Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
23 November 2016 Melanin ornament brightness and aggression at the nest in female Northern Cardinals ( Cardinalis cardinalis)
Caitlin P. Winters, Jodie M. Jawor
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Research is increasingly addressing the evolution and use of sexually selected traits in females. One strong area of interest is the display of intrasexual aggression and how female ornaments are used in intrasexual competition. One particular type of ornament focused on for its use in intrasexual aggression in both sexes is the melanin pigmented ornaments. Over the course of 2 breeding seasons in a southeastern Mississippi population of cardinals, we assessed brightness of the melanin face mask plumage ornament in female Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis) and compared it to behavioral responses during intraspecific simulated nest intrusions (SNIs). All females responded to the SNI, but face mask brightness did not co-vary with the level of aggression shown. Our findings do not support earlier work suggesting that the face mask in female Northern Cardinals is an indicator of aggression at the nest. Potentially, differences in behavior and environmental variables between populations of cardinals could be factors in this difference and deserve further assessment. Future research should investigate other populations of cardinals to fully assess the communicative malleability of this ornament type and different selective pressures on female ornamentation and behavior.

INTRODUCTION

Sexually selected traits have been widely examined in males for a variety of species (Andersson 1994). Initially, less attention was given to sexually selected traits in females, although this has changed with time (Amundsen 2000, Amundsen and Pärn 2006, LeBas 2006, Kraaijeveld et al. 2007, Clutton-Brock 2009). As in males, females have a wide variety of sexually selected traits, for example colorful plumage ornaments and behaviors like song or aggressive displays used in communication and interactions with conspecifics. In many species, female ornaments are not as elaborate as male ornaments. This lack of elaborate ornaments may protect against nest depredation, be linked to females redirecting resources into reproduction, or help females avoid male harassment (Amundsen and Pärn 2006, Dale et al. 2015, Hosken et al. 2016). Female ornaments, however, even when less elaborate, still convey information to conspecifics.

An increasing number of studies have been conducted to understand how ornaments convey information on aspects of condition, behavior, and individual quality in females (Amundsen 2000, Amundsen and Pärn 2006, Clutton-Brock 2009, Nordeide et al. 2013, Hosken et al. 2016). For example, plumage ornament expression has been found to positively co-vary with age (Bitton et al. 2008, Hegyi et al. 2008, Wiebe and Vitousek 2015) and various condition measures, including immune response (Piersma et al. 2001, Hanssen et al. 2006, Gladbach et al. 2010, Grindstaff et al. 2012, Laczi et al. 2013, Lumpkin et al. 2014). In addition, female dominance status (Murphy et al. 2009a, 2009b, Crowhurst et al. 2012) and reproductive behavior and success (Bókony and Liker 2005, Remeš et al. 2011, Midamegbe et al. 2013, Remeš and Matysioková 2013) co-vary positively with ornamentation. In a number of species, males have been shown to prefer more ornamented females as mates (Pilastro et al. 2003, Griggio et al. 2005, 2009, Torres and Velando 2005, Bitton et al. 2008, Chan et al. 2009, Rowe and Weatherhead 2011, but see Wolf et al. 2004). A widely studied behavior associated with ornamentation (in both sexes) is intrasexual aggression and how ornaments convey information on competitive ability.

Among bird species, females display intrasexual aggression for a wide variety of reasons, and becoming dominant over conspecifics can yield significant benefits. Females may be aggressive over mating opportunities and mate guarding (Jonart et al. 2007, Pärn et al. 2008, Papadopoulos et al. 2009), territorial ownership (Koloff and Mennill 2011, Muck and Goymann 2011, Lloyd-Jones and Briskie 2016), and access to breeding resources (Jawor et al. 2006, Cain and Ketterson 2012, Grønstøl et al. 2014, Moreno 2015). For example, Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolor) studies have shown that females are aggressive over access to limited nesting resources (nest cavities) and that more aggressive females are better at acquiring this limited resource (Rosvall 2008, 2011a, 2008b). In Red Junglefowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus), older females and females with greater social experience were more likely to be dominant in flocks, and those individuals showing greater levels of aggressive behavior were more likely to become dominant (Kim and Zuk 2000). Studies investigating aggression and ornamentation have shown that more ornamented females are often more behaviorally aggressive and become dominant (Murphy et al. 2009a, 2009b, Baldauf et al. 2011, Midamegbe et al. 2011, Beck 2013). Crowhurst et al. (2012) showed that Diamond Firetail (Stagonopleura guttata) females with more white spots on their sides were dominant at food resources, and Moreno (2015) showed that more ornamented European Pied Flycatcher (Ficedula hypoleuca) females are more successful at defending nest cavities. Together, findings such as these demonstrate that individuals showing greater aggressive behavior gain benefits and that ornaments can function in aggressive interactions and communication in females.

