Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2009 Early Behavioral Development of a Free-Ranging Howler Monkey Infant (Alouatta Guariba Clamitans) in Southern Brazil
Luciana Regina Podgaiski, Mércia Maria de Assis Jardim
Author Affiliations +

Introduction

In comparison with other mammals, the offspring of primates undergo a relatively long period of behavioral development and dependency upon their mothers. During this period, the mother is a secure base from which the infant is able to explore the environment and engage in social behavior (Vochteloo et al., 1993), acquiring the ecological skills of the species and the social traditions of the family troop (Southwick and Siddiqi, 1974). Independence is the result of a long period of increasing self-sufficiency in activities such as locomotion and feeding, as well as growing sociability and distance from the mother (Altmann, 1980; Odalia-Rímoli, 1992). Mother-infant ties constitute one of the most intense types of social relationship in primates (Harlow and Zimmermann, 1958, Altmann, 1959), and have a positive effect on infant survival and development.

Howler monkeys (genus Alouatta Lacépède, 1799) are the most widely-distributed Neotropical primates (Chapman and Balcomb, 1998), and are also the best studied in the wild. They occupy a variety of habitat types, and are well-adapted to anthropogenic fragmentation (Crocket, 1998), although some species are declining rapidly in the wild and have been classified as threatened (IUCN, 2009). Howlers have been translocated and re-introduced at a number of sites (e.g. Agoramoorthy, 1995), and a critical factor for such management procedures is the successful handling of females with nursing infants (Baker, 2002). Knowledge of the behavioral development of infants can obviously contribute to the efficiency of such procedures (International Primatological Society, 2007).

Studies of the behavioral development of primate infants have focused on their social behavior and spatial relationships with their mothers. In howlers, studies have been conducted on free-ranging Alouatta guariba (Kats and Otta, 1991), Alouatta palliata (Altmann, 1959; Clarke, 1990; Lyall, 1996), and Alouatta seniculus (Mack, 1979; Cabrera, 1997). Allomaternal care is well documented in female howlers (Calegaro-Marques and Bicca-Marques, 1993), but is rare in males (Marques and Adis, 2000). In this study, the early behavioral development of a male infant A. guariba clamitans was monitored in a free-ranging group in southern Brazil.

Materials and methods

Study area and subjects

The study was undertaken in Itapuã State Park (30°23′S, 51°30′W), in the city of Viamão, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil, from August 2003 to April 2004. The climate in the region is temperate, with hot summers and no clear dry season (Cfa type of Köppen's classification; Peel et al., 2007). The home range of the study group was estimated to be approximately 8.71 ha of semi-deciduous forest on a granite hillside bordering a sandy beach, known as Pedreira (Marques, 2001; Jardim, 2005). In August 2003, the study group was composed of two adult males, one sub-adult male, one juvenile male, two adult females, and one male infant (the study subject—see below). Although the exact date of this infant's birth could not be determined, we estimated that it was between two and three months of age, based primarily on body size (Carpenter, 1934; Altmann, 1959; Kowalewski and Zunino, 2004) and the ontogeny of independent behaviors. In March 2004, another male infant was born in the troop, an adult female immigrated, and an adult male disappeared.

Observation methods

From August, 2003 to April, 2004, the behavior of the infant male subject was monitored for two days each month, except October (one day). Data were collected using focalanimal sampling with continuous recording (Altmann, 1974) from sunrise to sunset (around 9 hours of observation per day). Samples of three minutes duration were collected at ten-minute intervals, with a total of 810 samples collected over 155 hours of monitoring. During each sample, the infant's behavior and its position in relation to its mother were recorded according to the categories defined in Table 1. Other behaviors, such as drinking and rejection by the mother, were recorded in ad libitum fashion (Altmann, 1974). Nursing was not recorded here because of the difficulty of determining this behavior reliably.

Results

Infant-mother relationships

In the first month of observations, when the infant was two or three months old, he spent 86.8% of the day on the mother, mostly in the ventral position (Fig. 1), although he was carried on the dorsum during troop movements. In subsequent months, the infant spent increasingly less time on the mother. Ventral carrying during troop movements ceased in November (5–6 months), and dorsal carrying in December (6–7 months), although ventral and dorsal contact with the mother were recorded up to March and April, respectively. By the end of the study period, the infant was in contact with its mother less than 30 percent of the time.

Resting (Fig. 2a)

Initially, the infant spent most of its time at rest, and almost always rested while being carried by the mother until 4–5 months old. The infant was observed resting at a distance from its mother for the first time in November, but this only became common by the end of the study. By the age of 8–9 months, time spent resting stabilized at 55–60% of observation time.

Locomotion (Fig. 2b)

The earliest bouts of independent locomotion occurred in September (3–4 months old). This activity increased progressively until 6–7 months, but was subsequently irregular. Independent movement of the infant was monitored and stimulated by the mother, by moving very slowly through the trees and waiting for the infant to follow. Sometimes, the mother would move to a branch and emit a vocalization until the infant arrived. When crossing a wide gap in the trees, the mother used her own tail or body as a bridge or carried the infant on her dorsum.

