Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
9 March 2016 Stopover biology of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds ( Archilochus colubris) during autumn migration
Theodore J. Zenzal, Frank R. Moore
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Surprisingly little is known about the migration and stopover biology of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds (Archilochus colubris), and even less is known about their sex- or age-dependent migration. First, we provide basic information on the migration and stopover biology of this species along the northern coast of the Gulf of Mexico during autumn, including phenology, stopover duration, fuel deposition rate (FDR), arrival mass, and estimated flight ranges. Second, we investigate whether these stopover variables are influenced by age or sex. Age-dependent migration is expected because young, hatch-year birds on their first migration lack the experience of older individuals. Sex-dependent migration is expected because of sexually dimorphic characteristics in wing morphology and body size. We obtained information on arrival mass, phenology, FDR, stopover duration, and estimated flight ranges through banding data, passive integrated transponder tags, radio telemetry, and color marking at a long-term migration station along the northern coast of the Gulf of Mexico. Our data provide strong evidence for age-dependent migration and only weak evidence for sex-dependent migration. Older birds arrived earlier, had larger fuel loads, and had shorter stopover durations than younger birds. In younger birds, we found no effect of sex on FDR, arrival mass, stopover duration, or phenology. Older males arrived with larger fuel loads than females. Finally, we used flight simulation software and our data to estimate that males and older birds were capable of longer potential flight ranges than either females or younger birds.

INTRODUCTION

Approximately two-thirds of all birds breeding in eastern North America are Nearctic–Neotropical migrants (Rappole 1995). Although migratory distance can range from a few thousand kilometers to tens of thousands of kilometers, individuals rarely cover the whole distance in one flight; rather, they stop en route and search for suitable habitat in which to rest and refuel (e.g., Moore et al. 2005). It is estimated that ≥70% of time during migration is spent on stopover (Hedenström and Alerstam 1997), when energy expended is thought to be high in relation to migratory flight (Wikelski et al. 2003). Traveling long distances across areas that vary in suitability comes with uncertainties, including unfamiliar habitat (Németh and Moore 2007), predation pressure (Cimprich and Moore 1999), competition (Lindström et al. 1990), and weather (Newton 2007). Consequently, the mortality associated with migration can be substantial (Sillett and Holmes 2002, Newton 2007, Paxton et al. 2007, Klaassen et al. 2014, Lok et al. 2015; but see Leyrer et al. 2013).

Obtaining food resources during stopover is crucial for birds because it allows them to resume migration in a timely manner. Access to food may be influenced by intrinsic factors such as an individual's sex and age (Lindström et al. 1990, Carpenter et al. 1991, 1993b, 1993c, Moore et al. 2003). Sex-based differences may be due to sexual size dimorphism (e.g., Temeles 1986, Mulvihill et al. 1992, Székely et al. 2000, Hatch and Smith 2009); hormonal differences, especially testosterone (e.g., Geslin et al. 2004, Covino et al. 2015); or differences in aggression (e.g., Moore et al. 2003, Dierschke et al. 2005). Most studies testing sex-dependent asymmetries have found that males dominate females during migration and thereby gain priority access to resources, thus increasing fuel loads and fueling rates (Carpenter et al. 1991, 1993b, 1993c, Maitav and Izhaki 1994, Yong et al. 1998, Moore et al. 2003, Németh and Moore 2012). However, not all studies have found differences between the sexes during migration (Otahal 1995, Izhaki and Maitav 1998).

Age-related asymmetries may also influence migration because younger, hatch-year birds tend to be less efficient foragers and are often subordinate to older, after-hatch-year birds (Gauthreaux 1978, Burger 1988, Wunderle 1991, Woodrey 2000). Banding records during migration are consistent with that expectation, in that young birds typically carry less fat, have lower mass, and stay longer during stopover (Morris et al. 1996, Woodrey and Moore 1997, Woodrey 2000), although an individual's fuel load may drive priority access to resources irrespective of age differences (Moore et al. 2003). In any case, many of the challenges that migrants encounter are likely magnified for young, inexperienced birds, especially during their first migration in autumn.

The Ruby-throated Hummingbird (Archilochus colubris) is a Nearctic–Neotropical migrant and the only species of hummingbird that breeds in eastern North America, yet surprisingly little is known about its migration and stopover ecology. Ruby-throated Hummingbirds, especially older males, are territorial during the breeding season but apparently less so during the remainder of the annual cycle (Weidensaul et al. 2013). In general, hummingbirds represent a unique system compared to other taxa, owing to their aggressive nature, small size, reverse sexual size dimorphism (Ridgway 1911, Weidensaul et al. 2013), high metabolism (0.022 mL O2 min−1; Lasiewski 1963), high energy assimilation efficiency when feeding on nectar (97–99%; Hainsworth 1974), and use of torpor for energy conservation (Weidensaul et al. 2013). From a strictly energetic standpoint, hummingbirds should have a larger fuel-load capacity than most migrants, given that smaller birds can generally carry more fuel reserves in relation to their fat-free mass (Hedenström and Alerstam 1992) and have larger fuel deposition rates (FDR; Lindström 2003). The ability to carry such high fuel loads may seem beneficial during migration; however, in reality, hummingbirds must cope with additional challenges compared to larger birds, such as more frequent feeding and costs associated with maintaining a higher body temperature (Vogel 1988).

Ruby-throated Hummingbirds exhibit reverse sexual size dimorphism, as well as sex- and age-dependent wing morphologies (Stiles et al. 2005). Adult males have the shortest and most tapered wings compared to other age and sex classes. Immature males have slightly longer and less tapered wings compared to adult males. All females share a similar wing morphology, with longer, more rounded wings than males of either age class (Stiles et al. 2005). Short wings permit increased agility, at the expense of increased energetic costs of flight due to high wing disc loading (WDL; Feinsinger and Chaplin 1975, Norberg 1990); whereas long wings permit increased load bearing and lower WDL, though flight speed and agility are decreased (Norberg 1990). Sex- and age-dependent wing morphology may influence migration speed (but see Chai et al. 1999) as well as foraging behavior (e.g., Feinsinger and Chaplin 1975, Feinsinger and Colwell 1978) during stopover.

The first objective of the present study was to provide basic information on the stopover biology of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds, including phenology, stopover duration, FDR, arrival condition, and estimated flight ranges of individuals that had stopped along the northern coast of the Gulf of Mexico in autumn. Our second objective was to evaluate the influence of sex and age on the stopover biology of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds during autumn migration. We hypothesized that males would differ from females during stopover because of differences in wing morphology (Stiles et al. 2005) and increased likelihood of territorial behavior in males (Weidensaul et al. 2013). We also hypothesized that adult birds would differ from young birds during stopover because adults are more experienced and socially dominant to birds of the year, at least in other hummingbird species (e.g., Stiles 1973, Ewald and Rohwer 1980). Specifically, we predicted that (1) males and adults would arrive earlier and in better condition than females and young birds, respectively; (2) males would have larger FDR than females; (3) stopover duration would be shorter in adults and males; and (4) females (with their more energetically efficient wing design) and adults would have longer flight ranges than males and young birds (cf. Pennycuick 2008).

