Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 April 2003 The Mind Through Chick Eyes : Memory, Cognition and Anticipation
Toshiya Matsushima, Ei-Ichi Izawa, Naoya Aoki, Shin Yanagihara
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

To understand the animal mind, we have to reconstruct how animals recognize the external world through their own eyes. For the reconstruction to be realistic, explanations must be made both in their proximate causes (brain mechanisms) as well as ultimate causes (evolutionary backgrounds). Here, we review recent advances in the behavioral, psychological, and system-neuroscience studies accomplished using the domestic chick as subjects. Diverse behavioral paradigms are compared (such as filial imprinting, sexual imprinting, one-trial passive avoidance learning, and reinforcement operant conditioning) in their behavioral characterizations (development, sensory and motor aspects of functions, fitness gains) and relevant brain mechanisms. We will stress that common brain regions are shared by these distinct paradigms, particularly those in the ventral telencephalic structures such as AIv (in the archistriatum) and LPO (in the medial striatum). Neuronal ensembles in these regions could code the chick's anticipation for forthcoming events, particularly the quality/quantity and the temporal proximity of rewards. Without the internal representation of the anticipated proximity in LPO, behavioral tolerance will be lost, and the chick makes impulsive choice for a less optimized option. Functional roles of these regions proved compatible with their anatomical counterparts in the mammalian brain, thus suggesting that the neural systems linking between the memorized past and the anticipated future have remained highly conservative through the evolution of the amniotic vertebrates during the last 300 million years. With the conservative nature in mind, research efforts should be oriented toward a unifying theory, which could explain behavioral deviations from optimized foraging, such as “naïve curiosity,” “contra-freeloading,” “Concorde fallacy,” and “altruism.”

INTRODUCTION TO THE ISSUE OF “ANIMAL MIND”

Do animals have mind? Do non-mammalian vertebrates in particular have mental processes similar to humans? Recent advances in evolutionary (or, comparative) cognitive neuroscience have shown a variety of non-mammalian cases, which suggest common mental processes. Particular attention has been paid to the high cognitive capability of birds. A short list of such outstanding researches includes; visual recognition of subjective contour in barn owls (Nieder and Wagner, 1999), episodic-like memory in food-storing bird (scrub jays) (Clayton and Dickinson, 1998; also see Emery and Clayton, 2001), discrimination of paintings by Picasso and Monet in pigeons (Watanabe et al., 1995; Watanabe, 2001), and verbal communication and Piagetian development of cognition in parrots (Pepperberg, 2002).

One of the possible ideas is that birds have mind similar to us, and the similarity is due to common selective pressures that are shared by birds and humans. The similarity therefore represents an analogy or a homoplasy (footnote 1) due to evolutionary convergence. In other words, they are similar but different from us. Alternative idea is that the physiological constraint is so strong and the brain-mind linkages cannot easily be dissociated. The similarity could therefore represent a homology, and the mental process is deeply rooted in the common Bauplan of our brains. We could therefore argue that they are basically identical to us.

To address this question in a scientifically realistic manner, we have accomplished a series of neuro-behavioral studies to unravel the brain-mind linkages using chicks of the domestic chicken and the Japanese quails. In this review article, we will synthesize our recent findings in close comparisons with their mammalian counterparts. We will focus mostly on the issue of cognitive processes in the domestic chicks, and would rather regret to miss the recent advances in songbird studies; please see reviews (Doupe and Kuhl, 1999; Carr, 2000; Okanoya, 2003). We would also encourage readers to refer to monographs by Vouclair (1996), Rogers (1997), and Hauser (2000) for extensive facts and discussions on the issues of “animal minds.”

BACKGROUNDS: EVOLUTION AND BRAIN ANATOMY OF BIRDS

In this session, as an introductory note, we will briefly review some of the important issues that have long caught attentions of, or even annoyed, the avian neurobiologists; i.e, the evolution of birds and the nomenclatures of brain structures.

Evolution of amniotes

According to the current view of evolutionary relationships among jawed vertebrates, several lines of early aminiotes derived from a common ancestor during the Carboniferous in the Paleozoic era, c.a. 320 million years ago (Carrol, 1988). Amniotes therefore constitute a monophyletic group composed of synapsids (leading to mammlas), diapsids (leading to dinosaurs and birds), and anapsids (leading to extinct reptiles; the linkage to the present turtles is questioned), the classification based on the patterns of temporal openings in the skull as the critical cue. Ancestors of mammals are supposed to date back to the amniotic origin, and showed a massive diversification during the whole period of the Permian. Most groups of the primitive mammals perished at the Permian mass extinction, however, some survived, giving rise to the Triassic cynodonts. Accordingly, all of the late Mesozoic and Cenozoic mammals are supposed to have stemmed from this group. During the era of great reptiles, or dinosaurs, the cynodonts stayed relatively small in their diversity. With their small size and high metabolic activities, shrews-like ancestors survived without major changes until the dawn of the Cenozoic era.

