Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 January 2011 Paternal Investment and the “Sexually Selected Hypothesis” for the Evolution of Eggshell Coloration: Revisiting the Assumptions
Christina Riehl
Author Affiliations +

THE ADAPTIVE SIGNIFICANCE of bright blue eggs has interested evolutionary biologists for at least a century (Wallace 1889, Underwood and Sealy 2002). Several factors have been proposed as explanations for the evolution of conspicuous eggshell colors, including thermoregulation (Montevecchi 1976, Wink et al. 1985), camouflage (Lack 1958, Oniki 1979), increased visibility to cavity-nesting parents (von Haartman 1957), avoidance of brood parasitism (Victoria 1972), protection against solar radiation (Lahti 2008), and, most recently, sexual conflict within the breeding pair (Hanley et al. 2010). However, the explanation that has received the most attention in the past decade is the “sexually selected hypothesis” of Moreno and Osorno (2003), which was developed to explain the occurrence of blue-green eggs in bird species with biparental care. This hypothesis posits that biliverdin, the antioxidative pigment responsible for the blue-green color of the eggshell, is costly for females to produce and may therefore serve as a reliable indicator of female quality. If so, males should be able to judge the genetic fitness of their mates by the intensity of the coloration of the eggshell, and then adjust their investment in the clutch accordingly. Since the publication of Moreno and Osorno's paper, several experimental studies have attempted to test whether the intensity of eggshell color does, in fact, predict male parental investment.

The majority of these studies (reviewed below) have used a cross-fostering approach to decouple the effects of parental quality and eggshell color, swapping entire clutches of eggs between pairs of nests of approximately the same age and clutch size. Male parental investment is then measured in terms of the frequency of food deliveries to the nestlings, typically several days after hatching. Here, I show that this is an inaccurate measure of paternal investment: it controls for correlations between eggshell color and maternal fitness, but it does not control for correlations between eggshell color and nestling fitness. I discuss several variables that can confound the results of such cross-fostering experiments and recommend ways to improve future studies. I also raise concerns regarding fundamental assumptions of the sexually selected hypothesis, particularly the relationship between paternal care and offspring quality and the heritability of small differences in eggshell color.

The sexually selected hypothesis relies on two assumptions. The first is that the intensity of the blue-green eggshell color is a costly and reliable indicator of maternal quality. This has largely been supported through a series of correlations: although only one study has shown a direct relationship between biliverdin and blue-green eggshell chroma (Moreno et al. 2006a), the intensity of shell coloration has been positively correlated with many maternal traits, including age, body mass, body condition, provisioning rate, and antioxidative capacity (reviewed in Reynolds et al. 2009; but see Cassey et al. 2008, Hanley and Doucet 2009). The second assumption is that high-quality females produce high-quality eggs, because they are able to allocate more costly resources to them. This, too, is fairly well supported: the intensity of eggshell coloration has been shown to be positively correlated with egg volume (Moreno et al. 2004), yolk antibody concentrations (Morales et al. 2006), yolk luteins (Hargitai et al. 2008), and yolk testosterone (López-Rull et al. 2008). These traits, particularly the last, have in turn been linked to several nestling traits that reflect quality and condition, including gape color (Saino et al. 2003), body mass (Schwabl 1996, Quillfeldt et al. 2006), growth rates (Navara et al. 2005), and begging intensity (Eising and Groothuis 2003, Goodship and Buchanan 2006).

Eggshell color, therefore, appears to be correlated with a suite of phenotypic traits that may all be reliable indicators of nestling fitness. If the sexually selected hypothesis is tested by measuring paternal food delivery to nestlings, it is impossible to tell which of these traits—alone or in concert—are actually being used to assess offspring quality. In the Common Canary (Serinus canaria), for example, rates of food delivery depend on the interaction of several factors, including nestling size and position (Kilner 1995), gape color (Kilner 1997), and begging posture (Kilner 2002). Exchanging eggs between sets of foster parents controls for the correlation between eggshell color and maternal fitness, but it leaves open the possibility that nestling quality itself may confound the relationship between eggshell color and paternal investment.