METHODS

Field Site and Data Collection

We captured Ruby-throated Hummingbirds using 29–32 nylon mist nets (12.0 × 2.6 m or 6.0 × 2.6 m with 30 mm mesh) at the Bon Secour National Wildlife Refuge, Fort Morgan, Alabama, USA (30°10′N, 88°00′W; Figure 1), during fall migration, 2010–2014 (Table 1). Nets were open from approximately sunrise until noon (Central Daylight Time), daily weather permitting. Our study site was located 2 km from the end of the Fort Morgan Peninsula and was dominated by slash pine (Pinus elliottii), sand live oak (Quercus geminata), myrtle oak (Q. myrtifolia), hollies (Ilex spp.), saw palmetto (Serenoa repens), and greenbrier (Smilax spp.) (for a complete description, see Zenzal et al. 2013). Netting effort was both passive and active. To increase capture efficiency, we used artificial feeders and baited a subset of nets (2010: n = 11; 2011–2014: n = 4) within our overall netting array. Starting in 2011, we equipped 10 feeders with radio frequency identification (RFID) readers as part of a different project. These 10 feeders were distributed throughout the study site and were not always associated with nets. While the placement of feeders at nets was intended to increase sample sizes, we recognize that this subsequently influenced other factors, such as FDR.

FIGURE 1.

(A) Gulf of Mexico region, with Mobile Bay area enclosed by a square. (B) Fort Morgan peninsula in coastal Alabama, USA. Asterisk indicates study site.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f01.tif

TABLE 1.

Year of study, capture effort, and capture rate per 100 net-hours (1 net-hour = 12 m of net open for 1 hr) of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds during autumn migration in coastal Alabama, USA.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-t01.tif

We banded Ruby-throated Hummingbirds (n = 2,729) with a USGS aluminum leg band, aged them as hatch-year (HY; “young”) or after-hatch-year (AHY; “old”), sexed them according to Pyle (1997), estimated fat (Helms and Drury 1960), measured natural wing chord and mass (to nearest 0.01 g using an electronic balance), and took a wing photo (when time permitted) to determine wing span and wing area for flight range estimates. During 2011–2014, a subset of individuals received auxiliary markers in the form of either a radio-tag (~300 mg; n = 55; JDJC Corp., Fisher, Illinois, USA) or a passive integrated transponder (PIT) tag (~100 mg; n = 549; Cyntag, Cynthiana, Kentucky, USA). During 2012–2014, a subset of birds were auxiliary-marked with back color tags (~30 mg; n = 446) as described by Kapoor (2012); these birds received only one type of auxiliary marker in addition to an aluminum leg band. All auxiliary-marked birds were included in subsequent analyses because radio-tags, the marker type most likely to have effects on individuals due to weight and size, did not affect Ruby-throated Hummingbirds (Zenzal et al. 2014). During 2010, a subsample of birds (n = 88) were held for an aviary study (see Zenzal et al. 2014), and those individuals were included only in our phenology and arrival-condition analyses.

Stopover Variables

We assessed phenology using date of first capture, standardizing date across all years by converting calendar date to ordinal day. Data from 2013 were not used in our analyses of phenology because a U.S. Government shutdown prohibited access to our study site (Table 1). Any calendar dates presented are based on non-leap years.

We corrected for size in our estimate of fuel load to control for sexual size dimorphism and variation in fat deposition among birds. Fat-free body mass was calculated on the basis of regressions of mass and wing length of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds captured at our site between 2010 and 2014 (see Ellegren 1992, Owen and Moore 2006). For each sex, we regressed mass on fat score for individuals with the same wing chord (1 mm increments). The intercept from each wing chord class was then regressed by wing chord for each sex. The resulting equation from the second linear regression provided the size-specific fat-free mass of each wing chord class by sex (males: 2.83 ± 0.29 g; females: 3.12 ± 0.10 g; these and all other results are presented as median ± SD unless otherwise indicated). Estimated fat-free mass was then subtracted from the mass of each bird at initial capture; the difference should provide the fuel load carried by the individual upon capture.

We estimated minimum stopover duration by subtracting the last detection date (recapture, RFID reader, observed color marker, or radio transmitter) from the initial capture date (Cherry 1982). We acknowledge that each marker type likely differs in detection probability; however, we believe that using the most precise data provides an accurate portrayal of stopover biology. Birds captured only once (transient birds; sensu Schaub et al. 2008) and not redetected by any other means were not included in the analysis of stopover duration. We compared age groups when years were pooled. Comparison between the sexes was limited to HY birds because of low sample sizes of AHY birds captured each year.

We estimated FDR (g day−1) using the method described by Cherry (1982), which provides a way to standardize the amount of time between initial capture and final capture, given that some birds have been found to increase mass over the course of the day (e.g., Woodrey and Moore 1997). We first determined the amount of fuel deposited per hour (0.02 ± 0.04 g; mean ± SD) using birds recaptured on the same day, ≥3 hr after the initial capture (transient birds, n = 37; mean [± SD] time between captures = 5.85 ± 2.17 hr). We chose a 3 hr time lapse because birds may continue to show a stress response to handling up to 1 hr after capture (Holberton et al. 1996). Individuals should have acclimated to handling and resumed refueling after 3 hr. We corrected the mass of all individuals used in this analysis to noon using the mean hourly FDR. We determined FDR by subtracting the corrected mass at initial capture from the corrected mass at final capture and dividing that value by the number of days elapsed between captures. We were able to compare only HY males and females that stopped over (nontransient birds) in the FDR analysis because of inadequate samples of recaptured AHY individuals (males: n = 2; females: n = 1). We also performed a simple linear regression to determine mass change of birds captured only once throughout the season (see Woodrey and Moore 1997). This approach allowed us to take advantage of our large dataset, given that we recaptured only ~11% of all birds banded, and to examine refueling rates of each age and sex group. For this analysis, we regressed arrival fuel load (see above) by time of day as a way to test the assumption that birds increased mass over the course of the day.

Finally, we used Pennycuick's (2008) program Flight 1.24 to estimate flight ranges from a random subsample of individuals from each age–sex group (n = 35 for all groups) using the same methods as Zenzal et al. (2014). For each age–sex class, we randomly selected birds with wing photos across all years. The software utilizes wingspan, wing area, fat-free mass, and fuel load upon arrival (described above) to estimate flight range. Ideally, we would use departure condition in flight models rather than arrival condition, but low sample sizes of AHY recaptures (n = 5) precluded use of departure condition for analysis. Only individuals with a wing photo and a positive body condition (above fat-free mass) were included in the analysis, given that both are required to meet the conditions of the model. We derived wingspan and wing area from photographs using ImageJ (Abramoff et al. 2004), and we assumed flight in still-air conditions at an altitude of 500 m (air density = 1.17 kg m−3; based on Kerlinger and Moore 1989, Woodrey and Moore 1997). We used the default settings for all other parameters in the model.