Origin of birds

Origin of modern birds dates back to the Jurassic in the Mesozoic era, about 200 million years ago. The idea that the birds are rooted in the therapod dinosaurs has gained more and more supports from recent fossil records of common features shared by birds and therapods such as wishbone, breastbone, and feathers (Norell et al., 1997; Qiang et al., 1998). Although the intensive and earnest research activities have suffered from a fossil forgery (Zhou et al., 2002), steady lines of evidence have been accumulated for the therapod origin of the modern bird. However, consensus has not yet been reached, and an alternative hypothesis of older origin of the modern birds is still holding.

Bird brain

In accordance with the evolutionary relationships, brain of the amniotic vertebrates share many features in common. Neural organizations of subtelencephalic structures such as spinal cord, medulla oblongata, cerebellum, pons, mesencephalic, and diencephalic structures (optic tectum, tegmentum, thalamic and hypothalamic nuclei) are basically comparable wide among different classes of amniotes (Butler and Hodos, 1996). On the other hand, correspondence of telencephalic structures is much more vague, and has long been debated, seeking for the genuine homological relationships.

Traditional nomenclature

Traditional nomenclature has been used since it was summarized by Ariens-Kappers and his colleagues (1936), but now the presently used terminology has proved to be terribly misleading. For the brain atlases available to date, see Kuenzel and Masson (1988) for the domestic chick, and also see Karten and Hodos (1967) for the pigeon. According to the traditional view, most of the avian telencephalon was equated to sub-regions of the basal ganglia (or striatum) in the mammalian brain, and the nuclei were given names with “striatum” as post-fix; e.g., paleostriatum, archistriatum, neostriatum, and hyperstriatum. Actually, Golgi study (Tömböl et al., 1988a, 1988b; Tömböl, 1995) shows that cytoarchitecture of these avian telencephalic nuclei are somewhat similar to the mammalian striatum.

Genuine homologies

However, data obtained by analyses of embryonic gene expression patterns (Fernandez et al., 1998; Puelles et al., 2000), and detailed neuro-chemical examinations of transmitter and receptor types together with hodological data for neuronal connectivities (Reiner et al., 1998; Durstewitz et al., 1999), revealed that a considerable portion of these “striatal” structures have nothing to do with the mammalian striatum. Instead, structures in the dorsal telencephalon could be homologous to the mammalian cortex (Shimizu, 2001; Medina and Reiner, 2000), even though they lack laminated (layered) architecture and pyramidal neurons characteristic of the mammalian cortex. The ventro-medial telencephalic (sub-pallial) structures, on the other hand, proved to be highly conservative in their neural characters (Reiner et al., 1987; Reiner et al., 1998), thus some of them could deserve the post-fix of “striatum.” It remains still controversial as to whether the major evolutinary changes occurred at the transition from amphibians to the amniotes (Reiner et al., 1998), or at the transition from finned anamniotes to tetrapods (i.e., at the origin of amphibians) (Marín et al., 1998).

Nomenclature reform

The traditional terminology is now under a reform in the contemporary view of the evolution of telencephalon. Organized by E.D. Jarvis (Duke University, USA) and H. Karten (University of California San Diego, USA), comparative neuroanatomists formed a platform called “Avian Brain Nomenclature Exchange” (refer to the website  http://jarvis.neuro.duke.edu/nomen/). We will soon find the final form of the nomenclature report to be published, and most of the avian researchers will follow the proposal. The basic idea underlying the reform is that the inappropriate post-fix “striatum” should be removed, leaving many of the abbreviations unchanged. In this review, we will follow the traditional (and therefore incorrect) terminology, but state the homological relationships to the mammalian counterparts so far as reasonable consensus has been reached.

IMPRINTING

Chicks are born learners. When exposed to a conspicuous moving object for several hours, newly-hatched chicks of precocial birds selectively form a social attachment to that object; the process widely known as filial imprinting. Since it was documented by K. Lorenz, the imprinting has been assumed to be a simple but unique case of recognition learning (see review by Shettleworth, 1998) with many characteristic features; i.e., fixed nature of the sensitive period and irreversibility; for a critical examination of the fixed nature of sensitive period, see Bateson (1979). For comparisons with other forms of learning, see Table 1. We should emphasize that the imprinting is not a passive process in which an exposure to the hen-like object is sufficient. Instead, a behavioral contingency must be established between actions of the subject chick and the imprinting object for an intense preference to be formed (ten Cate, 1986). Similar requirement of social interactions has been pointed out also in the sensory phase of song learning in zebra finches (Houx and ten Cate 1999).

