Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 October 1999 Exploration into the Adaptive Design of the Arthropod “Microbrain”
Makoto Mizunami, Fumio Yokohari, Masakazu Takahata
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Arthropods have small but sophisticated brains which have enabled them to adapt their behavior to a diverse range of environments. The enormous evolutionary success of arthropods in terms of species richness and diversity depends on the sophistication of their brains. Advances in neurobiology have clarified some of the sensory and motor mechanisms of the arthropod brain, but the basic rules of computation underlying the central functions of the arthropod brain remain unknown. Consequently, it is not known how the basic design of the arthropod brain differs from, or is analogous to, that of other animals, especially mammals. In this report, we argue that characteristic features of the arthropod “microbrain” can be ascribed not only to the limited number of its constituting neurons but also to the optimization to life with a small body.

INTRODUCTION

Arthropods are the most diverse and abundant animal group representing about 80% of all known animal species (Wilson, 1988; Brusca and Brusca, 1990), and they play a vital role in the Earth's ecosystem. These animals have successfully adapted themselves to virtually every habitat and have developed amazingly diverse forms of behavior.

The diverse behavior of arthropods is all generated and controlled by their very small brains. The number of neurons contained in the arthropod brain is only 105−106 (Strausfeld, 1976), that is several orders of magnitude smaller than those of the mammalian brain (1010 in humans, Kandel et al., 1991). Therefore, the information-processing capacity of the arthropod brain is much more limited than that of the mammalian brain and, consequently, arthropod behavior is more stereotyped and less flexible than that of mammals (Hoyle, 1976). Nevertheless, the arthropod brain is, no doubt, one of the most successful information-processing devices to have evolved on Earth. This is analogous to the fact that, while microcomputers have only a limited information processing capacity, compared with that of supercomputers, they can be adapted more easily to meet personal and business needs and, consequently, play a vital role in the present human society. The arthropod brain, hereafter referred to as “microbrain,” is a low-cost, light-weight, and compact information processing device, a masterpiece which has evolved by natural selection over a period of 500 million years. We argue here that the microbrain is optimized to fulfil functional requirements that are in part different from those for the mammalian brain, hereafter referred to as “megalobrain”.

THE BRAIN AND EVOLUTIONARY SUCCESS OF ARTHROPODS

To attain insight into the design principles of arthropod “microbrain”, we first discuss features which have made arthropods so successful. A hard exoskeleton protects them from predators and from physiological stress, movable jointed appendages with powerful striated muscles allow for fast movement; all features with selective advantages over other animals (Willmer, 1990; Brusca and Brusca, 1990). The small size of most arthropods, due probably to limitations imposed by mechanical properties of the exoskeleton (Currey 1967; Schmidt-Nielsen 1986), is also an important determinant of species richness, because there are many more niches in any given environment for small animals to survive and develop (Pianka, 1970; May, 1978; Gullan and Cranston, 1994). In general, rapid motion requires good coordination of the appendages, and utilization of microhabitats requires the ability to recognize and respond to different environmental factors. Thus, the success of arthropods depends on sophistication of the nervous system.

The argument that the evolutionary success of arthropods has crucially depended upon the sophistication of the nervous system is supported by consideration of the reasons for the success of insects, the most prosperous group of arthropods that represent 90% of their total species (Wilson, 1988). The high species diversity of insects has been attributed, at least in part, to the following factors (Wessells and Hopson, 1988; Peters, 1988; Gullan and Cranston, 1994). First, the acquisition of wings in the adult stage enables insects to disperse more easily. The development of flight requires the development of a nervous system that can rapidly perceive visual and mechanosensory signals and control the motor pattern of flight muscles. Second, lepidopteran, coleopteran, hymenopteran, and dipteran insects maintain a special relationship with flowering plants, which made way for further division of niches. Color vision, odor discrimination and pattern recognition have become more sophisticated during the coevolution of these insects and flowering plants (e.g. Chittka and Menzel, 1992). Third, complete metamorphosis has allowed conspecific juveniles and adults to utilize different resources, thus reducing competition. To regulate metamorphosis, the development of an endocrine system and coordination with the nervous system were required.