Melanin ornaments (e.g., black or gray colors) have been strongly associated with conveying information concerning aggressive behavior (reviewed in Jawor and Breitwisch 2003, Senar 2006), although they can also convey information on condition (Piault et al. 2012, Guindre-Parker and Love 2014, Wiebe and Vitousek 2015). In both sexes, larger and darker, melanin ornaments (specifically eumelanic or black-colored ornaments) are associated with higher social dominance, often achieved by a demonstration of greater aggressive behavior (reviewed in Jawor and Breitwisch 2003, Senar 2006). Here, we investigated how a melanin-pigmented plumage ornament, the face mask, co-varies with intrasexual aggression in female Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis). Female cardinals display both intrasexual and intersexual aggression over territories and nesting resources (Jawor et al. 2004, DeVries et al. 2015). In female cardinals, previous work found that face mask expression co-varied with aggressive behavior at the nest (Jawor et al. 2004). Females with a larger and darker face mask tended to be more behaviorally aggressive when faced with a simulated nest intruder (SNI). The assessment system used in Jawor et al. (2004), however, did not separate ornament brightness (how much light is reflected from the ornament) from mask area, but instead blended the 2 into a single scoring system. The amount of light reflected by melanin ornaments influences perceived ornament color and may be linked to the amount of melanin deposited in feathers, as has been shown for carotenoids (McGraw 2006, Montgomerie 2006), and greater pigment deposition in an ornament is often assumed to indicate a higher quality ornament. Additionally, the cells that produce melanin pigments are also responsive to testosterone, the same steroid hormone that can positively influence aggressive behavior, providing a potential physiological link between a communicative ornament and a behavior (Jawor and Breitwisch 2003, McGraw 2006, Bókony et al. 2008). Here, we reevaluated nest defense aggression in the female Northern Cardinal and investigated face mask brightness, specifically by using color reflectance spectrometry, and compared this to behavior shown during SNIs to determine if ornament brightness alone can provide information on aggressive behavior, improving our understanding of how this ornament is used in communication in cardinals.

METHODS

Study Site and General Information

This study was conducted during 2 breeding seasons in 2010 and 2013 at the Lake Thoreau Environmental Education and Research Center (31°25′30″N, 89°25′30″W), owned and maintained by the University of Southern Mississippi (USM), Hattiesburg, Mississippi, USA. This area is a 131 ac longleaf pine (Pinus palustris) forest with understory growth of various herbaceous species. Adult females, all at least 1 year of age but exact ages were unknown, included in the study were captured passively during the nonbreeding and early breeding seasons at food-baited mist nets and walk-in traps. All captured birds were banded with a U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service (USFWS) numbered band and a series of 3 plastic color bands in unique combinations to allow later identification. Individuals had melanin ornamentation measured (see below) and were released at their point of capture. During the early breeding season, territory boundaries and pairs were identified by observation of color-banded individuals. With the initiation of breeding (late March), we searched for nests daily and, once ownership of the nest was confirmed by color band identification and females had been incubating a completed clutch for 3 days, performed an SNI. We performed SNIs after clutches were completed and incubation had begun to avoid nest abandonment, which can occur if females are agitated at the nest prior to and during egg laying or on the first day of incubation (J.M.J. personal observation). Birds were captured and banded under USFWS Bird Banding Permit (#23479); Mississippi Department of Wildlife, Fisheries, and Parks Scientific Collecting Permit (#0319131); and USM's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approval (#08081401).

Simulated Nest Intrusions (SNIs)

Intrasexual aggressive behavior was assessed using an SNI based on methods described in Jawor et al. (2004) and modified slightly based on DeVries et al. (2015). On the day of SNI assessment, between 0600 and 1000 hr, a female Northern Cardinal decoy (mounted in a perching posture) was placed 1 m from the nest of a focal female after the focal female had left for a break in incubation. Nests were observed for female incubation behavior prior to SNIs, and females were allowed to leave the nest on their own initiative before setting up the decoy.

The SNI trial began once the female located the decoy and lasted either 5 min or until the female had struck the decoy. Focal females were given a single behavioral score of 1 to 5 representing a composite of behavior typically seen during aggressive interactions and corresponding to the highest level of aggression shown. A score of 1 was assigned if the focal female returned to the nest but stayed more than 15 m from the nest and showed no aggressive behavior, a score of 2 if the focal female came within 5–15 m of the decoy and displayed no aggressive behavior, a score of 3 if the focal female came within 5 m of the decoy but displayed no aggressive behavior, a score of 4 if the focal female came within 5 m of the decoy and displayed aggressive behavior (e.g., depressed crest, lowered body, song production, wing opening and shivering, flying over the model without striking it, and rapid chipping), and a score of 5 if the focal female struck the decoy. For analyses, females received a single score corresponding to the highest level of aggressive behavior observed during the trial. All females returned to their nests to resume incubation; therefore, the lowest possible score a female could receive was 1.