TABLE 1.

A review of studies that have tested the “sexually selected hypothesis” of Moreno and Osorno (2003) by quantifying male parental investment in relation to eggshell coloration.

t01_175.gif

A review of the literature reveals that the majority of studies have not controlled for the confounding correlations between eggshell coloration and other nestling traits, typically because paternal care was measured during the nestling period (Table 1). When these are excluded, only two studies have found a significant relationship between eggshell coloration and paternal investment (Moreno et al. 2008, Soler et al. 2008). These studies provide convincing evidence for the link between eggshell coloration and paternal investment, but it should be acknowledged that this type of experimental support is even more limited than it appears at first glance.

In order to ensure that changes in male provisioning rates are due to eggshell color rather than to confounding nestling traits, future studies should employ one of the many experimental designs that control for such correlations. One option is to measure paternal effort during the incubation period, in terms of nest defense, provisioning to the female, or incubation patterns (e.g., Morales et al. 2010b). For species in which fathers provide little care prior to hatching, nestlings in experimental broods can be re-swapped after hatching to break the correlation between eggshell color and nestling condition (e.g., Soler et al. 2008). Alternatively, Moreno et al. (2008) simply substituted one host egg with an artificial egg (either bright or pale) and subsequently measured food delivery to the host nestlings.

Other assumptions of the sexually selected hypothesis need to be reexamined with equal rigor. Several concerns have already been raised by Kilner (2006), Reynolds et al. (2009) and Cherry and Gosler (2010); to their lists I add two more. First, all of the above-mentioned studies have relied upon the prediction that male parental investment should increase with perceived offspring quality. However, several studies have shown that parents do not always maximize their reproductive output by investing more in high-quality offspring, largely because resource availability and the costs of provisioning influence investment decisions (Clutton-Brock 1991). When food is not limited, parents may deliver food more frequently to low-quality nestlings, presumably in an attempt to compensate for poor condition and increase their chances of fledging (Stamps et al. 1985, Davis et al. 1999, Leonard and Horn 2001). For example, Christe et al. (1996) found that male Great Tits (Parus major) made >50% more food deliveries to nests that contained low-mass offspring infested with parasitic fleas than to nests that were free of ectoparasites. In Pied Flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca)—one of the species commonly used to test the sexually selected hypothesis—Gottlander (1987) found that females were more likely to deliver food to smaller, lower-mass nestlings than to their heavier siblings, and males delivered food to all nestlings equally.

Only under certain circumstances should parents benefit by allocating more effort to high-quality offspring: when resources are limited, such that provisioning is costly, or when low-quality nestlings are in such poor condition that increased parental investment is unlikely to help (Lack 1968, Davis et al. 1999). For example, Bize et al. (2006) showed that Alpine Swifts (Apus melba) and European Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) preferentially fed smaller offspring early in the nesting season (when food was abundant), but preferentially fed larger offspring in later nesting attempts (when food was limited). Similarly, Ligon and Hill (2010) found that Eastern Bluebirds (Sialia sialis) fed high-quality and low-quality siblings at equal rates under natural conditions, but favored high-quality offspring when the differences between siblings were artificially exaggerated. Parents might also invest more resources in high-quality offspring if the marginal benefits to doing so are exceptionally high; for example, if a slight increase in parental effort brings about a disproportionately large increase in the nestling's prospects of survival and future reproductive value (Lessells 2002). Mock et al. (2005) found that male House Sparrows (Passer domesticus) delivered food more frequently to broods that were artificially provisioned, and their mathematical model (parameterized with data from the same population) supported the hypothesis that males should benefit more by increasing investment in the nestlings than by pursuing other activities (such as extrapair copulations).