While arthropods are the most successful group in terms of species richness and diversity, they are far less successful than higher vertebrates, especially mammals, in terms of complexity of the body structure and behavior. On the phylogenetic tree, arthropods and vertebrates are located at the top of the two major animal lineages, the protostomes and the deuterostomes, respectively (Fig. 1). These two groups attained evolutionary success by exploiting completely different life styles. Pianka (1970) argued that for short-lived animals with a small body size, such as insects, the reproductive strategy is to produce a large number of offspring at each reproduction; each offspring, however, has only a small probability of survival to adulthood. For long-lived animals with a large body size, such as terrestrial vertebrates, the strategy is to produce a small number of offspring, each with a high chance of survival. The former species are referred to as relatively r-selected and the latter as K-selected (K refers to the carrying capacity of the environment at equilibrium and r to the maximal intrinsic rate of reproduction in the logistic formula). Table 1 shows some of correlates of r- and K-selections (Pianka, 1970). Animals with different reproductive strategies require different behavioral capabilities. For example, a high capability of dispersal is important for r-selected species, while K-selected animals require a high capability of learning since they typically adapt to environmental change by modifying individual behavior. In addition, small, r-selected animals utilizing microhabitats require relatively limited behavioral repertories compared to large, K-selected animals utilizing diverse environmental resources. Optimization to support life style of relatively r-selected animals is probably one of design principles of arthropod “microbrain”.

Fig. 1

Phylogenetic tree of the animal kingdom showing the evolution of nervous systems. The animal kingdom is divided into two branches, the protostomia and the deuterostomia. The most complicated physical structures and behavioral patterns are found among vertebrata, arthropoda, and mollusca. The nervous systems of arthropods share a number of features with higher vertebrates, reflecting convergent evolution, but also exhibit many specific features reflecting adaptation to their characteristic lifestyles. The dendrogram shown is based on Brusca and Brusca (1990).

i0289-0003-16-5-703-f01.gif

Table 1

Some of the correlates of r- and K-seection. From Pianka (1970).

i0289-0003-16-5-703-t01.gif

ORGANIZATION OF THE ARTHROPOD MICROBRAIN

In Fig. 2, a generalized scheme of the central nervous system of an arthropod is illustrated, using the honey bee as an example. Serially arranged ganglia along the length of the body are connected by a pair of axonal commissures to form a rudder-like nervous system. Ganglia of the head are fused to form a brain and a subesophageal ganglion. The brain contains sensory, associative and premotor centers that include: the antennal lobe (olfactory center); the dorsal lobe (antennal mechanosensory center); the optic lobe (visual center); the mushroom body, which is critical to olfactory learning (Davis, 1996; Heisenberg, 1998), place learning (Mizunami et al., 1998d), and higher motor control (Huber, 1960; Mizunami et al., 1998c; Okada et al., 1999); the central complex, which is implicated in motor coordination (Strauss and Heisenberg, 1993); and the posterior slope and the lateral accessory lobe, which are premotor centers from which descending neurons originate to supply the thoracic ganglia. Signals from the brain are transmitted to motor circuits in thoracic or abdominal ganglia for execution of locomotory or other behavior.

Fig. 2

A schematic illustration of the organization of the central nervous system of the honey bee, based on Mobbs (1985). The central nervous system consists of the supraesophageal, subesophageal, thoracic, and abdominal ganglia. The supraesophageal ganglion (brain) contains sensory, associative and premotor centers, including the optic lobe (the visual center that consists of three neuropils; i.e., the lamina, medulla, and lobula), the antennal lobe (olfactory center), the mushroom body (associative center), the central complex (a center for motor coordination), and the posterior slope and the lateral accessory lobe (premotor centers from which descending neurons originate). Signals from the brain are transmitted to the thoracic ganglia for execution of locomotory behavior. Thoracic ganglia contain motor circuits for locomotion. The subesophageal ganglion (SOG) contains a center for feeding. Abdominal ganglia contain centers for digestion, excretion and copulation.