Table 1

Important features of filial imprinting, sexual imprinting, passive avoidance, and reinforcement learning in chicks of precocial birds.

i0289-0003-20-4-395-t01.gif

ARE model

Attempts have been therefore accumulated toward finding common features of imprinting shared by other learning paradigms such as sexual imprinting, operant conditioning and Pavlovian conditioning (see Hollis et al. 1991 for review). Theoretical study using an abstract neural-net model (Analysis-Recognition-Execution model, or ARE model; Bateson and Horn, 1994) has been actually successful in unifying the learning paradigms in terms of common representations shared by distinct learning processes. Different learning paradigms could be understood in terms of distinct combination and distinct changes in connectivities among the presumed sub-processes of A, R, and E. However, biological implementation of the sub-processes (such as A, R, and E) into relevant brain structures remained totally untouched.

Brain mechanisms

Due to the high tractability of ducklings, goslings or domestic chicks as experimental subjects and reproducibility of the learning, the underlying brain mechanisms have been intensively studied in terms of relevant brain regions involved, underlying neurochemical cascades, and accompanying morphometric changes in neural structures (see reviews by Horn, 1985, 1998; Bolhuis and Honey, 1998; Bolhuis, 1999). Research activities have been concentrated on a telencephalic region abbreviated as IMHV (or, intermediate medial hyperstriatum ventrale). Note that the IMHV has nothing to do with the mammalian striatum; readers are rather requested to regard the term “IMHV” as a label for a distinct brain region, instead of “a portion of ventral striatum” that is just incorrect. The IMHV was initially identified as a region where the training procedure of imprinting selectively enhanced the uptake of radio-active uracil (Horn et al., 1979) and also of radioactive 2-Deoxyglucose (Kohsaka et al., 1979). Hodological (tract-tracing) study (Bradley et al., 1985) revealed that the IMHV have reciprocal connections wide with telencephalic structures that include hyperstriatum accessorium (area analogous to the primary visual cortex in mammals; not a “striatal” region) and archistriatum (a complex of structure analogous to the limbic and somato-motor cortices in mammals; also see below), suggesting that the IMHV could function as a site for association of signals issued from multimodal sensory inputs (see also Durstewitz et al., 1999).

Acquisition and retention

Localized lesions placed in the bilateral IMHV (i.e., IMHV regions in both right and left hemispheres) actually proved to prevent the chicks from successful learning in the imprinting paradigm (McCabe et al., 1981); therefore, IMHV is necessary for acquisition. When the IMHV was lesioned soon (within 3 hr) after the imprinting training, on the other hand, the ablated chicks also showed significantly less selective approaches at test accomplished at 24 hr post-training (McCabe et al., 1981); the IMHV is also necessary for retention at least for several hr after the end of training. When the bilateral lesions were made much later (6 hr or afterwards), however, the ablated chicks showed selective approaches at test; the IMHV is no longer required for recall (McCabe et al., 1982).

Permanent and transient storages

In a further series of sequential unilateral IMHV lesions (i.e., the right IMHV was ablated, and subsequently the left IMHV was lesioned, or vise versa), functional laterality has been shown in the involvements of the IMHV in memory formation (for detailed review, see McCabe, 1991). Briefly, the left IMHV is supposed to be a long-term storage site for the imprinting memory, whereas the right IMHV acts as a buffer storage (Cipolla-Neto et al., 1982; also see Bolhuis and Honey 1998). The right IMHV is required for another memory trace to be formed outside of the bilateral IMHV with a considerable delay (6 hr or longer). In other words, the memory traces are supposed to be represented in multiple brain regions, and copies are subsequently delivered from the right IMHV to other regions. The memory trace stored outside of the IMHV is referred to as S′ [S-dash], although its location has not been identified so far. Since the IMHV was assumed to be the major storage site of permanent memory, studies on the neural basis of imprinting have been concentrated on IMHV.

Recently, Nicol, Horn and their colleagues have been successful in analyzing single neuron activities in freely-behaving chicks both during and after the imprinting training (Nicol et al., 1995, 1998; Horn, 1998; Horn et al., 2001). Population of neural correlates of the imprinting object (such as coding of the color and the shape) in IMHV increased as the training proceeded, thus yielding direct evidence for the IMHV as a constituent of the memory system. For the system-level understanding of imprinting, however, we must specify what aspects of behavioral execution the IMHV is responsible for.