Taken together, these studies indicate that a male may either increase or decrease his investment if he perceives an increase in the quality of his nestlings. The direction of the change depends on the costs to the male (in terms of provisioning effort and his future reproductive potential) and the benefits to his offspring (in terms of fitness gains and future reproductive value). These parameters, in turn, are likely to depend on the particular life-history strategy of the study species, along with environmental variables in the study population and even individual characteristics of the fathers themselves (such as age and body condition). Most studies of the sexually selected hypothesis have focused on a small number of temperate, cavity-nesting passerines, relatively short-lived species in which fathers may not benefit from altering their levels of investment in response to small differences in offspring quality. As Cherry and Gosier (2010) noted, the sexually selected hypothesis is more likely to be applicable to long-lived species with extensive paternal care and pair bonds that persist across years, such as seabirds. Future researchers should be careful to ensure that—all other considerations aside—the ecology of their study species is such that fathers are actually predicted to benefit from increasing their investment in a high-quality clutch.

Second, there is a great deal of confusion in the literature as to whether shell color reflects a female's intrinsic genetic quality or her body condition at the time of laying. As Moreno and Osorno (2003) clearly stated in their original formulation of the hypothesis, eggshell color cannot be subject to sexual selection unless it is heritable. It should be repeatable within and across an individual female's clutches, and variance in eggshell coloration should be at least partly attributable to genetic, rather than environmental, variance. Evidence from poultry breeders suggests that eggshell ground color is indeed partly under polygenic control (Punnett 1933, Hutt 1949). However, these early studies quantified broad differences in pigmentation (such as blue vs. white) rather than the fine-scale differences in pigment intensity that are posited to be heritable by the sexually selected hypothesis. Only two studies have examined the repeatability of blue-green eggshell color for females in wild populations, and they yielded mixed results. Collias (1993) found that female Village Weavers (Ploceus cucullatus) laid eggs that were repeatable in basic ground color, but not in hue, lightness, or chromaticity. In Pied Flycatchers, by contrast, Morales et al. (2010a) found that four of nine egg color traits were significantly heritable, with heritability accounting for 15–54% of total phenotypic variance.

In seeking support for the sexually selected hypothesis, most researchers have focused on the value of eggshell color as a signal of female body condition, frequently relying on environmental variables and intra-female comparisons to support the relationship. These studies are consistently cited as evidence for the link between female quality and eggshell color, although they essentially confirm the environmental rather than genetic component of its variance. For example, Soler et al. (2008) found that female Spotless Starlings (Sturnus unicolor) laid paler eggs when their wing feathers were experimentally clipped, and Moreno et al. (2006a) found that female Pied Flycatchers laid brighter eggs when they were supplemented with additional food. Other authors have found that eggshell color is significantly affected by female age (Siefferman et al. 2006), pollution (Jagannath et al. 2008), rainfall and temperature (Avilés et al. 2007), carotenoid availability (Morales et al. 2011), and the egg's position in the laying order of the clutch (Krist and Grim 2007). These studies support the idea that biliverdin is limited and that shell color is an honest indicator of maternal condition, but they do not support the hypothesis that eggshell coloration is a heritable trait that has evolved through sexual selection.

In summary, several lines of evidence suggest that blue-green eggshell color reliably reflects maternal body condition and reliably predicts nestling body condition. However, few studies have convincingly demonstrated that male birds use eggshell color to assess the genetic quality of their mates. Cross-fostering experiments typically have not controlled for nestling condition, and the sole prediction that these experiments tested—that males invest more care in high-quality offspring—may be applicable only under particular circumstances. Finally, most authors have demonstrated the influence of environmental rather than genetic factors on eggshell coloration.

Future research will benefit from a broader and more rigorous approach to the adaptive value of eggshell coloration, particularly from studies that attempt to simultaneously evaluate multiple competing hypotheses. In order for researchers to correctly test the predictions of the sexually selected hypothesis, they must ensure that the life-history traits of their study species support its assumptions. The most productive studies are likely to come from populations of long-lived, monogamous birds in which a strong relationship between paternal investment and nestling quality has already been demonstrated. Much more information is needed on the extent to which genetic factors control intra-population variation in eggshell coloration, and on the degree to which male birds are able to perceive and judge this variation. Parental investment is likely to be influenced by a combination of several variables, of which nestling quality is only one. Nestling quality, in turn, is likely to be assessed through a combination of several traits, of which eggshell color is only one.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I am grateful to J. S. Adelman, D. C. Dearborn, J. Q. Ouyang, and two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful and substantive comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.