i0289-0003-16-5-703-f02.gif

FUNCTIONAL ADAPTATION OF THE ARTHROPOD MICROBRAIN

How can the basic design of the arthropod brain be understood in terms of functional adaptation and evolutionary constraints? We suggest that two factors, the smallness of the number of neurons and the optimization to the life style with a small body, are crucially related to the basic design of the arthropod brain.

a) Small number of neurons

The small body size of most arthropods has set a fundamental constraint on the design of their brain by limiting the number of its constituent neurons. Computation by means of a small number of neurons is, in essence, characterized by quickness, simplicity, and economy, whereas that by means of a large number of neurons is characterized by accuracy, complexity and flexibility. Economical and speed-oriented features of the arthropod “microbrain” are evident in that (1) the sensory systems of arthropods typically extract relatively small numbers of biologically significant features so that the need for further central processing is reduced (Wehner, 1987), (2) learning in arthropods is, typically, a modification of a specific element of stereotyped behavior (Alcock, 1989; Menzel, 1990; Wehner, 1992) and (3) most arthropod behavior is controlled by motor command systems that consist of a relatively small number of neurons and produce quick and relatively stereotyped motor patterns (Wiersma and Ikeda, 1964; Liebenthal et al., 1994; Kolton and Camhi, 1995; Sparks et al., 1997). It can be pointed out that the third point (3) is in accord with the functional need of small animals, since quickness is a major requirement for controlling motion of small animals because the time-scale of motion of smaller animals tends to be smaller due to smaller mass of inertia.

An example of adaptive specialization of the arthropod nervous system is discussed below. The motor systems of crustaceans and insects contain a number of non-spiking premotor interneurons that transmit signals by way of graded, not action potentials (Takahata and Hisada, 1991). Non-spiking interneurons have never been found in the motor systems of vertebrates. Each part of these interneurons can act as an independent processing unit and, thus, a single neuron can act as a multi-processor (Hikosaka and Takahata, 1998). Nonspiking interneurons are also able to operate as the integrating processor of various sensory and central inputs to allow for switching between different motor patterns or to form smooth motor responses (Takahata and Hisada, 1991). The versatility of non-spiking neurons is a good example of adaptation to attain an enhanced signal processing capability with a limited number of neurons.

The mushroom body of the insect brain provides a curious exception to the general rule that a relatively small number of neurons serve as the basic processing unit in the arthropod brain. In the cockroach, each mushroom body contains 200,000 intrinsic neurons (Neder 1959) that are organized into ca. 15 repetitive modular subunits, each subunit consisting of a pair of slabs (Mizunami et al., 1998a, b). It is likely that slabs act as functional units (Mizunami et al. 1998a, Iwasaki et al. 1999). Computation in each slab may be based on the synergistic operation of a large number of neurons, as is typical in the mammalian cortex (Georgopoulos et al., 1986, 1988).

b) Adaptation to life with a small body

Small terrestrial arthropods that utilize microhabitats are threatened by changes in the environment. This is related to the physical rule that the smaller a given animal is, the higher is the surface-to-volume ratio and, thus, the more susceptible it is to environmental change. For this reason, terrestrial arthropods developed the ability to detect minute changes in temperature, humidity and carbon dioxide concentration. Here, we focus on humidity detection by insects. Cockroaches, honey bees and many other insects have hygroreceptors on their antennae. The response of hygroreceptors depends on relative humidity, and the mechanism of the primary process of hygroreception is non-chemical (Yokohari and Tateda, 1976; Yokohari, 1978). There is evidence to suggest that the hygroreceptors transduce humidity changes to electrical potential through mechanical strain induced in hygroscopic components of the sensillum (Yokohari, 1978, 1981). Thus, insects probably developed their hygroreceptors through modifying mechanoreceptors. In accord with this theory, axon terminals of hygroreceptors occupy the dorsal margin of the antennal lobe, an area adjacent to the dorsal lobe in which axons of antennal mechanoreceptors terminate (Nishikawa et al., 1995).