Recognition of occluded image and biological motion

Imprinting has been useful also in revealing the cognitive capability of chicks. Selective approaches toward partly occluded imprinting object have suggested that the chicks can utilize the partial visual features for recognition (Regolin and Vallortigara, 1995). Further analysis of orienting behaviors toward a hidden imprinting object has successfully shown that the chicks can maintain the location of invisible (hidden) object for up to 3 min (Vallortigara et al., 1998), similarly to the delayed matching-to-sample task. Object permanence and working memory have, however, not been proved unequivocally in the chick. Chicks can also recognize the imprinting object by its biological motion, or point-light animation sequences depicting a walking hen (Johanson's biological motion; Regolin et al., 2000). All these facts suggest a high degree of similarity in the capability in visual cognition between newly-hatched chicks and humans.

PASSIVE AVOIDANCE LEARNING

Chicks also learn by association. Development of the one-trial passive avoidance task in the domestic chick is credited to Cherkin (1969). This task takes advantage of the innate tendency of chicks (up to 3-5 days post-hatch) to peck at visually conspicuous small objects in a non-selective manner. When a colored bead is presented, chicks repeatedly peck at the bead even when the pecking gives rise to no immediate consequences such as food delivery. Instead, when the bead was soaked in a strong bitter liquid, the chick would peck at the bead once, taste the solution, and show characteristic disgust responses such as head shaking and bill-wiping. Within a few to several tens of min, the chicks become somewhat depressed or inactivated, and even falling in sleep. Afterwards, the chicks recall the visual characteristics of the bead (mostly the color; Aoki et al., 2000), and learn not to peck at the similar beads.

Taste aversion

Passive avoidance learning has some features common with the taste aversion learning (Mazur, 2002); in both cases, the memory is formed only after one-trial experience of association, and the chick learns to avoid the object. However, these two paradigms can be clearly distinguished. In the taste aversion, the subject animal was given a food, and subsequently an intra-peritoneal injection of a LiCl solution that makes the animal feel ill several hr afterwards. The taste-aversion is assumed to represent a case of classical conditioning, with the food acting as a conditioned stimulus, and the illness as an unconditioned stimulus. However, the taste-aversion does not require a strict contingency of events to be associated; the induced illness causes the subject to recall the characteristics of food that was ingested several hours previously. In the passive avoidance, on the other hand, a strict temporal contingency is required between pecking the bead and tasting the bitter liquid; with a delayed delivery of the bitter liquid by only 5 min, chicks failed to form the avoidance memory (M. Aoki and T. Matsushima, unpublished data).

Fitness gains of the chick's high performance in this task could be that the capable chicks have higher chance of survival because they can avoid bitter-tasting, therefore possibly poisonous objects. This argument is however questionable because of the following reasons. First, the bitter taste does not necessarily mean a poisonous food; the taste-aversion paradigm would be much more adaptive in this context. Second, the avoidance memory quickly generalizes within 24 hr after the training, so that the learned chicks would have a risk of avoiding even edible food items with slightly different colors (Aoki et al., 2000).

Common brain mechanisms

The underlying brain mechanisms for the formation of passive avoidance memory have been intensively studied in terms of neurochemical and morphological correlates (see reviews by Rose, 1991, 1995; Rose and Stewart, 1999). Rose's research strategy has been to identify specific changes at the molecular level, which have direct correspondence with the memory formation (Rose, 1993). Most importantly, time course of the changes must be compatible with development of the learning. The passive avoidance task is appropriate in this context, because the memory is established in single and short training trial. Experimenters can thus identify the exact instant when the memory is formed; in the imprinting paradigm, on the other hand, chicks are exposed to the imprinting object for a couple of 1-hr-long training sessions.

Neurochemical approaches

Again, the IMHV proved to be involved in the passive avoidance (Rose and Csillag, 1985; Davies et al., 1988). Learning-specific permanent changes were identified also in another brain region referred to as LPO (lobus parolfactorius) (Stewart et al., 1987; Csillag, 1999). The LPO constitutes the medial part of striatum, that is homologous to a complex of caudate-putamen / nucleus accumbens in mammals. In the IMHV, enhanced metabolic activities immediately after the training leads, through enhanced expression of immediate early genes (c-fos, but also see Yanagihara et al. (2000) for ZENK or zif/268) and expression of late response genes such as those coding cell adhesion molecules (NCAM and L1), to morphological changes in both pre- and post-synaptic structures. The permanent changes in LPO include; increase in the length of thickening of the post-synaptic density (indicative of the active zone) (Stewart and Rusakov, 1995), and enhanced neurogenesis in the post-hatch and post-training period (Dermon et al., 2002). Although not all of these events have been fully understood in their functional roles, the cellular / molecular studies proved to be extraordinarily fruitful when applied to such a simple association learning as passive avoidance.