Statistical Analysis

Data were checked for normality using a Shapiro-Wilk test (Shapiro and Wilk 1965). Potential flight range estimates were the only data to be normally distributed; therefore, we performed an analysis of variance on estimated flight ranges to determine an age or sex effect while grouping individual as a random factor within year. Because all other data were not normally distributed even after attempting standard transformations (log and square root), all 2-way comparisons used Mann-Whitney U-tests (Mann and Whitney 1947). For each phenology and arrival condition, we compared sexes of only HY birds for each year of the study (except 2013 for the phenology analysis), because sample sizes of AHY birds were low during each individual year. Subsequently these analyses would be considered multiple comparisons; therefore, we employed the Holm-Bonferroni correction (Holm 1979) to determine an adjusted alpha based on the number of comparisons for each stopover variable. In order to analyze differences between age groups, we pooled data across years to compare overall patterns between AHY and HY birds. We also pooled all years to compare differences between AHY males and females in phenology and arrival condition, but not FDR or stopover duration due to low sample sizes. Finally, a Spearman's rank correlation coefficient was used to look for a relationship between arrival condition and FDR or stopover duration for male and female HY birds that were recaptured on a subsequent day after initial capture. All statistical analyses were performed using R 3.1.3 (R Development Core Team 2015).

RESULTS

Phenology

The overall passage (2010–2014) of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds through coastal Alabama spanned some 62 days, occurring between day 238 (August 26) and day 300 (October 27), with a mean passage date of 268 ± 11 (September 25), capturing 0.12 birds net-hour−1. When examining individual age and sex classes, we found differences in arrival timing between age groups, but not between the sexes.

Older birds arrived earlier than younger birds when all years were pooled (W = 79,156, P < 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.60; Figure 2). The phenology of older birds was more constrained, with all but one arriving over a period of 35 days (from day 247 to 282; Figure 3A), compared to HY birds, which arrived over a period of 62 days (from day 238 to 300; Figure 3B).

FIGURE 2.

Passage dates of after-hatch-year (AHY) and hatch-year (HY) Ruby-throated Hummingbirds during autumn migration (y-axis indicates ordinal date; ordinal day 240 = August 28). Central black line indicates median, notch shows 95% confidence interval, top and bottom of box indicate interquartile range, and whiskers indicate total range. Circles above and below whiskers indicate location of outliers.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f02.tif

FIGURE 3.

Phenology of (A) after-hatch-year and (B) hatch-year Ruby-throated Hummingbirds captured from 2010 to 2014 (x-axis indicates ordinal date; ordinal day 240 = August 28).

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f03.tif

In 2010, HY females arrived earlier than males by 4 days (W = 53,916, P < 0.001, adjusted alpha = 0.01; Figure 4), but we found no difference in arrival timing during any of the subsequent years (P > 0.14; Figure 4). We also found no difference in the arrival timing of AHY males (n = 42) and females (n = 51) when all years were pooled (W = 1,148.5, P = 0.55). Given the overall trend that supported our null hypothesis, evidence suggests that Ruby-throated Hummingbirds do not exhibit differential migration timing by sex in autumn.

FIGURE 4.

Passage dates of hatch-year Ruby-throated Hummingbirds by male and female for 2010–2012 and 2014 (y-axis indicates ordinal date; ordinal day 240 = August 28). Asterisk indicates significant difference between sexes. Central black line indicates median, notch shows 95% confidence interval, top and bottom of box indicate interquartile range, and whiskers indicate total range. Circles above and below whiskers indicate location of outliers.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f04.tif

Arrival Condition

Overall, birds arrived with substantial fuel loads, carrying an estimated 0.92 ± 0.53 g (above lean body mass [LBM]: 33% males, 29% females) of fat upon initial capture (Figure 5). Arrival condition ranged from 0.53 g (below LBM: 19% males, 17% females) below fat-free body mass to 2.80 g (above LBM: 99% males, 90% females) above fat-free body mass (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5.

Fuel load of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds at initial capture. Shown are overall pattern, comparison between age groups (HY = hatch-year, AHY = after-hatch-year), and comparison between AHY males and females for all years pooled. Fat load = amount of fuel over fat-free body mass. Central black line indicates median, notch shows 95% confidence interval, top and bottom of box indicate interquartile range, and whiskers indicate total range. Circles above and below whiskers indicate location of outliers.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f05.tif

We found evidence of age-dependent arrival condition, but mixed results for sex-dependent effects. Older birds arrived in better condition than younger birds when data were pooled across years (W = 143,241, P = 0.001, Cohen's d = 0.33; Figure 5). We found no difference between the sexes in HY birds (P > 0.02, adjusted alpha = 0.01; Figure 6) when we analyzed each year (2010–2014) separately. However, when years were pooled to analyze older males and females, we found that older males arrived with larger fuel loads than older females (W = 762, P = 0.04; Figure 5). Our results suggest stronger differences between the sexes in older birds, but these differences are not reflected in younger birds.

FIGURE 6.

Fuel loads of hatch-year Ruby-throated Hummingbirds by male and female for 2010–2014. Fat load = amount of fuel over fat-free body mass. Central black line indicates median, notch shows 95% confidence interval, top and bottom of box indicate interquartile range, and whiskers indicate total range. Circles above and below whiskers indicate location of outliers.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f06.tif

Stopover Duration

The majority of birds captured (~89%) were transients, never detected again. Among nontransient birds, the overall length of stopover for birds recaptured ≥1 day after initial capture was 2 ± 2 days. Stopover duration of recaptured birds ranged from a minimum of 1 day to a maximum of 15 days (Figure 7). The majority of individuals (72%) that stopped over were redetected ≤3 days from initial capture. We found no difference between the sexes (HY only, P > 0.02, adjusted alpha = 0.01; Figure 8), but we found an effect of age (W = 213, P = 0.05, Cohen's d = 0.80; Figure 7) on stopover duration. All AHY birds (n = 3) were only redetected 1 day after initial capture, whereas 62% of HY birds (n = 376) were redetected ≥2 days after initial capture. Unsurprisingly, stopover length in both HY males (P < 0.001, r = −0.40, n = 244) and females (P = 0.003, r = −0.31, n = 132) was inversely correlated with fuel load at initial capture.

FIGURE 7.

Distribution of Ruby-throated Hummingbird stopover durations between age groups when all years are pooled (HY = hatch-year, AHY = after-hatch-year; x-axis shows minimum stopover durations).

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f07.tif

FIGURE 8.