Ornament Measurement and Analysis

When females were captured for banding, the melanin face mask of females was measured for brightness (e.g., amount of reflected light) using an Ocean Optics USB4000 color reflectance spectrometer with a UV-VIS (215–2000 nm) deuterium tungsten halogen light source (Ocean Optics DH-2000) and a reflection/backscattering measurement probe (T200-RT, 250–800 nm) outfitted with an aluminum shield that held the probe 1 cm from the surface of the feathers at a 45° angle and shielded the probe from outside light (following J. Endler personal communication). The spectrometer was calibrated before measurements using a white Ocean Optics diffuse reflectance standard (WS-1) and a black photographic reference (Kodak). Brightness was averaged across all wavelengths from 300 to 700 nm using SpectraSuite (Montgomerie 2006). Spectra for eumelanic ornaments are flat in shape, but they can differ in the measure of reflected light (brightness); more light reflected is a lighter color, more grayish than black, and less light reflected is a darker color, closer to full black. Face mask brightness was determined after SNIs had occurred.

Additionally, while birds were in hand, a mask score was determined following methods described in Jawor et al. (2004); mask scores ranged from 1 to 5, with a higher number indicating both a larger and darker mask. A score of 1 represents a very light grey mask that extended from the bill 1–3 mm. Increasing mask scores correspond to a progressively larger and darker mask, with a score of 2 extending just to the eyes and 3–4 mm below the bill but light grey in color; a score of 3 extending to behind the eyes and 5 mm below the bill and medium grey in color; a score of 4 extending to behind the eyes and 5 mm or more below the bill and a dark grey in color; and a score of 5 extending behind the eyes and 5 mm below the bill and black in color. Mask score was compared to mask brightness and to behavior shown during SNIs. Mask scores and plumage brightness were all determined by one researcher (C.P.W.).

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 20. Data were not normally distributed, and thus nonparametric tests were used as necessary. Although this study was conducted over 2 years, females appear in the data set only once (one female assessed in 2010 and 2013 had data from 1 year excluded; which year was excluded was determined randomly), and the ornaments a female possessed in the year she was behaviorally assessed were used in analyses of behavior collected from that same year. No female was assessed for ornaments in one year and then assessed for behavior in a different year (e.g., assessed for behavior in 2010 and ornaments assessed in 2013).

We used independent sample t-tests to investigate differences in ornament brightness or behavior between the years of the study; no significant differences were discovered between years (see results). Because both ornament expressions (brightness and mask score) and aggression displayed were not normally distributed, Spearman rank correlations were used to compare face mask brightness to aggression score, face mask score to aggression score, and face mask brightness to face mask score.

RESULTS

We assessed 23 females for intrasexual aggression during SNIs and for face mask expression (n = 13 in 2010; n = 10 in 2013). All females had some level of melanin face mask expression; face masks were quite variable among females. There were no differences in face mask brightness between the 2 years of this study (independent samples t-test: t = 0.72, df = 21, P = 0.48; Figure 1).

FIGURE 1.

Average face mask brightness in female Northern Cardinals in 2010 (n = 13) and 2013 (n = 10). Higher numbers equate to greater intensity, which means more light reflected and an overall brighter color (in the case of melanin a lighter color).

i0004-8038-134-1-128-f01.tif

Aggressive responses during SNIs tended to be category 3 or higher (e.g., females coming within 5 m of the decoy, some either displaying aggressive postures to the decoy or striking it). There was no difference between years in the highest level of aggression shown by females during SNIs (independent samples t-test: t < 0.0001, df = 21, P = 1.00). When comparing the aggression score achieved by a female with face mask brightness, no significant association was found between ornament brightness and aggressive behavior shown (Spearman rank correlation: rs = 0.078, P = 0.72, n = 23; Figure 2).

FIGURE 2.

Aggression score and face mask brightness in female Northern Cardinals. Lower aggression scores correspond to a less aggressive response; lower brightness values correspond to darker face mask coloration.

i0004-8038-134-1-128-f02.tif

Mask brightness, assessed via color reflectance spectrometer, did not co-vary with mask score, a measure determined by visual assessment of color and area simultaneously (Spearman rank correlation: rs = −0.214, P = 0.40, n = 23). Mask score did not co-vary with the level of aggression shown by females during SNIs (rs = −0.133, P = 0.66, n = 23).

DISCUSSION

Our results show that the brightness of the melanin-pigmented face mask plumage ornament in Northern Cardinal females in a southeastern Mississippi population did not seem to co-vary with behavior shown during a simulated nest intrusion. Additionally, we compared aggression to the previously developed mask scoring system for female cardinals (Jawor et al. 2004) and found neither co-variation between mask scores and aggressive behavior in this population nor co-variation between mask score and mask brightness. Initially, we attempted to interpret earlier findings where both a larger and darker face mask (assessed in a simultaneous score) was found to indicate aggressive behavior at the nest to determine whether any one component of the ornament was influential. We focused on brightness of the ornament because this trait could indicate pigment deposition, and typically ornaments with greater pigment deposition are considered higher quality ornaments displayed by high quality individuals. Our findings did not support earlier reports from Jawor et al. (2004) of a northern population of Northern Cardinal (Ohio), where females with a larger and darker face mask (measures combined into a single score) showed higher levels of aggression toward simulated nest intruders. Using the same scoring system in the current study, we saw no indication that the mask score co-varied with aggressive behavior. Several factors, behavioral and ecological, could have influenced the outcomes observed here and led to the differences with previous research.