Figure 1.

Mean percentage observation time spent by the A. guariba infant in different contact categories (see Table 1) in each month during the study period.

f01_27.eps

Figure 2.

Percentage of time spent by the howler infant in different behaviors and in different forms of contact with its mother during the study period. See Table 1 for behavior and contact categories.

f02_27.eps

Feeding (Fig. 2c)

The infant also started eating solid food at 3–4 months. Initially, the infant was usually either being carried by its mother or close to her during this behavior, which allowed it to observe the items ingested by the mother and then repeat her movements. Contact declined rapidly by the following month. The howlers usually drank water from bromeliads, and this behavior was first noticed in the infant at 8–9 months, the same period when the mother began rejecting the infant when it tried to nurse.

Grooming (Fig. 2d)

The infant was observed grooming on two occasions. On the first, he was 4–5 months old, and mimicked his mother when grooming an adult female. On the second occasion, at 6–7 months, the infant groomed its mother.

Play (Fig. 2e–f)

The infant already exhibited play behavior when observations began. The highest frequency occurred at 5–6 months, reaching around 50% of the daily observation period. After this peak, the behavior tended to decline towards the end of the study. Play almost always took place when the troop was at rest. During individual play, the infant explored its immediate environment. Social play initially involved the infant's mother, and gradually involved a juvenile male. Play was also recorded with the dominant adult male.

Discussion

While the infant grew progressively independent, it maintained relatively close ties with its mother, invariably resting together, even at the end of the study. Overall, the behavioral development of the infant was similar to that recorded in previous studies of howlers (Altmann, 1959; Mack, 1979; Clarke, 1990; Kats and Otta, 1991; Lyall, 1996; Cabrera, 1997). The transition from exclusive ventral carrying in the first few weeks of life to a predominance of the dorsal position is a typical pattern in monkeys, including howlers (Altmann, 1959; Mack, 1979; Shoemaker, 1979; Kats and Otta, 1991; Lyall, 1996; Miranda et al., 2005). Increasing distance from the mother is also typical. Kats and Otta (1991) confirmed a progressive increase in distance from the mother at 3–4 month of age in A. guariba, while Miranda et al., (2005) found that contact with the mother declined to 10% of activity time by the fifth month of age. In the present study, the infant was more than 1 m from its mother for 6% of the time at 2–3 months old, increasing to 70% only at 1 year. A possible regression in this trend was recorded at 7–8 months, however, which may represent a critical period of insecurity or a regression in suckling (Horwich, 1989).

Table 1.

Howler monkey infant behavioral categories based on proximity to the mother and activity.

t01_27.gif

According to Mack (1979), prior to six months of age, howler infants may bite and chew the same type of object that the adults are eating, but not necessarily consume it. Maybe, the high feeding percentages observed in the first months of this study refer to an exploratory feeding which occurred in contact or near the mother. The stabilization of rest at 55–60% by 7–8 months corresponds to the typical rate of adults in this study group (Marques, 2001). This stabilization occurred at the same time as the frequency of play behavior began to decline. Both play and grooming have an important social function (Southwick and Siddiqi, 1974). One possible factor determining rates of social play in the present study was the availability of potential partners, i.e. other immature individuals. Grooming appears to be relatively common in A. guariba in comparison with other howlers (Kinzey, 1997); this behavior was part of the behavioral development of the studied infan While behavior patterns are best evaluated on the basis of a sample of different individuals, the present study provides some useful preliminary insights on the behavioral development of infant howlers of the species Alouatta guariba. The patterns observed appeared to be typical of the genus.

Acknowledgments

We thank Ana Alice B. de Marques for initial advice on the project; Paula C. Colombo for technical support; Aline F. Quadros, André F. B. Lima, and Verônica G. Sydow for comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.

References

1.

G. Agoramoorthy 1995. Red howling monkey (Alouatta seniculus) reintroduction in a gallery forest of Hato Flores Moradas, Venezuela. Neotrop. Primates 3: 9–10. Google Scholar

2.

S.A. Altmann 1959. Field observation on howling monkey society. J. Mammal. 40: 317–330. Google Scholar

3.

J. Altmann 1974. Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–267. Google Scholar

4.

J. Altmann 1980. Baboon mothers and infants. Harvard University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

5.

L. R. Baker 2002. Guidelines for nonhumam primate reintroductions. Reintroduction News 21: 29–57. Google Scholar

6.

J. A. Cabrera 1997. Cambios en la actividad de juegos en infantes y jóvenes de mono aullador (Alouatta seniculus). Neotrop. Primates 5: 108–111. Google Scholar

7.