Stopover duration of hatch-year Ruby-throated Hummingbirds by male and female for 2010–2014 (y-axis indicates minimum stopover duration). Central black line indicates median, top and bottom of box indicate interquartile range, and whiskers indicate total range. Circles above and below whiskers indicate location of outliers.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f08.tif

Fuel Deposition Rate

The overall FDR of nontransient individuals was 0.10 ± 0.24 g day−1 (increase to LBM: 4% males, 3% females), ranging from a negative rate of 0.94 g day−1 (decrease to LBM: 33% males, 30% females) to a maximum of 1.04 g day−1 (increase to LBM: 37% males, 33% females). HY birds make up the majority of these data, heavily influencing the overall results (HY: n = 271; AHY: n = 3). We found no evidence of sex (HY only, P > 0.06, adjusted alpha = 0.01; Figure 9) or age (W = 419, P = 0.93, Cohen's d = 0.60) influencing FDR during autumn migration. We also found no relationship between capture time and fuel load when analyzing data overall (P = 0.17, r = −0.03, n = 2711), by sex (HY only: males, P = 0.11, r = −0.04, n = 1,463; females, P = 0.76, r = −0.009, n = 1,158), or by age (AHY: P = 0.25, r = −0.12, n = 91; HY: P = 0.24, r = −0.02, n = 2,620). However, we did find that the arrival mass of HY males (P < 0.001, r = −0.29, n = 202) and HY females (P = 0.008, r = −0.28, n = 104) was inversely correlated with FDR.

FIGURE 9.

Fuel deposition rates of hatch-year Ruby-throated Hummingbirds by male and female for 2010–2014. Central black line indicates median, top and bottom of box indicate interquartile range, and whiskers indicate total range. Circles above and below whiskers indicate location of outliers.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f09.tif

Flight Models

The overall estimated flight range was 2,261.63 ± 1,152.94 km, with a minimum of 16 km and a maximum of 4,960 km. We found that both age (F1,117 = 10.87, P = 0.001) and sex (F1,117 = 4.25, P = 0.04) affected range estimates. Older birds and males were estimated to be able to fly farther than younger birds and females, respectively (Figure 10). We found no interaction between age and sex (F1,117 = 1.30, P = 0.26). A visual sensitivity analysis of the models found that fuel load was the most influential factor of these flight range calculations.

FIGURE 10.

Potential flight ranges of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds by age (HY = hatch-year, AHY = after-hatch-year) and sex groups. Central black line indicates median, notch shows 95% confidence interval, top and bottom of box indicate interquartile range, and whiskers indicate total range.

i0004-8038-133-2-237-f10.tif

DISCUSSION

Our study fills a huge gap in the natural history of Ruby-throated Hummingbird migration by providing some of the first information on the temporal pattern of arrival, arrival condition, stopover biology (FDR and stopover duration), and departure (flight range), as well as reporting on age- and sex-dependent effects. Much of the information available on hummingbird migration has depended on work with Rufous Hummingbirds in western North America (e.g., Gass et al. 1976, Kodric-Brown and Brown 1978, Gass 1979, Carpenter et al. 1991, 1993a, 1993b, 1993c). Rufous Hummingbirds exhibit age- and sex-dependent migration as well as social dominance during stopover (Carpenter 1993b, and references therein). While we expected similarities between species, each experiences unique challenges during migration. Rufous Hummingbirds move over a continuous land mass, confront many inhospitable arid areas, and find suitable stopover habitat at small alpine meadows, skipping (sensu Piersma 1987) from one to the next (Gass et al. 1976). Ruby-throated Hummingbirds negotiate one large inhospitable area (i.e. the Gulf of Mexico), and otherwise have access to largely contiguous hospitable areas during passage (La Sorte et al. 2014). Moreover, the 2 species experience different competitive interactions: Ruby-throated Hummingbirds face interspecific competition on the wintering grounds (Weidensaul et al. 2013), whereas Rufous Hummingbirds encounter interspecific competition throughout their annual cycle (e.g., Wolf and Hainsworth 1971, Powers and McKee 1994, McCaffrey and Wethington 2008). The 2 migration systems likely reflect distinct ecological processes (sensu Kelly and Hutto 2005).

Overall Stopover Biology

Ruby-throated Hummingbirds migrate through the northern Gulf coast from late August through late October, with peak migration occurring from early September through early October. Our estimates are consistent with an earlier peak passage reported from stations at higher latitudes in North America (Mulvihill and Leberman 1987, Willimont et al. 1988, Weidensaul et al. 2013) and later arrival in the tropics (Deppe and Rotenberry 2005). Arrival condition varied widely among birds captured at our coastal study site, with some birds having almost no body fat and others having fuel loads that were nearly twice their lean body mass. Our recapture data showed that birds that were leaner upon arrival tended to have a higher FDR and longer stopover duration than birds that arrived with larger stores. This suggests that the birds that we recaptured refueled during stopover. That said, most birds that stopped along the Gulf coast were never redetected. We also found a lack of relationship between capture time and arrival fuel load. Not finding a relationship between mass and time of day might suggest that birds are arriving at our study site over the course of the day as they migrate, but more study is needed.

Although we are uncertain whether Ruby-throated Hummingbirds fly around or over the Gulf of Mexico, the average bird captured at our study site could make the ~1,000 km flight across the Gulf of Mexico in still air conditions. Our mean potential flight range was similar to those of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds sampled during migration using different estimation methods (Odum et al. 1961); however, our range was protracted compared to potential flight ranges from Odum et al. (1961). Nevertheless, even birds with adequate fuel loads may opt to move around the Gulf of Mexico, because atmospheric conditions favorable for a trans-Gulf flight do not occur with any regularity until late autumn (Able 1972, Kranstauber et al. 2015). Prevailing weather during migration surely influences the flight energetics of hummingbirds more than those of larger landbird migrants known to make trans-Gulf flights (see Ravi et al. 2015). It is also important to keep in mind that flight simulations may overestimate range, given that hummingbirds have higher metabolic rates than other migrants that were used to develop the models. In any case, the modeling software provides a metric to evaluate differences in wing morphology and condition between different groups within the same species.

Age-dependent Migration

Older birds arrive earlier than younger birds during fall passage, a pattern found in many passerine species (e.g., Woodrey and Chandler 1997, Woodrey and Moore 1997, Jakubas and Wojczulanis-Jakubas 2010). Adult Ruby-throated Hummingbirds leave more northerly areas earlier in the season than younger birds, possibly because males provide no parental care (Mulvihill and Leberman 1987, Peterjohn 1989, Weidensaul et al. 2013), which may explain earlier arrival at our stopover site. Moreover, molt occurs primarily on the wintering grounds (Baltosser 1995). Even if adults and young birds departed the breeding grounds at the same time, differential passage may reflect the fact that younger birds travel more slowly than adults (e.g., spend longer at each stopover site, make additional stops en route, or take a less direct migratory route), which would increase travel time and might explain the disparity we observed between the numbers of old and young birds at our study site.