It has been argued that small, short-lived animals are capable of responding to environmental change by genetic change (Pianka, 1970; Johnston, 1982), hence, the ability to learn from experience is relatively unimportant. Recent studies, however, showed that even one of the smallest and shortest-lived insects, the fruit-fly Drosophila, can perform various forms of olfactory, visual and motor learning (Davis, 1996, Wolf et al., 1998). Thus, learning is probably a more essential element of arthropod behavior than it has hitherto been considered. Mayr (1974) argued that the social or communicative behavior of animals tends to be relatively inflexible, while resource-directed behavior is more likely to be flexible because the environment is so variable. This argument is, no doubt, applicable to arthropods. Some of the fascinating resource-oriented behavior of arthropods that include memory-dependent components are: the mass migration of spiny lobsters on the basis of magnetic orientation (Lohmann et al., 1995); the homing by motor memory of fiddler crabs (Hagen, 1967), wandering spiders (Seyfarth et al., 1982) and orb-weaving spiders (Mittelstaedt, 1985); sun or skylight compass orientation of bees and ants (Wehner, 1989, 1992; Wehner et al., 1996); the foraging behavior of honey bees that is dependent upon the ability to learn about the odor, color, shape, and location of food sources (Frisch, 1967; Gould and Gould, 1988; Menzel, 1990; Chittka and Menzel, 1992, Srinivasan et al., 1998); and the communication between honey bees of information concerning the distance and direction to food sources by a waggling dance (Frisch, 1967). The brain mechanisms underlying these types of behavior remain for subjects of future investigations.

DIVERSITY AMONG ARTHROPOD MICROBRAINS

There are notable variations in the organization of brains among different arthropod groups. Scorpions have olfactory sensilla on abdominal appendages (Gaffin and Brownell, 1997) and these appendages are not homologous to insect antennae. Interestingly, axon terminals of scorpion olfactory sensilla form glomeruli in thoracic neuromeres (Brownell, 1991), just as axons of insect olfactory sensilla form glomeruli in the antennal lobe. In the crustacean brain, neuropil homologous to the insect mushroom body has not been identified (Strausfeld, 1998). Further, the central body of spiders is probably not homologous to the central complex of insects (Breidbach, 1995); i.e., the central body in spiders appears to be a visual center (Strausfeld et al., 1993), whereas the central complex of insects plays a role in motor coordination (Strauss and Heisenberg, 1993). The structures of first and second neuropils in the optic lobe of insects are in large part homologous to that of crustaceans, but the third optic neuropils may have evolved independently in each group (Strausfeld, 1998). How these varied features among arthropods are related to evolutionary history and functional adaptations remains to be elucidated.

COMMON FEATURES OF THE ARTHROPOD MICROBRAIN AND THE MAMMALIAN MEGALOBRAIN

Although we emphasize that the “microbrain” of arthropods is optimized to their specific lifestyle, it is also evident that the arthropod “microbrain” shares many features with the mammalian “megalobrain”. For example, the primary olfactory center of insects (antennal lobe) has many structural and functional similarities to that of mammals (olfactory bulb) (Laurent et al., 1996; Hildebrand and Shepherd, 1997). Signal processing in the visual system of insects also shares various features to that of mammals (Laughlin, 1981; Hengstenberg, 1991; Strausfeld and Lee, 1991; Land, 1992; O'Carroll, 1993); for example, the computational algorithm for motion detection in insects is the same as that in humans (Adelson and Bergen, 1985; Reichardt, 1987). Olfactory and visual learning in honey bees has features similar to those in mammals (Bitterman and Couvillon, 1991; Bitterman, 1996). The motor systems of arthropods share many features with those of mammals (Camhi, 1993; Pearson, 1993; Selverston et al., 1997; Sparks et al., 1997). Since the brains of arthropods and mammals have been evolved independently from the less-organized head ganglia of common flatworm-like ancestors (Willmer, 1990), these similarities are the result of convergent evolution. Functional convergence among different animal lineages is not surprising considering the similarity of physical environments in which animals survive and of the biological tasks in which animals engage; i.e., finding a habitat, searching for food, avoiding predation, finding a mate, and adapting behavior in response to internal and external environmental changes.