LITERATURE CITED

1.

J. M. Avilés , B. G. Stokke , A. Moksnes , E. Røskaft , and A. P. Møller . 2007. Environmental conditions influence egg color of Reed Warblers Acrocephalus scirpaceus and their parasite, the Common Cuckoo Cuculus canorus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61:475–485. Google Scholar

2.

P. Bize , R. Piault , B. Moureau , and P. Heeb . 2006. A UV signal of offspring condition mediates context-dependent parental favouritism. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 273:2063–2068. Google Scholar

3.

P. Cassey , J. G. Ewen , T. M. Blackburn , M. E. Hauber , M. Vorobyev , and N. J. Marshall . 2008. Eggshell colour does not predict measures of maternal investment in eggs of Turdus thrushes. Naturwissenschaften 95:713–721. Google Scholar

4.

M. I. Cherry , and A. G. Gosler . 2010. Avian eggshell coloration: New perspectives on adaptive explanations. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 100:753–762. Google Scholar

5.

P. Christe , H. Richner , and A. Oppliger . 1996. Begging, food provisioning, and nestling competition in Great Tit broods infested with ectoparasites. Behavioral Ecology 7:127–131. Google Scholar

6.

T. H. Clutton-Brock 1991. Parental care and offspring quality. Pages 164–165 in The Evolution of Parental Care ( T. H. CluttonBrock , Ed.). Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey. Google Scholar

7.

E. C. Collias 1993. Inheritance of egg-color polymorphism in the Village Weaver (Ploceus cucullatus). Auk 110:683–692. Google Scholar

8.

J. N. Davis , P. M. Todd , and S. Bullock . 1999. Environment quality predicts parental provisioning decisions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 266:1791–1797. Google Scholar

9.

C. M. Eising , and T. G. G. Groothuis . 2003. Yolk androgens and begging behaviour in Black-headed Gull chicks: An experimental field study. Animal Behaviour 66:1027–1034. Google Scholar

10.

N. M. Goodship , and K. L. Buchanan . 2006. Nestling testosterone is associated with begging behaviour and fledging success in the Pied Flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 273:71–76. Google Scholar

11.

K. Gottlander 1987. Parental feeding behaviour and sibling competition in the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca. Ornis Scandinavica 18:269–276. Google Scholar

12.

D. Hanley , and S. M. Doucet . 2009. Egg coloration in Ring-billed Gulls (Larus delawarensis): A test of the sexual signaling hypothesis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63:719–729. Google Scholar

13.

D. Hanley , S. M. Doucet , and D. C. Dearborn . 2010. A blackmail hypothesis for the evolution of conspicuous egg coloration in birds. Auk 127:453–459. Google Scholar

14.

D. Hanley , G. Heiber , and D. C. Dearborn . 2008. Testing an assumption of the sexual-signaling hypothesis: Does blue-green egg color reflect maternal antioxidant capacity? Condor 110:767–771. Google Scholar

15.

R. Hargitai , M. Herényi , and J. Török . 2008. Eggshell coloration in relation to male ornamentation, female condition, and egg quality in the Collared Flycatcher Ficedula albicollis. Journal of Avian Biology 39:413–422. Google Scholar

16.

F. B. Hutt 1949. Genetics of the Fowl. McGraw-Hill, New York. Google Scholar

17.

A. Jagannath , R. F. Shore , L. A. Walker , P. N. Ferns , and A. G. Gosler . 2008. Eggshell pigmentation indicates pesticide contamination. Journal of Applied Ecology 45:133–140. Google Scholar

18.

R. Kilner 1995. When do canary parents respond to nestling signals of need? Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 260:343–348. Google Scholar

19.

R. Kilner 1997. Mouth color is a reliable signal of need in begging canary nestlings. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 264:963–968. Google Scholar

20.