Older birds arrived with larger fuel loads and tended to stay a shorter time at our site than younger birds, both of which may be due to younger birds having less efficient foraging, social subordinacy, or disorientation than older birds. Our findings are also consistent with evidence that older birds are more efficient at foraging (Burger 1988, Wunderle 1991, Woodrey 2000) and typically gain priority access to resources (Gauthreaux 1978, Woodrey 2000). Although we were not able to assess differences in FDR between age groups, we would not be surprised if older birds had higher FDRs, given differences in fuel load upon arrival. Larger arrival fuel loads would ensure that older birds had longer potential flight ranges, which is what we found. Others have reported age-dependent fuel load and mass in other landbird migrants (e.g., Veiga 1986, Morris et al. 1996, Woodrey and Moore 1997, Jakubas and Wojczulanis-Jakubas 2010). Moreover, stopover duration was negatively correlated with fuel load in young Ruby-throated Hummingbirds, which has been reported in other landbird migrants (Pettersson and Hasselquist 1985, Biebach et al. 1986, Moore and Kerlinger 1987, Yong and Moore 1994).

The ratio of AHY:HY Ruby-throated Hummingbirds captured at our study site was highly skewed (1:28), well beyond what one would expect (1:1 to 1:3) assuming 2 broods per breeding season and 100% survivorship (see Peck and James 1983). In other migrant landbirds, inland sites tend to have a more balanced age ratio than coastal sites (Robbins et al. 1959, Ralph 1971, 1978, 1981), and our migration station is on the immediate coast of the Gulf of Mexico. Young birds on their first migration may not recognize the Gulf of Mexico as an ecological barrier until over the water and then reorient to the closest landmass (e.g., Diehl et al. 2003). It is also possible that young birds stop along the coast to obtain enough fuel for a Gulf crossing (e.g., Woodrey and Moore 1997), whereas adults depart from locations farther inland. Some young migrants arriving at our site may be disoriented (Ralph 1978) or follow a migratory route different from that of adults (Hake et al. 2003, Agostini 2004); perhaps older birds depart inland sites and make trans-Gulf flights, whereas younger birds follow the coast around the Gulf of Mexico.

Sex-dependent Migration

We found little evidence of sex-dependent migration during autumn, contrary to the pattern in spring passage (Németh and Moore 2012). Males and females of both age classes showed similar phenology of passage on the northern Gulf coast. During autumn, there may be little pressure for a particular sex to minimize time spent on migration compared to spring (Smith and Moore 2004, and references therein), but we know little about the wintering ecology of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds. The need to arrive early in order to acquire feeding areas may be diminished because the generalist foraging strategy of this species helps them adapt to changes in resource density (Lara 2006) and/or reduced energetic demands during winter. More study is needed to connect patterns found during autumn migration with the biology of birds on the wintering grounds (sensu Marra et al. 2015).

Among young birds, the sexes did not differ in fuel load upon initial capture, amount of time on stopover, or FDR. Young males and females also exhibited a similar relationship between arrival fuel load and stopover duration, as well as FDR. While we may have expected differences due to morphology and behavior, it is possible that performance differences are minimized when solely analyzing young birds. Young of both sexes are expected to have equivalent levels of experience when facing the challenges of migration, such as resource acquisition, habitat selection, and predator avoidance. Further, differences in wing morphology between young males and females are not as strong as those found in older individuals (Stiles et al. 2005), which might lessen any differences in aerodynamic performance.

Although there was no difference between sexes in young birds, older males arrived with larger fuel loads than older females. The strong differences in wing morphology and WDL (Stiles et al. 2005) could affect not only the ability for individuals to obtain resources, but also the manner in which individuals acquire resources (Feinsinger and Chaplin 1975). Wing morphology is known to influence foraging behavior; males attempt to monopolize resources, whereas females use a traplining strategy (Feinsinger and Chaplin 1975, Feinsinger and Colwell 1978, Norberg 1990; but see Altshuler et al. 2004). It is also possible that adult male Ruby-throated Hummingbirds are socially dominant over females as in other hummingbird species (e.g., Feinsinger and Colwell 1978, Kodric-Brown and Brown 1978, Carpenter et al. 1993b). Note that gorget coloration of older males may provide a competitive advantage if used to convey dominance as in other hummingbird species (Stiles 1973, Ewald and Rohwer 1980).

Although we expected longer potential flight ranges in females because of their more efficient wing design, we found the opposite to be true. However, adult males arrive with larger fuel loads, which were found to override any differences attributable to wing morphology in the calculations. Although female wing design may yield lower energetic costs of flight (Norberg 1990), the ability to obtain resources to fuel flight is likely more important for extended flight bouts.

Conclusions

Our results describe the autumn migration of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds along the northern coast of the Gulf of Mexico. We found evidence of age-dependent migration in phenology, arrival mass, stopover duration, and potential flight ranges, consistent with older birds having a competitive edge during migration. We also found an interaction between sex and age; male and female HY birds did not differ in their stopover biology, but older males had larger fuel loads upon arrival than older females. When age classes were pooled, males had longer potential flight ranges than females. The disparity between the number of old and young birds captured might suggest differences in migration routes or in habitat use along the Gulf coast. Young birds may also rely on the coast for orientation as a leading line (sensu Mueller and Berger 1967), whereas older birds might take a more direct route. Our results differ from those reported on age- and sex-dependent stopover biology of Rufous Hummingbirds, possibly indicating a more structured social hierarchy in that species during migration. The short stopover durations of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds might prohibit social hierarchies from forming, minimizing differences between ages and sexes in how they spend their stopover time.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the Bon Secour National Wildlife Refuge, Fort Morgan Historic Site, and the State of Alabama Department of Conservation and Natural Resources, Wildlife and Freshwater Fisheries Division, for allowing us to work on their properties; the 2010–2014 Fort Morgan field crews for invaluable help with data collection; J. Schaefer for statistical advice; E. Bridge and R. Diehl for assistance with PIT tags and RFID setup; M. Ward and L. Schofield for assistance with radio telemetry data analysis; the 2010–2014 University of Southern Mississippi (USM) radio tower crews; and—last but not least—the past and present members of the Migratory Bird Research Group at USM for their support. We truly appreciate the constructive criticism of Å. Lindström, Z. Németh, and R. Smith, which greatly improved the manuscript. We are also grateful to Wouter Vansteelant and 2 anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript.

Funding statement: Research funding was provided by USM, the National Science Foundation (NSF; IOS no. 1147096), and the National Geographic Society (NGS) Committee on Research and Exploration (no. 8971-11) to J. L. Deppe; and by the following grants awarded to T.J.Z.: NGS Young Explorers Grant (no. 9155-12), Birmingham Audubon Society Walter F. Coxe Research Fund, Alabama Ornithological Society Holliman Research Fund, Wilson Ornithological Society Paul A. Stewart Award, Louisiana Ornithological Society Research Grant, and Eastern Bird Banding Association Research Grant. Research funding and support for T.J.Z. was also provided by a fellowship from the NSF GK–12 program “Molecules to Muscles” (no. 0947944) through USM. None of the funders had any input into the content of the manuscript. None of the funders required their approval of the manuscript before submission or publication.

Ethics statement: All research activities were approved by the USM Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (protocol no. 11092210), U.S. Geological Survey Bird Banding Laboratory (permit no. 21221), and Bon Secour National Wildlife Refuge.