Many fundamental similarities have been found in molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying brain functions among insects and mammals. Examples of these similarities are (1) the presence of homeotic genes that determine the morphology of the body and body parts (Carroll, 1995), (2) the common cellular process for the development of the nervous system (Murphey, 1986), and (3) common molecular basis for learning and memory (Kandel and Abel, 1995; Davis, 1996). Most of these appear to be successional from common ancestors. Arthropod brains are highly accessible to detailed experimental examination and provide useful model systems for the study of common mechanisms underlying brain functions.

CONCLUDING REMARK

One ultimate goal of neurobiology is to elucidate the diversity and evolution of brains throughout the animal kingdom, and elucidation of the arthropod brain will be an important step for achieving this goal. Many unanswered questions remain in this broad field of research. For example, how can the brains of molluscs, which include the most sophisticated brains of all invertebrates (i.e., that of the octopus), be characterized in comparison to the arthropod microbrain and mammalian megalobrain? Elucidation of diversity among animal brains will lead to a deeper understanding of principles related to their design, including those of the human brain.

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. T. Shimozawa and Y. Iwasa for helpful discussion and Dr. T. Itoh for providing materials for Figs. 1 and 2. M. Ohara provided helpful comments. This research was supported by Presto, JST and by the grant in aid from the Ministry of Education, Science, Sports and Culture of Japan.

REFERENCES

1.

E. H. Adelson and J. R. Bergen . 1985. Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. J Opt Soc Am A2:284–299. Google Scholar

2.

J. Alcock 1989. Animal Behavior. Sinauer. Sunderland. Google Scholar

3.

M. E. Bitterman 1996. Comparative analysis of learning in honeybees. Animal Learning Behav 24:123–141. Google Scholar

4.

M. E. Bitterman and P. A. Couvillon . 1991. Failures to find evidence of adaptive specialization in the learning of honeybees. In “The Behavior and Physiology of Bees”. Ed by L. J. Goodman and R. C. Fisher , editors. CAB International. Wallingford. pp. 288–305. Google Scholar

5.

O. Breidbach 1995. Is the evolution of arthropod brain convergent. In “The Nervous Systems of Invertebrates: An Evolutionary and Comparative Approach”. Ed by O. Breidback and W. Kutsch , editors. Birkhäuser. Basel. pp. 383–406. Google Scholar

6.

P. H. Brownell 1991. Structure and function of ventromedial chemosensory systems in two arachnid orders (Scorpionida, Solpugida). Abst 3rd Int Congress Comp Physiol and Biochem. Tokyo. pp. 99. Google Scholar

7.

R. C. Brusca and G. J. Brusca . 1990. Invertebrates. Sinauer. Sunderland. Google Scholar

8.

J. M. Camhi 1993. Neural mechanisms of behavior. Curr Opin Neurobiol 3:1011–1019. Google Scholar

9.

S. B. Carroll 1995. Homeotic genes and the evolution of arthropods and chordates. Nature 376:479–485. Google Scholar

10.

L. Chittka and R. Menzel . 1992. The evolutionary adaptation of flower colours and the insect pollinator's colour vision. J Comp Physiol 171:171–181. Google Scholar

11.

J. D. Currey 1967. The failure of exoskeletons and endoskeletons. J Morph 123:1–16. Google Scholar

12.

R. L. Davis 1996. Physiology and biochemistry of Drosophila learning mutants. Physiol Rev 76:299–317. Google Scholar

13.

E. S. Deevey 1947. Life tables for natural populations of animals. Quart Rev Biol 22:283–314. Google Scholar

14.

Kv Frisch 1967. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Harvard Univ Press. Cambridge. Google Scholar

15.

D. D. Gaffin and P. H. Brownell . 1997. Response properties of chemosensory peg sensilla on the pectines of scorpions. J Comp Physiol 181:291–300. Google Scholar

16.

A. P. Geogopoulos, A. B. Schwartz, and R. E. Kettner . 1986. Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science 233:1416–1419. Google Scholar

17.