Underlying cognition

For system-level understanding, however, the passive avoidance task fails to give us few clues for elucidating the neural representations. First, chicks are trained once, and tested once for recall; any neuronal activities recorded in single trials cannot be a basis for reliable functional analyses. Second, memory contents of chicks are too much simplified; chick is either recalling (successfully avoiding the bead) or amnestic (pecking at the bead), without telling how the chick recognized the aversive bead.

SPATIAL MEMORY

Chicks move. Along the movements, visual images on the retinal surface move accordingly. But, it is not the world that moves, but the chick itself. The chick must reconstruct own location in space based on the changes in sensory signals. For the signal conversion, concurrent retinal images are referenced to the memorized images, so that place of the chick in a familiar space is determined. Internally represented reference for the localization is the cognitive map, which is supposed to be one of the universal mental toolkits shared by diverse animals with distinct evolutionary histories, such as desert ants, foraging honeybees, homing pigeons, and migrating salmons (Hauser, 2000).

Right or left

Contemporary researches on the spatial memory in chicks emerged from a psychological study on the right-left asymmetry in position learning, indicative of a functional lateralization of telencephalic hemispheres (Vallortigara and Zanforlin, 1986; Vallortigara et al., 1988). Basically, the subject chicks (1-2 weeks post-hatch) are tested in a rectangular arena, the front wall of which is equipped with a pair of food container boxes. Chicks are introduced from the entrance on the other side of the arena, approach to the boxes, and are requested to peck either one of these two boxes; pecks at the correct box is immediately rewarded by an opening of the box for chicks to gain the food inside (Vallortigara et al., 1996).

Taking an advantage of biased preferences for food items, it has been shown that the chicks memorize both of the content (“what” information) and the position (“where” information) of the boxes (Cozzutti and Vallortigara, 2001), reminiscent of the “episodic-like” memory in jays (Clayton & Dickinson, 1998). Briefly, under a control condition, chicks approaches to the box of their preferred food. When the chicks were fed sufficiently with the preferred food, the satiated chicks would re-orient to the other box, presumably due to the reduced attractiveness of the over-fed food; this process is referred to as “devaluation.”

Center of a place

Further elegant experiments developed by the same group of Italian psychologists revealed that chicks adopt two distinct strategies in spatial localization (Tommasi et al., 1997). In this paradigm, chicks were trained to find a food item hidden at the center of a training arena. The food was initially placed on the surface, and subsequently hidden in the sawdust on the floor. By simply observing the locations where the subject chick scratched the floor in a test arena, experimenters could study how the chick localized the center. The trick is that the test arena differed from the training arena in either the size (with the shape being identical) or the shape (with the size being identical). In order to localize the center, the chicks could utilize either the absolute distance from one wall (local absolute cue), or depend on the equal distance from both of the opposing walls (global relational cue) (Tommasi and Vallortigara, 2000). Surprisingly, the right and the left telencephalic hemispheres differed in localizing strategies; chicks with the operational right hemisphere (with its right eye covered by eye-patch) adopted the global cue, whereas chicks with the left hemisphere (with its left eye covered) searched for food based on the local cue (Tommasi and Vallortigara, 2001). Further unilateral lesion of hippocampus suggested that the global and local cues are separately stored in the right and left hippocampi, respectively (Tommasi et al., 2003).

Position as supplementary cue for association

Position could serve an important cue for the chicks, which depend on seeds and grains scattered unevenly in their foraging ground. In a reinforced concurrent choice task, quail chicks proved to recognize beads primarily by color, and secondarily by position (N. Aoki and T. Matsushima, unpublished); the positional cue appeared operational only when the color cue was no longer available. It will be extremely interesting to see if the IMHV-lesioned chicks (therefore, possibly color-blind subjects; see below) could discriminate objects by the second supplementary positional cues. So far, on the other hand, color-cue dependent object discrimination proved to remain intact in the domestic chicks with bilateral hippocampal lesions (S. Nakajima and T. Matsushima, unpublished data), suggesting a possible double dissociation of neural representations of color and position.

MEMORY OF COLORS AND SHAPES

Chicks depend on vision. All of these paradigms depend on the chick's ability to recognize objects by visual cues. To examine the similarity and differences of visual world between chicks and us, systematic survey has been accomplished.

Color map

With their tetra-chromatic nature of the retinal cone photoreceptor cells (ultra-violet, blue, green, and red; Bowmaker et al., 1997), domestic chicks are supposed to be endowed with acute sense of colors. Visual discrimination task with food reinforcement actually demonstrated that domestic chicks have accurate color memory for foraging (Osorio et al., 1999); the pattern proved much less significant. Basically identical conclusion was drawn in the quail chicks, in which visual memory was examined by selective habituation and passive avoidance task (Aoki et al., 2000). It is to be emphasized that chicks have a context-independent representation of colors. Subjective distance of a green measured from memorized image of a yellow was identical to the distance of the yellow from the green image (Aoki et al., 2000); quail chick could have an internal color map as reference.