R. M. Kilner 2002. Sex differences in canary (Serinus canaria) provisioning rules. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 52: 400–407. Google Scholar

21.

R. M. Kilner 2006. The evolution of egg colour and patterning in birds. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 81:383–406. Google Scholar

22.

M. Krist , and T. Grim . 2007. Are blue eggs a sexually selected signal of female Collared Flycatchers? A cross-fostering experiment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61:863–876. Google Scholar

23.

D. Lack 1958. The significance of the color of turdine eggs. Ibis 100:145–166. Google Scholar

24.

D. Lack 1968. Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. Methuen, London. Google Scholar

25.

D. C. Lahti 2008. Population differentiation and rapid evolution of egg color in accordance with solar radiation. Auk 125:796–802. Google Scholar

26.

M. L. Leonard , and A. G. Horn . 2001. Begging calls and parental feeding decisions in Tree Swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 49:170–175. Google Scholar

27.

C. M. Lessells 2002. Parentally biased favoritism: Why should parents specialize in caring for different offspring? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B: 357:381–403. Google Scholar

28.

R. A. Ligon , and G. E. Hill . 2010. Feeding decisions of Eastern Bluebirds are situationally influenced by fledgling plumage color. Behavioral Ecology 21:456–464. Google Scholar

29.

I. López-Rull , I. Miksik , and D. Gil . 2008. Egg pigmentation reflects female and egg quality in the Spotless Starling Sturnus unicolor. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62:1877–1884. Google Scholar

30.

D. W. Mock , P. L. Schwagmeyer , and G. A. Parker . 2005. Male House Sparrows deliver more food to experimentally subsidized offspring. Animal Behaviour 70:225–236. Google Scholar

31.

W. A. Montevecchi 1976. Field experiments on the adaptive significance of avian eggshell pigmentation. Behaviour 58:26–39. Google Scholar

32.

J. Morales , S.-Y Kim , E. Lobato , S. Merino , G. Toma , J. Martínez-De La Puente , and J. Moreno . 2010a. On the heritability of blue-green eggshell coloration. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23:1783–1791. Google Scholar

33.

J. Morales , J. J. Sanz , and J. Moreno . 2006. Egg colour reflects the amount of yolk maternal antibodies and fledging success in a songbird. Biology Letters 2:334–336. Google Scholar

34.

J. Morales , R. Torres , and A. Velando . 2010b. Parental conflict and blue egg coloration in a seabird. Naturwissenschaften 97:173–180. Google Scholar

35.

J. Morales , A. Velando , and R. Torres . 2011. Biliverdin-based egg coloration is enhanced by carotenoid supplementation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65:197–203. Google Scholar

36.

J. Moreno , E. Lobato , S. Merino , and J. Martínez-De La Puente . 2008. Blue-green eggs in Pied Flycatchers: An experimental demonstration that a supernormal stimulus elicits improved nestling condition. Ethology 114:1078–1083. Google Scholar

37.

J. Moreno , E. Lobato , J. Morales , S. Merino , G. Tomás , J. Martínez-De La Puente , J. J. Sanz , R. Mateo , and J. J. Soler . 2006a. Experimental evidence that egg color indicates female condition at laying in a songbird. Behavioral Ecology 17:651–655. Google Scholar

38.

J. Moreno , J. Morales , E. Lobato , S. Merino , G. Tomás , and J. Martínez-De La Puente . 2006b. More colourful eggs induce higher relative paternal investment in the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca: A cross-fostering experiment. Journal of Avian Biology 37:555–560. Google Scholar

39.

J. Moreno , and J. L. Osorno . 2003. Avian egg colour and sexual selection: Does eggshell pigmentation reflect female condition and genetic quality? Ecology Letters 6:803–806. Google Scholar

40.

J. Moreno , J. L. Osorno , J. Morales , S. Merino , and G. Tomás . 2004. Egg colouration and male parental effort in the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca. Journal of Avian Biology 35:300–304. Google Scholar

41.