A. P. Georgopoulos, R. E. Kettner, and A. B. Schwartz . 1988. Primate motor cortex and free arm movements to visual targets in three-dimensional space. II. Coding of the direction of movement by a neuronal population. J Neurosci 8:2928–2937. Google Scholar

18.

J. L. Gould and C. G. Gould . 1988. The Honey Bee. Sci Am Library. New York. Google Scholar

19.

P. J. Gullan and P. S. Cranston . 1994. The Insects: An Outline of Entomology. Chapman & Hall. London. Google Scholar

20.

H. Ov Hagen 1967. Nachweis einer kinästhetischen Orientierung bei Uca rapax. Z Morph Ökol Tiere 58:301–320. Google Scholar

21.

M. Heisenberg 1998. What do the mushroom bodies do for the insect brain? An introduction. Learning Memory 5:1–10. Google Scholar

22.

R. Hengstenberg 1991. Gaze control in the blowfly Calliphora: a multisensory, two-stage integration process. Neurosciences 3:19–29. Google Scholar

23.

R. Hikosaka and M. Takahata . 1998. Quantitative analyses of anatomical and electrotonic structures of crayfish nonspiking interneurons by three-dimensional morphometry. J Comp Neurol 392:373–389. Google Scholar

24.

J. G. Hildebrand and G. M. Shepherd . 1997. Mechanisms of olfactory discrimination: Converging evidence for common principles across phyla. Annu Rev Neurosci 20:595–631. Google Scholar

25.

G. Hoyle 1976. Approaches to understanding the neurophysiological bases of behavior. In “Simpler Networks and Behavior”. Ed J. C. Fentress , editor. Sinauer. Sunderland. pp. 21–38. Google Scholar

26.

F. Huber 1960. Untersuchungen über die Funktion des Zentralnervensystems und insbesondere des Gehirnes bei der Fortbewegung und der Lauterzeugung der Grillen. Z Vergl Physiol 44:60–132. Google Scholar

27.

M. Iwasaki, M. Mizunami, M. Nishikawa, T. Itoh, and Y. Tominaga . 1999. Ultrastructural analysis of modular subunits in the mushroom bodies of the cockroach. J Elect Microsc 48:55–62. Google Scholar

28.

T. D. Johnston 1982. Selection costs and benefits in the evolution of learning. Adv in the Study of Behav 12:65–106. Google Scholar

29.

E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, and T. M. Jessell . 1991. Principles of Neural Science. Elsevier. New York. Google Scholar

30.

E. Kandel and T. Abel . 1995. Neuropeptides, adenylyl cyclase, and memory storage. Science 268:825–826. Google Scholar

31.

C. E. King 1964. Relative abundance of species and MacArthur's model. Ecology 45:716–727. Google Scholar

32.

L. Kolton and J. M. Camhi . 1995. Cartesian representation of stimulus direction: parallel processing by two sets of giant interneurons in the cockroach. J Comp Physiol 176:691–702. Google Scholar

33.

M. F. Land 1992. Visual tracking and pursuit: humans and arthropods compared. J Insect Physiol 12:939–951. Google Scholar

34.

S. B. Laughlin 1981. Neural principles in the visual system. In “Handbook of Sensory Physiology VII/6B”. Ed H. Autrum , editor. Springer. Berlin. pp. 133–280. Google Scholar

35.

G. Laurent, M. Wehr, and H. Davidowitz . 1996. Temporal representations of odors in an olfactory network. J Neurosci 16:3837–3847. Google Scholar

36.

E. Liebenthal, O. Uhlmann, and J. M. Camhi . 1994. Critical parameters of the spike trains in a cell assembly: coding of turn direction by the giant interneurons of the cockroach. J Comp Physiol 174:281–296. Google Scholar

37.

K. J. Lohmann, N. D. Pentcheff, G. A. Nevitt, G. D. Stetten, R. K. Zimmer-Faust, H. E. Jarrard, and L. C. Boles . 1995. Magnetic orientation of spiny lobsters in the ocean; experiments with undersea coil systems. J Exp Biol 198:2041–2048. Google Scholar

38.