Genetic basis of color preference

Experimental manipulations of color perception must be carefully accomplished, because the chicks have innate preference to specific colors and the preference is genetically determined (Kovach, 1980). With traditional genetic selection, Kovach established several lines of quails with innate blue- and red-preference (blue- and red-line). Furthermore, quail chicks can be imprinted to the color opposite to their original preference by simply exposing the subject chicks to the color (Kovach, 1990). It is to be noted, however, that the genetically determined color preference reflects a selective choice for shorter (blue-line) or longer (red-line) wavelength, respectively. When confronted with a concurrent choice between yellow and green, chicks of the blue-line chose green over yellow; in the red-line, on the other hand, the same test revealed yellow preference over green. Innate color preference thus could represent a process, which is distinct from that involved in the color map based discrimination (Aoki et al., 2000).

Neural basis of color discrimination

In parallel with the two distinct processes of color discrimination, two relevant brain regions have been pointed out; a telencephalic region (IMHV) and a subtelencephalic region (dorsomedial thalamus). In a series of lesion experiments in passive avoidance task in domestic chicks, it has been shown that a post-training lesion placed to bilateral IMHV failed to cause amnesia (Gilbert et al., 1991), in contrast to the pre-training lesion experiment (Davies et al., 1988); it was thus concluded that the IMHV is required for acquisition, but not for recall, reminiscent to the functional involvement of the right IMHV in the imprinting (see above; Cipolla-Neto et al., 1982). Further examination of the post-lesion effects revealed, however, that the lesioned chicks avoided the bitter-tasting bead by some (yet unidentified) non-color cues (Patterson and Rose, 1992); memory-based color discrimination was selectively impaired. In contrast, lesion experiments on the innate color preference revealed that even total telencephaloectomy (the whole telencephalon aspirated on the hatching day) does not impair posture, sensorimotor coordination for pecking, locomotion, and selective approach to the genetically preferred color (Kovach and Kabai, 1993). Much smaller lesion localized in the dorsomedial thalamic complex proved to attenuate the genetically determined color preferences (Csillag et al., 1995); lesions to an ascending visual pathway (nucleus rotundus) failed to have effects. Most probably, color is multiply represented in the chick brain, with distinct controls over the behavioral executions.

Shapes

Objects might also be recognized by the shape cue. Actually, the domestic chicks with bilateral IMHV lesions were successful in avoiding the bitter bead by non-color cue(s) as has been described above (Patterson and Rose, 1992); the shape cue was supposed the most plausible candidate for discrimination. However, to date, even intensive examinations failed to reveal the chicks' ability to discriminate objects by shapes in quail chicks (Sakai et al., 2000; Ono et al., 2002). Our inability to reveal the shape recognition might reflect the ecological situation of chicks, which do not depend on the food shape for selective foraging. Another study of visual behavior in the quail chicks (Hayashi et al., 2001) suggested the chick's capability to discriminate conspecific hatchlings by fine plumage patterns; biological motion might be another cue as has been shown in imprinting (Regolin et al., 2000).

NEURONAL REPRESENTATIONS

Brain is full of spikes. But, the neuronal spikes tell us nothing, so long as we are unaware of their codes. To break the codes, we must find the causal link between the sensory signals and the neuron under study, i.e., in a peripheral-to-center approach of the “sensory physiology.” In this approach, we understand how the brain detects specific features of an external stimulus through a cascade of signal processing. We might also search for the causal link between the neural activity and the behavioral execution, i.e., in a center-to-peripheral approach of “motor control.” In this approach, we understand how the brain organizes coordinated behaviors. As the third approach, we could directly penetrate into the mental process that may lie between the (sensory) recognition and the (motor) execution. In this approach, we understand how the brain makes decisions. We adopted the third approach, because it was important and new in the bird researches. In the following, we will describe the task together with some technical tips, and summarize the logical consequences of our recent findings (Yanagihara et al., 2001; Izawa et al., 2001, 2002, 2003).

Reinforcement color discrimination task

Housed in an experimental chamber, the subject chick was presented with a bead (Fig. 1A). The bead was protruded from a hole on a wall for a short period of time (2-4 sec cue-period). The bead was colored either in red, green or blue. When a red bead was presented, chick was required to peck at the bead, and food reward was subsequently delivered after a short delay (2–4 sec reward-period after a 1-sec delay). Red was thus associated with a delayed reward via pecking as operant (rewarded GO). When a green bead was presented, on the other hand, chick was required NOT to peck in order to be rewarded (rewarded NOGO). When a blue bead was presented, chick learned not to peck, because reward was not delivered irrespectively of whether the chick pecked or not (non-rewarded NOGO). This is the basic configuration of the task designed and developed by Yanagihara et al. (2001). In this task, we can clearly dissociate the overall procedure into distinct phases, i.e., perception of color, recall of association memory, execution of operant pecking, anticipation of reward during the delay, recognition of food item, execution of food pecking, and finally ingestion of food.