K. J. Navara , G. E. Hill , and M. T. Mendonca . 2005. Variable effects of yolk androgens on growth, survival, and immunity in Eastern Bluebird nestlings. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 78:570–578. Google Scholar

42.

Y. Oniki 1979. Nest-egg combinations: Possible antipredatory adaptations in Amazonian birds. Revista Brasileira de Biologia 39:747–767. Google Scholar

43.

R. C. Punnett 1933. Genetic studies in poultry. IX. Blue eggs. Journal of Genetics 27:465–470. Google Scholar

44.

P. Quillfeldt , J. F. Masello , I. J. Strange , and K. L. Buchanan . 2006. Begging and provisioning of Thin-billed Prions Pachyptila belcheri is related to testosterone and corticosterone. Animal Behaviour 71:1359–1369. Google Scholar

45.

S. J. Reynolds , G. R. Martin , and P. Cassey . 2009. Is sexual selection blurring the functional significance of egg coloration hypotheses? Animal Behaviour 78:209–215. Google Scholar

46.

N. Saino , R. Ambrosini , R. Martinelli , P. Ninni , and A. P. Møller . 2003. Gape coloration reliably reflects immunocompetence of Barn Swallows. Behavioural Ecology 14:16–22. Google Scholar

47.

J. J. Sanz , and V. García-Navas . 2009. Eggshell pigmentation pattern in relation to breeding performance of Blue Tits Cyanistes caeruleus. Journal of Animal Ecology 78:31–41. Google Scholar

48.

H. Schwabl 1996. Maternal testosterone in the avian egg enhances postnatal growth. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology A 114:271–276. Google Scholar

49.

L. Siefferman , K. Navara , and G. E. Hill . 2006. Female condition is correlated with egg coloration in Eastern Bluebirds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 59:651–656. Google Scholar

50.

J. J. Soler , C. Navarro , T. P. Contreras , J. M. Avilés , and J. J. Cuervo . 2008. Sexually selected egg coloration in Spotless Starlings. American Naturalist 171:183–194. Google Scholar

51.

J. A. Stamps , A. Clark , P. Arrowood , and B. Kus . 1985. Parent—offspring conflict in Budgerigars. Behaviour 94:1–39. Google Scholar

52.

T. J. Underwood , and S. G. Sealy . 2002. Adaptive significance of egg coloration. Pages 280–289 in Avian Incubation, Behavior, Environment, and Evolution ( D. C. Deeming , Ed.). Oxford University Press, Oxford, United Kingdom. Google Scholar

53.

J. K. Victoria 1972. Clutch characteristics and egg discriminative ability of African village weaverbird Ploceus cucullatus. Ibis 114:367–376. Google Scholar

54.

L. Von Haartman 1957. Adaptation in hole-nesting birds. Evolution 11:339–347. Google Scholar

55.

A. W. Wallace 1889. Darwinism: An Exposition of the Theory of Natural Selection, with Some of Its Applications. Macmillan, London. Google Scholar

56.

L. A. Walters , and T. Getty . 2010. Are brighter eggs better? Egg color and parental investment by House Wrens. Journal of Field Ornithology 81:155–166. Google Scholar

57.

M. Wink , D. Ristow , and C. Wink . 1985. Biology of Eleanora's Falcon (Falco eleanorae): 7. Variability of clutch size, egg dimensions, and egg coloring. Raptor Research 19:8–14. Google Scholar
© 2011 by The American Ornithologists' Union. All rights reserved. Please direct all requests for permission to photocopy or reproduce article content through the University of California Press's Rights and Permissions website, http://www.ucpressjournals.com/reprintInfo.asp.
Christina Riehl "Paternal Investment and the “Sexually Selected Hypothesis” for the Evolution of Eggshell Coloration: Revisiting the Assumptions," The Auk 128(1), 175-179, (1 January 2011). https://doi.org/10.1525/auk.2011.10171
Received: 14 July 2010; Accepted: 1 September 2010; Published: 1 January 2011
Back to Top