In the Ohio population of Northern Cardinal (roughly 1,198 km from current study population), initially investigated for aggressive behavior at the nest (Jawor et al. 2004), intraspecific brood parasitism was identified through genetic analyses (Linville 1997). Additionally, opportunistic behavioral observations occurred in the Ohio population of females approaching nests that were not theirs only to be attacked and chased by the actual nest-owning female (J.M.J. personal observation). There is little evidence that the intraspecific brood parasitism identified in the Ohio population is occurring in the Mississippi population studied here (C.P.W. and J.M.J. personal observation). For a different study in Mississippi, both authors completed nest watches while focal females were laying eggs, and eggs were marked for laying order immediately after they were laid (Winters 2011). In all of these observations, we had no female at a nest that was not her own, and we had no eggs removed or added after they were marked. This apparent lack of intraspecific brood parasitism in this southern population would need to be confirmed with appropriate genetic analyses, but it suggests a population difference in this particular behavior. Potentially, the lack of brood parasitism has led to no selective pressure for an ornament that advertises how aggressive a female will be concerning the defense of her nest and its contents. Alternatively, the face mask of female cardinals could still be a signal of aggressive intent and behavior, but in this southern population it may not be used in a nest defense context. Female cardinals participate with their mate in territory defense, aggressively attacking both male and female territory intruders (DeVries 2013). This ornament is possibly used exclusively in territorial aggression and not in maternally related aggression in the Mississippi population under study here. Future assessment of territorial aggression and ornamentation in females would allow this distinction to be made.

Environmental differences exist between the populations of cardinals assessed here and in Jawor et al. (2004) that could also impact the ornament/behavior association. Individuals in the Mississippi population defend territories that appear larger than in the Ohio population assessed in Jawor et al. (2004), and nesting habitat appears to be more abundant in Mississippi compared to Ohio, where land clearing has occurred to a greater extent (J.M.J. personal observation.). This finding suggests that different local environmental and selective pressures may be affecting these populations. Knell (2009) showed that when population density is increased by reduced habitat availability, aggressive interactions are more likely, and individuals showing more aggressive behavior can benefit from being able to acquire and hold onto limited resources. Potentially, where female cardinals have less suitable habitat for nesting (e.g., Ohio population) and population density is concentrated in smaller areas of high habitat quality, more aggression is displayed to acquire and defend the limited nesting resources available, leading to the display of an ornament communicating aggressive ability in defending nesting resources and nests. Coupled with potential nest parasitism, this may be a stronger combination of selective factors to facilitate the linking of an ornament to aggressive nest defense behavior in the Ohio population. Studies designed to test behavior and ornamentation in disparate cardinal populations simultaneously could further determine this possibility.

The potential geographic variation suggested here is not unusual because in a number of species with broad geographic ranges, such as the Northern Cardinal (Halkin and Linville 1999), variation in ornament expression and information content has been found between populations (Roulin et al. 2009, Holand et al. 2011, Dey et al. 2012, Simpson et al. 2015, Argüelles-Ticó et al. 2016). For example, in the Common Yellowthroat (Geothlypis trichas), ornaments that are the focus of mate choice and that co-vary with condition and behavior in males vary between populations (Thusius et al. 2001, Dunn et al. 2010, Freeman-Gallent et al. 2010, Whittingham et al. 2015). In some populations of the Common Yellowthroat, the yellow bib on the throat of males is the focus of choice, whereas in others it is the black face mask; similar measures of quality co-vary with these different ornaments between populations. In widespread populations of the Barn Swallow (Hirundo rustica rustica in Europe, H. r. erythrogaster in North America, and H. r. gutturalis in Japan), variation exists in what ornaments seem to be the focus of mate choice and sexual selection in both sexes (Safran and McGraw 2004, Aparicio et al. 2012, Hasegawa and Arai 2013). Clearly, population variation can exist in ornament expression, and local selective factors will impact how and whether ornaments convey information and are used in communication; therefore, variation in findings between this current study and Jawor et al. (2004) may represent normal ornament variation possible in a wide-spread species like the Northern Cardinal.

Two potential impacts on aggressive behavior not assessed here are body size and condition. Larger and/or individuals in better physical condition may be more likely to initiate more aggressive responses to intruders, either real or simulated, and may have a better chance at winning these interactions. In the Northern Cardinal past work has found that carotenoid-based plumage ornaments can indicate body size and condition (Jawor et al. 2004, Jawor and Breitwisch 2004); however, this has not been the case for melanin-based ornaments. Furthermore, neither size nor condition has been found to co-vary with hormone production (Jawor 2007, DeVries et al. 2011), territory and nest defense (DeVries et al. 2012, 2015), or parental care behavior (Jawor et al. 2004, Jawor and Breitwisch 2004, DeVries and Jawor 2013) in the Northern Cardinal. Previous findings concerning condition, size, and behavior are from both the current study population in Mississippi and the Ohio population previously studied and suggest that body size and condition may not play a strong factor in aggressive behavior in this species. Future studies with Northern Cardinals should investigate more aspects of behavior, ornamentation, body size, and physiological condition between populations, thus elucidating the possibility that populations of this wide-spread species are locally adapted and varied in ornament expression and what those adaptations communicate.