R. M. May 1978. The dynamics and diversity of insect faunas. Symp R Ent Soc Lond 9:188–204. Google Scholar

39.

E. Mayr 1974. Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies. Amer Scientist 62:650–659. Google Scholar

40.

R. Menzel 1990. Learning, memory and “cognition” in honey bees. In “Neurobiology of Comparative Cognition”. Ed by R. P. Kesner and D. S. Olton , editors. Lawrence Erlbaum. Hillsdale. pp. 238–292. Google Scholar

41.

H. Mittelstaedt 1985. Analytical cybernetics of spider navigation. In “Neurobiology of Arachnids”. Ed by F. G. Barth , editor. Springer. Berlin. pp. 298–316. Google Scholar

42.

M. Mizunami, M. Iwasaki, R. Okada, and M. Nishikawa . 1998a. Topography of modular subunits in the mushroom bodies of the cockroach. J Comp Neurol 399:153–161. Google Scholar

43.

M. Mizunami, M. Iwasaki, R. Okada, and M. Nishikawa . 1998b. Topography of four classes of Kenyon cells in the mushroom bodies of the cockroach. J Comp Neurol 399:162–175. Google Scholar

44.

M. Mizunami, R. Okada, Y. Li, and N. J. Strausfeld . 1998c. Mushroom bodies of the cockroach: activity and identities of neurons recorded in freely moving animals. J Comp Neurol 402:501–519. Google Scholar

45.

M. Mizunami, J. M. Weibrecht, and N. J. Strausfeld . 1998d. Mushroom bodies of the cockroach: their participation in place memory. J Comp Neurol 402:520–537. Google Scholar

46.

P. G. Mobbs 1985. Brain structure. In “Comprehensive Insect Physiology, Biochemistry and Pharmacology, Vol 5. Nervous Systems: Structure and Motor Function”. Ed by G. A. Kerkut and L. I. Gilbert , editors. Pergamon. Oxford. pp. 299–370. Google Scholar

47.

R. K. Murphey 1986. The myth of the inflexible invertebrate: competition and synaptic remodelling in the development of invertebrate nervous systems. J Neurobiol 17:585–591. Google Scholar

48.

R. Neder 1959. Allometrisches Wachstum von Hirnteilen bei drei verschieden großen Schabenarten. Zool Jahrb Anat 4:411–464. Google Scholar

49.

M. Nishikawa, F. Yokohari, and T. Ishibashi . 1995. Central projections of the antennal cold receptor neurons and hygroreceptor neurons of the cockroach Periplaneta americana. J Comp Neurol 361:165–176. Google Scholar

50.

D. O'Carroll 1993. Feature-detecting neurons in dragonflies. Nature 362:541–543. Google Scholar

51.

R. Okada, J. Ikeda, and M. Mizunami . 1999. Sensory responses and movement-related activities in extrinsic neurons of the cockroach mushroom bodies. J Comp Physiol 185:115–129. Google Scholar

52.

K. G. Pearson 1993. Common principles of motor control in vertebrates and invertebrates. Annu Rev Neurosci 16:265–297. Google Scholar

53.

T. M. Peters 1988. Insects and Human Society. Van Nostrand Reinhold. New York. Google Scholar

54.

E. R. Pianka 1970. On r and K selection. Amer Natur 104:592–597. Google Scholar

55.

W. Reichardt 1987. Evaluation of optical motion information by movement detectors. J Comp Physiol 161:533–547. Google Scholar

56.

K. Schmidt-Nielsen 1986. Animal physiology. Cambridge Univ Press. Cambridge. Google Scholar

57.

E. A. Seyfarth, R. Hergenröder, H. Ebbes, and F. G. Barth . 1982. Idiothetic orientation of a wandering spider: compensation of detours and estimates of goal distance. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 11:139–148. Google Scholar

58.

A. I. Selverston, Y. V. Panchin, Y. I. Arshavsky, and G. N. Orlovsky . 1997. Shared features of invertebrate central pattern generators. In “Neurons, Networks, and Motor Behavior”. Ed by P. S. G. Stein, S. Grillner, A. I. Selverston, and D. G. Stuart , editors. The MIT press. Cambridge. pp. 105–117. Google Scholar

59.

