazil. Am. J. Primatol. 69: 1173–1178. Google Scholar

6.

S. Boinski , A. Treves and C. Chapman 2000. A critical evaluation of the influence of predators on primates: effects on group travel. In: On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups , S. Boinski and P. A. Garber (eds.), pp.43–72. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Google Scholar

7.

F. A. Chinchilla 1997. La dieta del jaguar (Panthera onca), el puma (Felis concolor) y el manigordo (Felis pardalis) (Carnivora: Felidae) en el Parque Nacional Corcovado, Costa Rica. Rev. Biol. Trop. 45(3): 1223–1229. Google Scholar

8.

V. B. De Oliveira , A. M. Linares , G. L. C. Corrêa and A. G. Chiarello 2008. Predation on the black capuchin monkey Cebus nigritus (Primates: Cebidae) by domestic dogs Canis lupus familiaris (Carnivora: Canidae), in the Parque Estadual Serrado Bigadeiro, Minas Gerais, Brazil. Rev. Bras. Zool. 25(2): 376–378. Google Scholar

9.

A. Di Fiore 2002. Predator sensitive foraging in Ateline primates. In: Eat or be Eaten: Predator Sensitive Foraging Among Primates , L. E. Miller (ed.), pp.242–267. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

10.

P. Eason 1989. Harpy eagle attempts predation on adult howler monkey. The Condor 91: 469–470. Google Scholar

11.

A. D. Grieser Johns and B.G. Grieser Johns 1995. Tropical forest primates and logging: long—term coexistence? Oryx 29: 205–211. Google Scholar

12.

C. L. Henderson 2002. Field Guide to the Wildlife of Costa Rica. University of Texas Press, Austin. Google Scholar

13.

I. A. Isbell 1994. Predation on primates: ecological patterns and evolutionary consequences. Evol. Anthrop. 3(2): 61–71. Google Scholar

14.

L. A. Isbell and T. P. Young 1993. Human presence reduces predation in a free-ranging vervet monkey population in Kenya. Anim. Behav. 45: 1233–1235. Google Scholar

15.

C. Julliot 1994. Predation of a young spider monkey (Ateles paniscus) by a crested eagle (Morphnus guianensis). Folia Primatol. 63: 75–77. Google Scholar

16.

G. Ludwig , L. M. Aguiar , J. M. D. Miranda , G. M. Tixeira , W. K. Svoboda , L. S. Malanski , M. M. Shiozawa , C. L. S. Hilst , I. T. Navarro , and F. C. Passos 2007. Cougar predation on black-and-gold howlers on Mutum Island, Southern Brazil. Int. J. Primatol. 28(1): 39–46. Google Scholar

17.

J. M. D. Miranda , I. P. Bernardi , K. C. Abreu , and F. C. Passos 2005. Predation on Aloautta guariba clamitans Cabrera (Primates, Atelidae) by Leopardus pardalis (Linnaeus) (Carnivora, Felidae). Rev. Bras. Zool. 22(3): 793–795. Google Scholar

18.

J. M. D. Miranda , I. P. Bernardi , R. F. Moro-Rios and F. C. Passos 2006. Antipredator behavior of brown howlers attacked by black hawk-eagle in Southern Brazil. Int. J. Primatol. 27(4): 1097–1101. Google Scholar

19.

J. L. Morrison and S. R. Humphrey 2001. Conservation value of private lands for crested caracaras in Florida. Conserv. Biol. 15(3): 675–684. Google Scholar

20.

J. L. Morrison and L. M. Phillips 2000. Nesting habitat and success of the Chimango caracara in southern Chile. The Wilson Bulletin 112(2): 225–232. Google Scholar

21.

A. Peetz , M. A. Norconk and W. G. Kinzey 1992. Predation by jaguar on howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus) in Venezuela. Am. J. Primatol. 28: 223–228. Google Scholar

22.

K. Phillips 1995. Differing responses to a predator (Eira Barbara) by Alouatta and Cebus. Neotrop. Primates 3(2): 45–46. Google Scholar

23.

M. Raguet-Schofield 2008. The effects of human encroachment and seasonality on the risk of mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata) predation by dogs on Ometepe Island, Nicaragua. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. Sup 46: 176. Google Scholar

24.

A. R. Richmond 1976. Feeding of nestlings by the caracara in Costa Rica. Wilson Bull. 88(4): 667. Google Scholar

25.

R. Rodríguez-Estrella , J. A. Donázar and F. Hiraldo 1998. Raptors as indicators of environmental change in the scrub habitat of Baja California Sur, Mexico. Conserv. Biol. 12(4): 921–925. Google Scholar

26.

J. A. Sánchez-Zapata and J. F. Calvo 1999. Raptor distribution in relation to landscape composition in semi-arid Mediterranean habitats. J. Appl. Ecol. 36: 254–262. Google Scholar

27.

J. Seidensticker 1983. Predation by Panthera cats and measures of human influence in habitats of south Asian monkeys. Int. J. Primatol. 4(3): 323–326. Google Scholar

28.

P. T. Sherman 1991. Harpy eagle predation on a red howler monkey. Folia Primatol. 56(1): 53–56. Google Scholar

29.

S. Shultz , R. Noë , W. S. McGraw and R. I. M. Dunbar 2004. A community-level evaluation of the impact of prey behavioural and ecological characteristics on predator diet composition. Proc. R. Soc. B. 271: 725–732. Google Scholar

30.

C. B. Stanford 2002. Avoiding predators: expectations and evidence in primate antipredator behavior. Int. J. Primatol. 23(4): 741–757. Google Scholar

31.

A. Travaini , J. A. Donázar , O. Ceballos and F. Hiraldo 2001. Food habits of the crested caracara (Caracara plancus) in the Andean Patagonia: the role of breeding constraints. J. Arid Environ. 48: 211–219. Google Scholar

32.

A. Treves 2002. Predicting predation risk for foraging, arboreal primates. In: Eat or be Eaten: Predator Sensitive Foraging Among Primates. L. E. Miller (ed.), pp.222–241. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

33.

C. P. Van Schaick 1983. Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87: 120–144. Google Scholar

34.

R. J. Vargas , M. S. Bo and M. Favero 2007. Diet of the southern caracara (Caracara plancus) in Mar Chiquita Reserve, Southern Argentina. J. Raptor Res. 41(2): 113–1. Google Scholar
Tracie McKinney "Anthropogenic Change and Primate Predation Risk: Crested Caracaras (Caracara plancus) Attempt Predation on Mantled Howler Monkeys (Alouatta palliata)," Neotropical Primates 16(1), 24-27, (1 June 2009). https://doi.org/10.1896/044.016.0105
Published: 1 June 2009
Back to Top