In conclusion, we found that Northern Cardinal females in a southeastern Mississippi population did not show co-variation between aspects of melanin coloration and aggression. Reduced nesting habitat availability and the occurrence of brood parasitism in northern populations of this species may have selected for an ornament conveying information on competitive ability and/or nest defense motivation vs. southern populations where nesting habitat is more abundant and nest parasitism may be lower, or even nonexistent. Findings here suggest that melanin ornaments and aggressive behavior could be more malleable in terms of how and why they are expressed, and future work is needed to incorporate multiple levels of physiology and behavior to understand how selection has shaped this ornament–behavior association in the Northern Cardinal and other species with melanin ornamentation.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank M. S. DeVries, M. Duckworth, A. Holbrook, J. Hooker, J. McGlothlin, A. Winters, and M. Winters for valuable assistance in the field and in the lab, and we thank 2 anonymous reviewers for valuable feedback concerning this work.

Funding statement: Funding provided by USM's Aubrey Keith Lucas and Ella Ginn Lucas Endowment for Faculty Excellence award.

Ethics statement: Birds were captured and banded under USFWS Bird Banding Permit (#23479); Mississippi Department of Wildlife, Fisheries, and Parks Scientific Collecting Permit (#0319131); and USM's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee approval (#08081401).

Author contributions: C.P.W. and J.M.J conceived the idea, design, method development, and experiment; C.P.W. and J.M.J performed the experiments (collected data); C.P.W. wrote the paper with edits from J.M.J.; J.M.J. contributed materials, resources, and funding.

LITERATURE CITED

1.

Amundsen, T. (2000). Why are female birds ornamented? Trends in Ecology & Evolution 15:149–155. Google Scholar

2.

Amundsen, T., and H. Pärn (2006). Female coloration: Review of functional and nonfunctional hypotheses. In Bird Coloration: Function and Evolution, Vol. II. ( G. Hilland K. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA.pp. 280–345. Google Scholar

3.

Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual Selection. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, USA. Google Scholar

4.

Aparicio, J. M., A. Muñoz, R. Bonal, and A. P. Møller (2012). Population differences in density and resource allocation of ornamental tail feathers in the Barn Swallow. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 105:925–936. Google Scholar

5.

Argüelles-Ticó, A., C. Küpper, R Kelsh, A. Kosztolányi, T. Székely, and R. E. van Dijk (2016). Geographic variation in breeding system and environment predicts melanin-based plumage ornamentation of male and female Kentish plovers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 70:49–60. Google Scholar

6.

Baldauf, S. A., T. C. M. Bakker, H. Kullman, and T. Thünken (2011). Female nuptial coloration and its adaptive significance in a mutual mate choice system. Behavioral Ecology 22:478–485. Google Scholar

7.

Beck, M. L. (2013). Nest-box acquisition is related to plumage coloration in male and female Prothonotary Warblers (Protonotaria citrea). The Auk 130:364–371. Google Scholar

8.

Bitton, P., R. D. Dawson, and C. L. Ochs (2008). Plumage characteristics, reproductive investment and assortative mating in Tree Swallows Tachycineta bicolor. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62:1543–1550. Google Scholar

9.

Bókony, V., L. Z. Garamszegi, K. Hirschenhauser, and A. Liker (2008). Testosterone and melanin-based black plumage coloration: A comparative study. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62:1299–1238. Google Scholar

10.

Bókony, V., and A. Liker (2005). Melanin-based black plumage coloration is related to reproductive investment in Cardueline finches. The Condor 107:775–787. Google Scholar

11.

Cain, K. E., and E. D. Ketterson (2012). Competitive females are successful females: Phenotype, mechanism, and selection in a common songbird. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66:241–252. Google Scholar

12.

Chan, R., D. Stuart-Fox, and T. S. Jessop (2009). Why are females ornamented? A test of the courtship stimulation and courtship rejection hypotheses. Behavioral Ecology 20:1334–1342. Google Scholar

13.

Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). Sexual selection in females. Animal Behaviour 77:3–11. Google Scholar

14.

Crowhurst, C. J., V. Zanollo, M. Griggio, J. Robertson, and S. Kleindorfer (2012). White flank spots signal feeding dominance in female Diamond Firetails, Stagonopleura guttata. Ethology 118:63–75. Google Scholar

15.

Dale, J., C. J. Dey, K. Delhey, B. Kempenaers, and M. Valcu (2015). The effects of life history and sexual selection on male and female plumage colouration. Nature 527:367–370. Google Scholar

16.

DeVries, M. S. (2013). Interrelationships between testosterone, aggression, and parental care of a temperate-zone, resident songbird, the Northern Cardinal (Cardinalis cardinalis). Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, MS, USA. Google Scholar

17.