C. Calegaro-Marques and J. C. Bicca-Marques 1993. Allomaternal care in the black howler monkey (Alouatta caraya). Folia Primatol. 61: 104–109. Google Scholar

8.

C. R. Carpenter 1934. A field study of the behaviour and social relations of howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Com. Psychol. Monogr. 10: 1–168. Google Scholar

9.

C. A. Chapman and S. R. Balcomb 1998. Population characteristics of howlers: ecological conditions or group history. Int. J. Primatol. 19: 385–403. Google Scholar

10.

M. R. Clarke 1990. Behavioral development and socialization of infants in a free-ranging group of howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata). Folia Primatol. 54: 1–15. Google Scholar

11.

H. F. Harlow and R. R. Zimmermann 1958. The development of affectional responses in infant monkeys. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc. 102: 501–509. Google Scholar

12.

R. H. Horwich 1989. Cyclic development of contact behaviour in apes and humans. Primates 30: 269–279. Google Scholar

13.

International Primatological Society. 2007. IPS International guidelines for acquisition, care and breeding of nonhuman primates. Website:  http://www.internationalprimatologicalsociety.org/docs/IPS International Guidelines for the Acquisition Care and Breeding of Nonhuman Primates Second Edition 2007. pdf. Accessed 18 August 2008. Google Scholar

14.

IUCN. 2009. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2009.1. Website:  www.iucnredlist.org Accesed 18 June 2009. Google Scholar

15.

M. M. A. Jardim 2005. Ecologia populacional de bugios-ruivos (Alouatta guariba) nos municípios de Porto Alegre e Viamão, RS, Brasil. Doctoral thesis, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, SP. Google Scholar

16.

B. Kats and E. Otta 1991. Comportamento lúdico do bugio (Alouatta fusca clamitans, Cabrera, 1940) (Primates: Cebidae: Alouattinae). Biotemas 4: 61–82. Google Scholar

17.

W. G. Kinzey 1997. Alouatta. In: New world primates: ecology, evolution and behaviour , W. G. Kinzey (ed.), pp. 174–185. Aldine de Gruyter, New York. Google Scholar

18.

M. Kowalewski and G. E. Zunino 2004. Birth seasonality in Alouatta caraya in Northern Argentina. Int. J. Primatol. 25: 383–400. Google Scholar

19.

Z. S. Lyall 1996. The early development of behavior and independence in howler monkeys Alouatta palliata mexicana. Neotrop. Primates 4: 4–8. Google Scholar

20.

D. Mack 1979. Growth and development of infant red howling monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in a free-ranging population. In: Vertebrate Ecology in the Northern Neotropics , J. F. Eisenberg (ed.), pp. 127–136. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington. Google Scholar

21.

A. A. B. Marques 2001. Estratégias de uso do espaço por Alouatta guariba clamitans Cabrera, 1940 em habitats temperado e subtropical no sul do Brasil. Doctoral thesis, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, MG. Google Scholar

22.

A. A. B. Marques and C. Adis 2000. Male care in a group of wild Alouatta fusca clamitans in Southern Brazil. Folia Primatol. 71: 409–412. Google Scholar

23.

R. Menegat , F. V. Mohr , C. C. Carraro and R. Flores 1998. Porto Alegre em dados. In: Atlas Ambiental de Porto Alegre , R. Menegat (ed.), pp. 203–205. Editora da Universidade, Porto Alegre. Google Scholar

24.

J. M. D. Miranda , L. M. Aguiar , G. Ludwig , R. F. Moro-Rios and F. C. Passos 2005. The first seven months of an infant of Alouatta guariba (Humboldt) (Primates, Atelidae): interactions and the development of behavioral patterns. Rev. Bras. Zool. 22: 1191–1195. Google Scholar

25.

A. Odalia-Rímoli 1992. O filhote muriqui (Brachyteles arachnoides): um estudo do desenvolvimento da independência. Master dissertation. Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, SP. Google Scholar

26.

A. H. Shoemaker 1979. Reproduction and development of the Black howler monkey Alouatta caraya at Columbia Zoo. Int. Zoo Yearb. 19: 150–155. Google Scholar

27.

C. H. Southwick and M. F. Siddiqi 1974. Contrasts in primate social behaviour. Bioscience 24: 398–406. Google Scholar

28.

J. D. Vochteloo , P. J. A. Timmermans , J. A. H. Duijghuisen and M. H. Vossen 1993. Effects of reducing the mother's radius of action on the development of motherinfant relationships in longtailed macaques. Anim. Behav. 45: 603–612. Google Scholar
Luciana Regina Podgaiski and Mércia Maria de Assis Jardim "Early Behavioral Development of a Free-Ranging Howler Monkey Infant (Alouatta Guariba Clamitans) in Southern Brazil," Neotropical Primates 16(1), 27-31, (1 June 2009). https://doi.org/10.1896/044.016.0106
Published: 1 June 2009
Back to Top