Fig. 1

Behavioral paradigms developed for the study of cognitive processes underlying foraging behaviors in the domestic chick. A: In the single choice task, the subject chick was presented with a single cue bead, and required either to peck or to stay not pecking. The chick must memorize the association between the color cue (red, green or blue) and the required operant (peck or not peck), as well as the association between the cue and the consequence (i.e., whether a reward is delivered or not). In this manner, sub-processes (such as cue recognition, reward anticipation, and operant behavior) could be experimentally dissociated (Yanagihara et al., 2001). B: In the concurrent choice task, the subject chick was presented with a pair of cue beads, and required to peck either one of them. The consequences (quantity and proximity of rewards) depend on the choice. The chick must memorize the associations as in the case of single choice task. At execution, the chick should recall the associated rewards, anticipate the consequence for each choice, compare them, and finally make a choice. In this manner, anticipation of the temporal proximity of the reward could be experimentally dissociated from that of the reward quantity (Izawa et al., 2003).

i0289-0003-20-4-395-f01.jpg

Single neuron as a “pin-hole”

By a miniature micro-drive mounted on the chick skull together with FET-input buffer amplifiers, we obtained stable extracellular recording of action potentials (spikes) from single neurons continuously for up to 6–9 hr. But, what could a neuron tell us? What do we know by analyzing spiking behaviors of a single neuron, that is truly a “microscopic” entity among millions of similar cells in the whole brain? A simple rationale behind the single-unit analysis could be that we observe the whole brain system through the neuron as a “pin-hole.” Assume that a neuron is connected with a network. We search for positive correlation of the neuronal firing with various behavioral events, and fortunately find a link. For example, the neuron fires in response to a stimulus (light or buzzer) that is given in advance to delivery of reward (food or water). One interpretation is that the neuron codes the “memory-based anticipation of the forthcoming reward.” If we could dissect out the neuron under study and put it in a culture dish, however, the neuron might generate a regular pattern of spikes in isolation, but the spiking would tell us nothing about “anticipation” any longer. Without that neuron, on the other hand, rest of the whole brain would “anticipate” the reward by the associated stimuli, due to redundant organization of the brain. The link between spikes and the code is not an attribute to the neuron; instead, it is an attribute to the whole brain system. For the observer, the neuron operates as a “pin-hole,” and the projected “image” represents the whole relevant process viewed through that neuron. One recorded neuron produces one “image,” and thousand simultaneous neuronal recordings give rise to thousand “images” of the single brain. Thus, our job is to synthesize the brain performance from these thousand of “images.”

Memory correlates in IMHV

Neuronal “pin-hole images” of memory should meet at least the following criteria. First, changes in the neuronal spikes (excitatory or inhibitory) should occur in response to the presentation of associated stimuli in a specific manner. Second, the responses should emerge only after relevant training, showing a good parallelism with the memory retention. Neuronal spikes that meet these criteria have been found in the chick brain in several different regions including the IMHV. Using the imprinting paradigm, it has been established that IMHV contains neuronal correlates of the imprinting memory (Nicol et al., 1995, 1998; Horn et al., 2001). Some IMHV neurons responded specifically to a combination of color AND shape, whereas others showed generalized responses to color OR shape. These authors argued that the IMHV neurons principally represent stored visual features of the imprinting object.

Code of attention in IMHV

In our reinforcement learning paradigm, on the other hand, IMHV neurons responded to wider range of objects, such as rewarding colors as well as novel colors. When habituated, presentation of a familiar color failed to elicit any responses (E-I Izawa et al., 2000). Most probably, these IMHV neurons are related to the chick's subjective “attention,” or what appears conspicuous to the chick. Note that the “attentions” should be generated only after specific experience with the rewarding colors. Similarly, the novelty responses should appear only after the chick had experienced, memorized and recalled a finite number of colors. In this sense, these responses represent the memory, and our interpretation of the IMHV as “attention-generator” fits the memory trace hypothesis. A possibility is not excluded that the memory trace resides somewhere outside of the IMHV, and the IMHV neurons responded secondarily. To localize the memory trace, therefore, we should systematically survey the brain regions that are interconnected with the IMHV; these candidate regions include, e.g., visual Wulst in the dorsal pallium, Arch (arch-istriatum), and LPO.