DeVries, M. S., A. L. Holbrook, C. P. Winters, and J. M. Jawor (2011). Non-breeding gonadal testosterone production of male and female Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis) following GnRH challenge. General and Comparative Endocrinology 174:370–378. Google Scholar

18.

DeVries, M. S., and J. M. Jawor (2013). Natural variation in circulating testosterone does not predict nestling provisioning rates in the Northern Cardinal, Cardinalis cardinalis. Animal Behaviour 85:957–965. Google Scholar

19.

DeVries, M. S., C. P. Winters, and J. M. Jawor (2012). Testosterone elevation and response to gonadotropin-releasing hormone challenge by male Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis) following aggressive behavior. Hormones and Behavior 62:99–105. Google Scholar

20.

DeVries, M. S., C. P. Winters, and J. M. Jawor (2015). Testosterone might not be necessary to support female aggression in incubating Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis). Animal Behaviour 107:139–146. Google Scholar

21.

Dey, C. J., I. G. Jamison, and J. S. Quinn (2012). Reproductive skew and female trait elaboration in a cooperatively breeding rail. Ibis 154:452–460. Google Scholar

22.

Dunn, P. O., J. C. Garvin, L. A. Whittingham, C. R. Freeman-Gallant, and D. Hasselquist (2010). Carotenoid and melanin-based ornaments signal similar aspects of male quality in two populations of the Common Yellowthroat. Functional Ecology 24:149–158. Google Scholar

23.

Freeman-Gallant, C. R., C. C. Taff, D. F. Morin, P. O. Dunn, L. A. Whittingham, and S. M. Tang (2010). Sexual selection, multiple male ornaments, and age- and condition-dependent signaling in the Common Yellowthroat. Evolution 64:1007–1017. Google Scholar

24.

Gladbach, A., D. J. Gladbach, B Kempenaers, and P. Quillfeldt (2010). Female-specific colouation, carotenoids and reproductive investment in a dichromatic species, the Upland Goose Chloephaga picta leucoptera. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64:1779–1789. Google Scholar

25.

Griggio, M., A. Devigili, H. Hoi, and A. Pilastro (2009). Female ornamentation and directional male mate preference in the Rock Sparrow. Behavioral Ecology 20:107–1078. Google Scholar

26.

Griggio, M., F. Valera, A. Casas, and A. Pilastro (2005). Males prefer ornamented females: A field experiment of male choice in the rock sparrow. Animal Behaviour 69:1243–1250. Google Scholar

27.

Grindstaff, J. L., M. B., Lovern , J. L. Burtka, and A. Hallmark-Sharber (2012). Structural coloration signals condition, parental investment, and circulating hormone levels in Eastern Bluebirds (Sialia sialis). Journal of Comparative Physiology A 198:625–637. Google Scholar

28.

Grønstøl, G., T. Lislevand, and I. Byrkjedal (2014). Resident female Northern Lapwings Vanellus vanellus fight to exclude settlement by secondary females. Ibis 156:461–465. Google Scholar

29.

Guindre-Parker, S., and O. P. Love (2014). Revisiting the condition-dependence of melanin-based plumage. Journal of Avian Biology 45:29–33. Google Scholar

30.

Halkin, S. L. and S. U. Linville (1999). Northern Cardinal (Cardinalis cardinalis). InBirds of North America Online, no. 440 ( A. Poole, Editor). Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Icatha, NY, USA.  http://bna.birds.cornell.edu/bna/species/440 Google Scholar

31.

Hanssen, S. A., I. Folstad, and K. E. Erikstad (2006). White plumage reflects individual quality in female eiders. Animal Behaviour 71:337–343. Google Scholar

32.

Hasegawa, M., and E. Arai (2013). Divergent tail and throat ornamentation in the Barn Swallow across the Japanese islands. Journal of Ethology 31:79–83. Google Scholar

33.

Hegyi, G., B. Rosivall, E. Szöllősi, R. Hargitai, M. Eens, and J. Török (2008). Phenotypic plasticity in a conspicuous female plumage trait: Information content and mating patterns. Animal Behaviour 575:977–989. Google Scholar

34.

Holand, A. M., H. Jensen, J. Tufto, and R. Moe (2011). Does selection or genetic drift explain geographic differentiation of morphological characters in House Sparrows Passer domesticus? Genetic Research 93:367–379. Google Scholar

35.

Hosken, D. J., S. H. Alonzo, and N. Wedell (2016). Why aren't signals of female quality more common? Animal Behaviour 114:199–201. Google Scholar

36.

Jawor, J.M. (2007). Testosterone in Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis): Possible influence of prolonged territorial behavior. The Auk 124:331–338. Google Scholar

37.

Jawor, J. M., and R. Breitwisch (2003). Melanin ornaments, honesty and sexual selection. The Auk 120:249–265. Google Scholar

38.