D. L. Sparks, W. B. Kristan, and B. K. Shaw . 1997. The role of population coding in the control of movement. In “Neurons, Networks, and Motor Behavior”. Ed by P. S. G. Stein, S. Grillner, A. I. Selverston, and D. G. Stuart , editors. The MIT press. Cambridge. pp. 21–32. Google Scholar

60.

M. V. Srinivasan, S. W. Zhang, and H. Zhu . 1998. Honeybees link sights to smells. Nature 396:637–638. Google Scholar

61.

N. J. Strausfeld 1976. Atlas of an Insect Brain. Springer. Berlin. Google Scholar

62.

N. J. Strausfeld 1998. Crustacean-insect relationships: the use of brain characters to derive phylogeny amongst segmented invertebrates. Brain Behav Evol 52:186–206. Google Scholar

63.

N. J. Strausfeld and J. K. Lee . 1991. Neuronal basis for parallel visual processing in the fly. Visual Neurosci 7:13–33. Google Scholar

64.

N. J. Strausfeld, P. Weltzien, and F. G. Barth . 1993. Two visual systems in one brain: neuropils serving the principal eyes of the spider Cupiennius salei. J Comp Neurol 328:63–75. Google Scholar

65.

R. Strauss and M. Heisenberg . 1993. A higher control center of locomotor behavior in the Drosophila brain. J Neurosci 13:1852–1861. Google Scholar

66.

M. Takahata and M. Hisada . 1991. Local nonspiking interneurons as a separate integrator of motoneurons in crayfish. Comp Biochem Physiol 98C:73–78. Google Scholar

67.

R. Wehner 1987. ‘Matched filters’–Neural models of the external world. J Comp Physiol 161:511–531. Google Scholar

68.

R. Wehner 1989. Neurobiology of polarization vision. TINS 12:353–359. Google Scholar

69.

R. Wehner 1992. Arthropods. In “Animal Homing”. Ed by F. Papi , editor. Chapman & Hall. London. pp. 45–144. Google Scholar

70.

R. Wehner, B. Michel, and P. Antonsen . 1996. Visual navigation in insects: coupling of egocentric and geocentric information. J Exp Biol 199:129–140. Google Scholar

71.

N. K. Wessells and J. L. Hopson . 1988. Biology. Random House. New York. Google Scholar

72.

C. A. G. Wiersma and K. Ikeda . 1964. Interneurons commanding swimmeret movements in the crayfish, Procambarus clarki (Girard). Comp Biochem Physiol 12:509–525. Google Scholar

73.

P. Willmer 1990. Invertebrate Relationships: Patterns in Animal Evolution. Cambridge Univ Press. Cambridge. Google Scholar

74.

E. O. Wilson 1988. Biodiversity. Nat Acad Sci press. Washington. Google Scholar

75.

R. Wolf, T. Wittig, K. Liu, G. Wustmann, D. Eyding, and M. Heisenberg . 1998. Drosophyla mushroom bodies are dispensable for visual, tactile and motor learning. Learning Memory 5:166–178. Google Scholar

76.

F. Yokohari 1978. Hygroreceptor mechanism in the antenna of the cockroach Periplaneta. J Comp Physiol 124:53–60. Google Scholar

77.

F. Yokohari 1981. The sensillum capitulum, an antennal hygro- and thermoreceptive sensillum of the cockroach, Periplaneta americana. Cell Tissue Res 216:525–543. Google Scholar

78.

F. Yokohari and H. Tateda . 1976. Moist and dry hygroreceptors for relative humidity of the cockroach, Periplaneta americana L. J Comp Physiol 106:137–152. Google Scholar
Makoto Mizunami, Fumio Yokohari, and Masakazu Takahata "Exploration into the Adaptive Design of the Arthropod “Microbrain”," Zoological Science 16(5), 703-709, (1 October 1999). https://doi.org/10.2108/zsj.16.703
Received: 30 July 1999; Published: 1 October 1999
Back to Top