Author contributions: T.J.Z. and F.R.M. conceived the study and developed the methods. T.J.Z. conducted the research and analyzed the data. T.J.Z. and F.R.M. wrote the paper. T.J.Z. and F.R.M. contributed substantial materials, resources, and funding.

LITERATURE CITED

1.

K. P. Able (1972). Fall migration in coastal Louisiana and the evolution of migration patterns in the Gulf region. The Wilson Bulletin 84:231–242. Google Scholar

2.

M. D. Abramoff P. J. Magalhãesand S. J. Ram (2004). Image processing with ImageJ. Biophotonics International 11:36–42. Google Scholar

3.

N. Agostini (2004). Additional observations of age-dependent migration behavior in Western Honey Buzzards Pernis apivorus. Journal of Avian Biology 35:469–470. Google Scholar

4.

D. L. Altshuler F. G. Stilesand R. Dudley (2004). Of hummingbirds and helicopters: Hovering costs, competitive ability, and foraging strategies. The American Naturalist 163:16–25. Google Scholar

5.

W. H. Baltosser (1995). Annual molt in Ruby-throated and Black-chinned Hummingbirds. The Condor 97:484–491. Google Scholar

6.

H. Biebach W. Friedrichand G. Heine (1986). Interaction of bodymass, fat, foraging and stopover period in trans-Sahara migrating passerine birds. Oecologia 69:370–379. Google Scholar

7.

J. Burger (1988). Effects of age on foraging birds. Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress 19:1127–1140. Google Scholar

8.

F. L. Carpenter M. A. Hixon C. A. Beuchat R. W. Russelland D. C. Paton (1993a). Biphasic mass gain in migrant hummingbirds: Body composition changes, torpor, and ecological significance. Ecology 74:1173–1182. Google Scholar

9.

F. L. Carpenter M. A. Hixon D. C. Paton E. J. Temelesand R. W. Russell (1991). Sexual differences in resource acquisition by migrant hummingbirds. Acta XX Congressus Internationalis Ornithologici 2:1156–1165. Google Scholar

10.

F. L. Carpenter M. A. Hixon R. W. Russell D. C. Patonand E. J. Temeles (1993b). Interference asymmetries among age–sex classes of Rufous Hummingbirds during migratory stopovers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 33:297–304. Google Scholar

11.

F. L. Carpenter M. A. Hixon E. J. Temeles R. W. Russelland D. C. Paton (1993c). Exploitative compensation by subordinate age–sex classes of migrant Rufous Hummingbirds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 33:305–312. Google Scholar

12.

P. Chai D. L. Altshuler D. B. Stephensand M. E. Dillon (1999). Maximal horizontal flight performance of hummingbirds: Effects of body mass and molt. Physiological and Biolchemical Zoology 72:145–155. Google Scholar

13.

J. D. Cherry (1982). Fat deposition and length of stopover of migrant White-crowned Sparrows. The Auk 99:725–732. Google Scholar

14.

D. A. Cimprichand F. R. Moore (1999). Energetic constraints and predation pressure during stopover. Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress 22:834–846. Google Scholar

15.

K. M. Covino S. R. Morrisand F. R. Moore (2015). Patterns of testosterone in three Nearctic–Neotropical migratory songbirds during spring passage. General and Comparative Endocrinology 224:186–193. Google Scholar

16.

J. L. Deppeand J. T. Rotenberry (2005). Temporal patterns in fall migrant communities in Yucatan, Mexico. The Condor 107:228–243. Google Scholar

17.

R. H. Diehl R. P. Larkinand J. E. Black (2003). Radar observations of bird migration over the Great Lakes. The Auk 120:278–290. Google Scholar

18.

V. Dierschke B. Mendeland H. Schmaljohann (2005). Differential timing of spring migration in Northern Wheatears Oenanthe oenanthe: Hurried males or weak females? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57:470–480. Google Scholar

19.

H. Ellegren (1992). Estimated effects of age and sex on the fat-free body mass of autumn migrating Bluethroats. Ardea 80:255–259. Google Scholar

20.

P. W. Ewaldand S. Rohwer (1980). Age, coloration and dominance in nonbreeding hummingbirds: A test of the asymmetry hypothesis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 7:273–279. Google Scholar

21.

P. Feinsingerand S. B. Chaplin (1975). On the relationship between wing disc loading and foraging strategy in hummingbirds. The American Naturalist 109:217–224. Google Scholar

22.

P. Feinsingerand R. K. Colwell (1978). Community organization among Neotropical nectar-feeding birds. American Zoologist 18:779–795. Google Scholar

23.

C. L. Gass (1979). Territory regulation, tenure, and migration in Rufous Hummingbirds. Canadian Journal of Zoology 57:914–923. Google Scholar

24.

C. L. Gass G. Angehrand J. Centa (1976). Regulation of food supply by feeding territoriality in the Rufous Hummingbird. Canadian Journal of Zoology 54:2046–2054. Google Scholar

25.

S. A. Gauthreaux Jr (1978). The ecological significance of behavioral dominance. Perspectives Ethology 3:17–54. Google Scholar

26.

T. Geslin O. Chasteland M.-C. Eybert (2004). Sex-specific patterns in body condition and testosterone level changes in a territorial migratory bird: The Bluethroat Luscinia svecica. Ibis 146:632–641. Google Scholar

27.

F. R. Hainsworth (1974). Food quality and foraging efficiency: The efficiency of sugar assimilation by hummingbirds. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 88:425–431. Google Scholar

28.

M. Hake N. Kjellénand T. Alerstam (2003). Age-dependent migration strategy in Honey Buzzards Pernis apivorus tracked by satellite. Oikos 103:385–396. Google Scholar

29.

M. I. Hatchand R. J. Smith (2009). Absence of protandry in a population of Gray Catbirds Dumetella carolinensis. Ibis 151:771–774. Google Scholar

30.

A. Hedenströmand T. Alerstam (1992). Climbing performance of migrating birds as a basis for estimating limits for fuel-carrying capacity and muscle work. Journal of Experimental Biology 164:19–38. Google Scholar

31.

A. Hedenströmand T. Alerstam (1997). Optimum fuel loads in migratory birds: Distinguishing between time and energy minimization. Journal of Theoretical Biology 189:227–234. Google Scholar

32.

C. W. Helmsand W. H. Drury (1960). Winter and migratory weight and fat: Field studies on some North American buntings. Bird-Banding 31:1–40. Google Scholar

33.

R. L. Holberton J. D. Parrishand J. C. Wingfield (1996). Modulation of the adrenocortical stress response in Neotropical migrants during autumn migration. The Auk 113:558–564. Google Scholar

34.

S. Holm (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics 6:65–70. Google Scholar

35.

I. Izhakiand A. Maitav (1998). Blackcaps Sylvia atricapilla stopping over at the desert edge; physiological state and flight-range estimates. Ibis 140:223–233. Google Scholar

36.