Jawor, J. M., and R. Breitwisch (2004). Multiple ornaments in male Northern Cardinals (Cardinalis cardinalis) as indicators of condition. Ethology 110:113–126. Google Scholar

39.

Jawor, J. M., N. Gray, S. M. Beall, and R. Breitwisch (2004). Multiple ornaments correlate with aspects of condition and behaviour in female Northern Cardinals, Cardinalis cardinalis. Animal Behaviour 67:875–882. Google Scholar

40.

Jawor, J. M., R. Young, and E. D. Ketterson (2006). Females competing to reproduce: Dominance matters but testosterone may not. Hormones and Behavior 49:362–368. Google Scholar

41.

Jonart, L. M., G. E. Hill, and A. V. Badyaev (2007). Fighting ability and motivation: Determinants of dominance and contest strategies in females of a passerine bird. Animal Behaviour 74:1675–1681. Google Scholar

42.

Kim, T., and M. Zuk (2000). The effects of age and previous experience on social rank in female Red Junglefowl, Gallus gallus spadiceus. Animal Behaviour 60:239–244. Google Scholar

43.

Knell, R. J. (2009). Populaiton density and the evolution of male aggression. Journal of Zoology 278:83–90. Google Scholar

44.

Koloff, J., and D. Mennill (2011). Aggressive responses to playback of solos and duets in a Neotropical antbird. Animal Behaviour 82:587–593. Google Scholar

45.

Kraaijeveld, K., F. J. L. Kraaijeveld-Smit, and J. Komdeur (2007). The evolution of mutual ornamentation. Animal Behaviour 74:657–677. Google Scholar

46.

Laczi, M., G. Hegyi, M. Herenyi, D. Kiss, G. Marko, G. Nagy, B. Rosivall, E. Szollosi, and J. Torok (2013). Integrate plumage colour variation in relation to body condition, reproductive investment and laying date in the Collared Flycatcher. Naturwissenschaften 100:983–991. Google Scholar

47.

LeBas, N. R. (2006). Female finery is not for males. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 21:170–173. Google Scholar

48.

Linville, S. U. (1997). Sexual selection and plumage ornamentation in a socially monogamous passerine, the Northern Cardinal (Cardinalis cardinalis). Ph.D. dissertation, University of Dayton, Dayton, OH, USA. Google Scholar

49.

Lloyd-Jones, D. J., and J. V. Briskie (2016). Mutual wattle ornaments in the South Island Saddleback (Philesturnus carunculatus) function as armaments. Ethology 22:61–71. Google Scholar

50.

Lumpkin, D. C., T. G. Murphy, and K. A. Tarvin (2014). Blood parasite infection differentially related to carotenoid-based plumage and bill color in the American Goldfinch. Ecology and Evolution 4:3210–3217. Google Scholar

51.

McGraw, K. J. (2006). Mechanics of melanin-based coloration. In Bird Coloration: Mechanisms and Measurements, Vol. I ( G. Hilland K. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA.pp. 243–294. Google Scholar

52.

Midamegbe, A., A. Grégoire, P. Perret, and C. Doutrelant (2011). Female–female aggressiveness is influenced by female coloration in Blue Tits. Animal Behaviour 82:245–253. Google Scholar

53.

Midamegbe, A., A. Grégoire, V. Staszewski, P. Perret, M. M. Lambrechts, T. Boulinier, and C. Doutrelant (2013). Female Blue Tits with brighter yellow chests transfer more carotenoids to their eggs after an immune challenge. Oecologia 173:387–397. Google Scholar

54.

Montgomerie, R. (2006). Analyzing colors. In Bird Coloration: Mechanisms and Measurements, Vol. I ( G. Hilland K. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA.pp. 90–147. Google Scholar

55.

Moreno, J. (2015). The incident of clutch replacements in the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca is related to nest-box availability: Evidence of female–female competition? Areola 62:67–80. Google Scholar

56.

Muck, C., and W. Goymann (2011). Throat patch size and darkness covaries with testosterone in females of a sex-role reversed species. Behavioral Ecology 22:1312–1319. Google Scholar

57.

Murphy, T. G., D. Hernández-Muciño, M. Osorio-Beristain, R. Montgomerie, and K. E. Omland (2009a). Carotenoid-based status signaling by females in the tropical Streak-backed Oriole. Behavioral Ecology 20:1000–1006. Google Scholar

58.

Murphy, T. G., M. F. Rosenthal, R. Montgomerie, and K. A. Tarvin (2009b). Female American Goldfinches use carotenoid-based bill coloration to signal status. Behavioral Ecology 20:1348–1355. Google Scholar

59.

Nordeide, J. T., J. Kekäläinen, M. Janhunen, and R. Kortet (2013). Female ornaments revisited – are they correlated with offspring quality? Journal of Animal Ecology 82:26–38. Google Scholar

60.

Papadopoulos, N. T., J. R. Carey, P. Liedo, H. Müller, and D. Sentürk (2009). Virgin females compete for mates in the male lekking species Ceratitis capitata. Physiological Entomology 34:238–245. Google Scholar

61.