D. Jakubasand K. Wojczulanis-Jakubas (2010). Sex- and age-related differences in the timing and body condition of migrating reed warblers Acrocephalus scirpaceus and Sedge Warblers Acrocephalus schoenobaenus. Naturwissenschaften 97:505–511. Google Scholar

37.

J. A. Kapoor (2012). Improved methods for color-marking hummingbirds. Journal of Field Ornithology 83:186–191. Google Scholar

38.

J. F. Kellyand R. L. Hutto (2005). An east–west comparison of migration in North American Wood Warblers. The Condor 107:197–211. Google Scholar

39.

P. Kerlingerand F. R. Moore (1989). Atmospheric structure and avian migration. Current Ornithology 6:109–142. Google Scholar

40.

R. H. G. Klaassen M. Hake R. Strandberg B. J. Koks C. Trierweiler K. M. Exo F. Bairleinand T. Alerstam (2014). When and where does mortality occur in migratory birds? Direct evidence from long-term satellite tracking of raptors. Journal of Animal Ecology 83:176–184. Google Scholar

41.

A. Kodric-Brownand J. H. Brown (1978). Influence of economics, interspecific competition, and sexual dimorphism on territoriality of migrant Rufous Hummingbirds. Ecology 59:285–296. Google Scholar

42.

B. Kranstauber R. Weinzierl M. Wikelskiand K. Safi (2015). Global aerial flyways allow efficient travelling. Ecology Letters 18:1338–1345. Google Scholar

43.

C. Lara (2006). Temporal dynamics of flower use by hummingbirds in a highland temperate forest in Mexico. Ecoscience 13:23–29. Google Scholar

44.

R. C. Lasiewski (1963). Oxygen consumption of torpid, resting, active, and flying hummingbirds. Physiological Zoology 36:122–140. Google Scholar

45.

F. A. La Sorte D. Fink W. M. Hochachka J. P. DeLongand S. Kelling (2014). Spring phenology of ecological productivity contributes to the use of looped migration strategies by birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 281:20140984. Google Scholar

46.

J. Leyrer T. Lok M. Brugge B. Spaans B. K. Sandercockand T. Piersma (2013). Mortality within the annual cycle: Seasonal survival patterns in Afro-Siberian Red Knots Calidris canutus canutus. Journal of Ornithology 154:933–943. Google Scholar

47.

Å. Lindström (2003). Fuel deposition rates in migrating birds: Causes, constraints and consequences. In Avian Migration ( P. Berthold E. Gwinnerand E. Sonnenschein Editors). Springer, Berlin, Germany. pp. 307–320. Google Scholar

48.

Å. Lindström D. Hasselquist S. Benschand M. Grahn (1990). Asymmetric contests over resources for survival and migration: A field experiment with Bluethroats. Animal Behaviour 40:453–461. Google Scholar

49.

T. Lok O. Overdijkand T. Piersma (2015). The cost of migration: Spoonbills suffer higher mortality during trans-Saharan spring migrations only. Biology Letters 11:20140944. Google Scholar

50.

A. Maitavand I. Izhaki (1994). Stopover and fat deposition by Blackcaps, Sylvia atricapilla, following spring migration of the Sahara. Ostrich 65:160–166. Google Scholar

51.

H. B. Mannand D. R. Whitney (1947). On a test of whether one of two random variables is stochastically larger than the other. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics 18:50–60. Google Scholar

52.

P. P. Marra E. B. Cohen S. R. Loss J. E. Rutterand C. M. Tonra (2015). A call for full annual cycle research in animal ecology. Biology Letters 11:20150552. Google Scholar

53.

R. E. McCaffreyand S. M. Wethington (2008). How the presence of feeders affects the use of local floral resources by hummingbirds: A case study from southern Arizona. The Condor 110:786–791. Google Scholar

54.

F. Mooreand P. Kerlinger (1987). Stopover and fat deposition by North American wood-warblers (Parulinae) following spring migration over the Gulf of Mexico. Oecologia 74:47–54. Google Scholar

55.

F. R. Moore S. Mabeyand M. Woodrey (2003). Priority access to food in migratory birds: Age, sex and motivational asymmetries. In Avian Migration ( P. Berthold E. Gwinnerand E. Sonnenschein Editors). Springer, Berlin, Germany. pp. 281–292. Google Scholar

56.

F. R. Moore R. J. Smithand R. Sandberg (2005). Stopover ecology of intercontinental migrants: Solutions to problems and consequences for reproductive performance. In Birds of Two Worlds: The Ecology and Evolution of Migration ( R. Greenbergand P. P. Marra Editors). Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD, USA. pp. 251–261. Google Scholar

57.

S. R. Morris D. W. Holmesand M. E. Richmond (1996). A ten-year study of the stopover patterns of migratory passerines during fall migration on Appledore Island, Maine. The Condor 98:395–409. Google Scholar

58.

H. C. Muellerand D. D. Berger (1967). Wind drift, leading lines, and diurnal migration. The Wilson Bulletin 79:50–63. Google Scholar

59.

R. S. Mulvihilland R. C. Leberman (1987). Bird banding at Powdermill, 1985—with a summary of Ruby-throated Hummingbird banding data. Powdermill Nature Reserve Research Report 46. Carnegie Museum of Natural History, Pittsburgh, PA, USA. Google Scholar

60.

R. S. Mulvihill R. C. Lebermanand D. S. Wood (1992). A possible relationship between reversed sexual size dimorphism and reduced male survivorship in the Ruby-throated Hummingbird. The Condor 94:480–489. Google Scholar

61.

Z. Némethand F. R. Moore (2007). Unfamiliar stopover sites and the value of social information during migration. Journal of Ornithology 148 (Supplement 2):S369–S376. Google Scholar

62.

Z. Némethand F. R. Moore (2012). Differential timing of spring passage of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds along the northern coast of the Gulf of Mexico. Journal of Field Ornithology 83:26–31. Google Scholar

63.

I. Newton (2007). Weather-related mass-mortality events in migrants. Ibis 149:453–467. Google Scholar

64.

U. M. Norberg (1990). Vertebrate Flight. Springer, Berlin, Germany. Google Scholar

65.

E. P. Odum C. E. Connelland H. L. Stoddard (1961). Flight energy and estimated flight ranges of some migratory birds. The Auk 78:515–527. Google Scholar

66.

C. D. Otahal (1995). Sexual differences in Wilson's Warbler migration. Journal of Field Ornithology 66:60–69. Google Scholar

67.

J. C. Owenand F. R. Moore (2006). Seasonal differences in immunological condition of three species of thrushes. The Condor 108:389–398. Google Scholar

68.

E. H. Paxton M. K. Sogge S. L. Durst T. C. Theimerand J. R. Hatten (2007). The ecology of the Southwestern Willow Flycatcher in Central Arizona: A 10-year synthesis report. U.S. Geological Survey Open File Report 2007–1381.  http://pubs.usgs.gov/of/2007/1381/ Google Scholar

69.

G. K. Peckand R. D. James (1983). Breeding Birds of Ontario: Nidiology and Distribution, vol. 1: Nonpasserines. Royal Ontario Museum, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Google Scholar

70.

C. J. Pennycuick (2008). Modelling the Flying Bird. Academic Press, Boston, MA, USA. Google Scholar

71.

B. G. Peterjohn (1989). The Birds of Ohio. Indiana University Press, Bloomington, IN, USA. Google Scholar

72.

J. Petterssonand D. Hasselquist (1985). Fat deposition and migration capacity of robins Erithacus rubecula and Goldcrest Regulus regulus at Ottenby, Sweden. Ringing & Migration 6:66–76. Google Scholar

73.

T. Piersma (1987). Hop, skip, or jump? Constraints on migration of Arctic waders by feeding, fattening, and flight speed. Limosa 60:185–194. Google Scholar

74.

D. R. Powersand T. McKee (1994). The effect of food availability on time and energy expenditures of territorial and non-territorial hummingbirds. The Condor 96:1064–1075. Google Scholar

75.

P. Pyle (1997). Identification Guide to North American Birds, part I: Columbidae to Ploceidae. Slate Creek Press, Bolinas, CA, USA. Google Scholar

76.

C. J. Ralph (1971). An age differential of migrants in coastal California. The Condor 73:243–246. Google Scholar

77.

C. J. Ralph (1978). Disorientation and possible fate of young passerine coastal migrants. Bird-Banding 49:237–247. Google Scholar

78.

C. J. Ralph (1981). Age ratios and their possible use in determining autumn routes of passerine migrants. The Wilson Bulletin 93:164–188. Google Scholar

79.

J. H. Rappole (1995). The Ecology of Migrant Birds: A Neotropical Perspective. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC, USA. Google Scholar

80.

S. Ravi J. D. Crall L. McNeilly S. F. Gagliardi A. A. Biewenerand S. A. Combes (2015). Hummingbird flight stability and control in freestream turbulent winds. Journal of Experimental Biology 218:1444–1452. Google Scholar

81.

R Development Core Team (2015). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Google Scholar

82.

R. Ridgway (1911). The birds of North and Middle America. Bulletin of the U.S. National Museum 50, part 5. Google Scholar

83.

C. S. Robbins D. Bridgeand R. Feller (1959). Relative abundance of adult male redstarts at an inland and a coastal locality during fall migration. Maryland Birdlife 15:23–25. Google Scholar

84.

M. Schaub L. Jenniand F. Bairlein (2008). Fuel stores, fuel accumulation, and the decision to depart from a migration stopover site. Behavioral Ecology 19:657–666. Google Scholar

85.

S. S. Shapiroand M. B. Wilk (1965). An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika 52:591–611. Google Scholar

86.

T. S. Sillettand R. T. Holmes (2002). Variation in survivorship of a migratory songbird throughout its annual cycle. Journal of Animal Ecology 71:296–308. Google Scholar

87.

R. J. Smithand F. R. Moore (2004). Arrival timing and seasonal reproductive performance in a long-distance migratory landbird. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57:231–239. Google Scholar

88.

F. G. Stiles (1973). Food supply and the annual cycle of the Anna Hummingbird. University of California Publications in Zoology 97. Google Scholar

89.

F. G. Stiles D. L. Altshulerand R. Dudley (2005). Wing morphology and flight behavior of some North American hummingbird species. The Auk 122:872–886. Google Scholar

90.

T. Székely J. D. Reynoldsand J. Figuerola (2000). Sexual size dimorphism in shorebirds, gulls, and alcids: The influence of sexual and natural selection. Evolution 54:1404–1413. Google Scholar

91.

E. J. Temeles (1986). Reversed sexual size dimorphism: Effect on resource defense and foraging behaviors of nonbreeding Northern Harriers. The Auk 103:70–78. Google Scholar

92.

J. P. Veiga (1986). Settlement and fat accumulation by migrant Pied Flycatchers in Spain. Ringing & Migration 7:85–98. Google Scholar

93.

S. Vogel (1988). Life's Devices: The Physical World of Animals and Plants. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, USA. Google Scholar

94.

S. Weidensaul T. R. Robinson R. R. Sargentand M. B. Sargent (2013). Ruby-throated Hummingbird (Archilochus colubris). In Birds of North America Online ( A. Poole Editor). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA.  http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/204 Google Scholar

95.

M. Wikelski E. M. Tarlow A. Raim R. Diehl R. P. Larkinand G. H. Visser (2003). Avian metabolism: Costs of migration in free-flying songbirds. Nature 423:704. Google Scholar

96.

L. A. Willimont S. E. Sennerand L. J. Goodrich (1988). Fall migration of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds in the northeastern United States. The Wilson Bulletin 100:482–488. Google Scholar

97.

L. L. Wolfand F. R. Hainsworth (1971). Time and energy budgets of territorial hummingbirds. Ecology 52:980–988. Google Scholar

98.

M. S. Woodrey (2000). Age-dependent aspects of stopover biology of passerine migrants. In Stopover Ecology of Nearctic-Neotropical Landbird Migrants: Habitat Relations and Conservation Implications (F. R. Moore, Editor). Studies in Avian Biology 20:43–52. Google Scholar

99.

M. S. Woodreyand C. R. Chandler (1997). Age-related timing of migration: Geographic and interspecific patterns. Wilson Bulletin 109:52–67. Google Scholar

100.

M. S. Woodreyand F. R. Moore (1997). Age-related differences in the stopover of fall landbird migrants on the coast of Alabama. The Auk 114:695–707. Google Scholar

101.

J. M. Wunderle Jr (1991). Age-specific foraging proficiency in birds. Current Ornithology 8:273–324. Google Scholar

102.

W. Yong D. M. Finch F. R. Mooreand J. F. Kelly (1998). Stopover ecology and habitat use of migratory Wilson's Warblers. The Auk 115:829–842. Google Scholar

103.

W. Yongand F. R. Moore (1994). Flight morphology, energetic condition, and the stopover biology of migrating thrushes. The Auk 111:683–692. Google Scholar

104.

T. J. Zenzal Jr R. H. Diehland F. R. Moore (2014). The impact of radio-tags on Ruby-throated Hummingbirds (Archilochus colubris). The Condor: Ornithological Applications 116:518–526. Google Scholar

105.

T. J. Zenzal Jr A. C. Fish T. M. Jones E. A. Ospinaand F. R. Moore (2013). Observations of predation and anti-predator behavior of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds during migratory stopover. Southeastern Naturalist 12:N21–N25. Google Scholar
Theodore J. Zenzal and Frank R. Moore "Stopover biology of Ruby-throated Hummingbirds ( Archilochus colubris) during autumn migration," The Auk 133(2), 237-250, (9 March 2016). https://doi.org/10.1642/AUK-15-160.1
Received: 24 August 2015; Accepted: 1 December 2015; Published: 9 March 2016
KEYWORDS
age-dependent
Archilochus colubris
intrinsic factors
migration
Ruby-throated Hummingbird
sex-dependent
stopover
Back to Top