Pärn, H., K. M. Lindström, M. Sandell, and T. Amundsen (2008). Female aggressive response and hormonal correlates – an intrusion experiment in a free-living passerine. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62:1665–1677. Google Scholar

62.

Piault, R., V. van den Brink, and A. Roulin (2012). Condition-dependent expression of a melanin-based coloration in the Eurasian Kestrel. Naturwissenschaften 99:391–396. Google Scholar

63.

Piersma, T., L. Mendes, J. Hennekens, S. Ratiarison, S. Groenewold, and J. Jukema (2001). Breeding plumage honestly signals likelihood of tapeworm infestation in females of a long-distance migrating shorebird, the Bar-tailed Godwit. Zoology 104:41–48. Google Scholar

64.

Pilastro, A., M. Griggio, and G. Matessi (2003). Male Rock Sparrows adjust their breeding strategy according to female ornamentation: Parental or mating investment? Animal Behaviour 66:265–271. Google Scholar

65.

Remeš, V., B. Matysioková, and B. Klejdus (2011). Egg yolk antioxidant deposition as a function of parental ornamentation, age, and environment in Great Tits Parus major. Journal of Avian Biology 42:387–396. Google Scholar

66.

Remeš, V., and B. Matysioková (2013). More ornamented females produce higher-quality offspring in a socially monogamous bird: An experimental study in the Great Tit (Parus major). Frontiers in Zoology.  http://www.frontiersinzoology.com/content/10/1/14 Google Scholar

67.

Rosvall, K. A. (2008). Sexual selection on aggressiveness in females: Evidence from an experimental test with Tree swallows. Animal Behaviour 75:1603–1610. Google Scholar

68.

Rosvall, K. A. (2011a). Intrasexual competition in females: Evidence for sexual selection? Behavioral Ecology 22:1131–1140. Google Scholar

69.

Rosvall, K. A. (2011b). Maintenance of variation in sexually selected traits in females: A case study using intrasexual aggression in Tree Swallows Tachycineta bicolor. Journal of Avian Biology 42:454–462. Google Scholar

70.

Roulin, A., M. Wink, and N. Salamin (2009). Selection on a eumelanic ornament is stronger in the tropics than in the temperate zones in the worldwide-distributed Barn Owl. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 22:345–354. Google Scholar

71.

Rowe, K. M. C., and P. J. Weatherhead (2011). Assortative mating in relation to plumage traits shared by male and female American Robins. The Condor 113:881–889. Google Scholar

72.

Safran, R. J., and K. J. McGraw (2004). Plumage coloration, not length or symmetry of tail-streamers, is a sexually selected trait in North American Barn Swallows. Behavioral Ecology 15:455–461. Google Scholar

73.

Senar, J. C. (2006). Color displays as intrasexual signals of aggression and dominance. In Bird Coloration: Function and Evolution, Vol. II ( G. Hilland K. McGraw, Editors). Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, USA.pp. 87–136. Google Scholar

74.

Simpson, R. K., M. A. Johnson, and T. G. Murphy (2015). Migration and the evolution of sexual dichromatism: Evolutionary loss of female coloration with migration among wood-warblers. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 282:20150375.  http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0375 Google Scholar

75.

Thusius, K. J., K. A. Peterson, P. O. Dunn, and L. A. Whittingham (2001). Male mask size is correlated with mating success in the Common Yellowthroat. Animal Behaviour 62:435–446. Google Scholar

76.

Torres, R., and A. Velando (2005). Male preference for female foot colour in the socially monogamous Blue-footed Booby, Sula nebouxii. Animal Behaviour 69:59–65. Google Scholar

77.

Whittingham, L. A., C. R. Freeman-Gallant, C. C. Taff, and P. O. Dunn. (2015). Different ornaments signal male health and MHC variation in two populations of a warbler. Molecular Ecology 24:1584–1595. Google Scholar

78.

Wiebe, K. L., and M. N. Vitousek (2015). Melanin plumage ornaments in both sexes of Northern Flicker are associated with body condition and predict reproductive output independent of age. The Auk: Ornithological Advances 132:507–517. Google Scholar

79.

Winters, C. (2011). Ornamentation, behavior and maternal effects in the female northern cardinal Cardinalis cardinalis. Master's thesis, University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, MS, USA. Google Scholar

80.

Wolf, W. L., J. M. Casto, V. Nolan, Jr., and E. D. Ketterson (2004). Female ornamentation and male mate choice in Dark-eyed Juncos. Animal Behaviour 67:93–102. Google Scholar
© 2017 American Ornithologists' Union
Caitlin P. Winters and Jodie M. Jawor "Melanin ornament brightness and aggression at the nest in female Northern Cardinals ( Cardinalis cardinalis)," The Auk 134(1), 128-136, (23 November 2016). https://doi.org/10.1642/AUK-16-83.1
Received: 26 April 2016; Accepted: 1 September 2016; Published: 23 November 2016
KEYWORDS
female ornamentation
intrasexual aggression
melanin ornamentation
northern cardinals
Back to Top