Anticipation code and “paradox” in LPO

Survey of the task-related neuronal activities in the LPO revealed two important populations of neurons. One group of neurons fired specifically to the visual cues associated with the reward, i.e., those responded in the cue- / delay-periods in both of the rewarded GO and rewarded NOGO trials, but not in the non-rewarded NOGO trials (Yanagihara et al. 2001; E-I Izawa and T Matsushima unpublished). Most probably, these neurons code memory-based anticipation of the forthcoming food reward. Another group of neurons fired when the chick actually gained a reward; a subset of these neurons fired irrespective of whether food or water was gained. Neurons of the second group might represent chicks' subjective evaluation of the gained reward.

What do these codes (anticipation and evaluation) do in the behavioral execution? The evaluation code could be responsible for the formation of novel cue-reward associations. If this were the case, localized LPO lesion should result in an acquisition failure (anterograde amnesia). Otherwise, the anticipation code could be responsible either for selective execution of color-selective pecking. If this were the case, LPO lesion should result in a recall error (retrograde amnesia). In a series of lesion experiments (Izawa et al., 2001, 2002, 2003), we analyzed the effects of pre-training and post-training lesions on a variety of learning paradigms in the domestic chicks; i.e., filial imprinting, passive avoidance learning, water-reinforced color discrimination task, food-reinforced GO-NOGO task, and food-reinforced concurrent choice task. For the imprinting, both of pre- and post-training lesions caused no effects. For the reinforcement learning, similar LPO lesions caused severe deficiency in the acquisition, whereas the learned associates were recalled without difficulties. Therefore, the evaluation code in LPO could actually have a role. For the GONOGO color discrimination task, similarly, the LPO lesion caused anterograde amnesia, but no retrograde amnesia. Here arises a “paradox.” The anticipation code is formed in LPO after the training, however, the LPO does not seem to be required for execution of selective pecking. Without the memorized code, how could chicks execute the correct operant pecking?

Impulsiveness and behavioral tolerance

One possible way to account for the “paradox” is to assume that the anticipation code in LPO is responsible for some other functions than behavioral execution per se. Alternative account for the “paradox,” though not exclusive, is that the anticipation code is multiply represented in various regions of the brain, and the lesion localized in LPO failed to interfere with the link between the color and pecking.

In concurrent choice task (Fig. 1B), post-training LPO lesion actually had an effect. In this task, chicks learned to peck one of two simultaneously presented beads, e.g., red and yellow. Red bead was associated with a large reward (6 pellets of millet grain), and yellow with a small reward (1 pellet). Naturally, chicks learned to peck the red bead to gain the large reward. The choice differed when the large reward was delivered with a delay of 1–3 sec. When the red bead was associated with a large but delayed reward (delay time of 3 sec), the chick learned to choose the yellow bead to gain the small reward. For a delay of 1–2 sec, chicks proved to be patient enough to wait for the large reward, just staring at the still empty food tray. Chemical lesion of LPO (particularly the posterior LPO) ablated the chick-sized patience, thus unmasking the underlying impulsiveness; no amnesia accompanied the lesion. With the anticipation code in LPO neurons, the future gain would be “guaranteed” so that the chick reasonably suppressed the impulsive action for immediate reward. In other words, past experiences yield an internal representation of the future reward in LPO, and the represented reward makes the chick behaviorally tolerant. At the neuronal level, we can further assume that the anticipation-coding neurons in LPO could be responsible specifically for temporal proximity, rather than quantity and quality of the reward; the quantity and quality of anticipated reward should be represented somewhere else in the chick brain. Note that the system for the patient choice is highly adaptive, because the net gain (i.e., total amount of food obtained) could be optimized, thus serving for a rapid growth and a higher chance of survival (see Table 1).

Alternative code of anticipation in AIv

Archistriatum located in the ventro-lateral telencephalon might be in charge. In particular, the AIv region (a ventral subdivision of intermediate archistriatum) has reciprocal connections with IMHV, and a massive efferent projection to LPO (Székely et al., 1994), thus could play an important role as relay center for the memory formation (Davies et al., 1997; Csillag, 1999). Actually, localized lesions to the Arch are reported to prevent memory formation in passive avoidance learning (Lownders and Davies, 1994) as well as in imprinting (Lownders et al., 1994). The “paradox” of LPO functions (see above) might thus be explained by assuming an alternative code of cue-reward association in the Arch. Actually, single-unit recordings revealed a population of Arch neurons that responded specifically to the cues associated with reward (Aoki et al., 2002, 2003). We should examine whether these Arch neurons code aspects of the anticipation, i.e., quality and / or quality of the forthcoming reward, rather than temporal proximity.

TOWARD A SYNTHESIS: A BIOLOGICAL IMPLEMENTATION OF THE ARE MODEL

With the present data available, we can reasonably formulate a working hypothesis on the functional network; block diagram shown in Fig. 2 show the basic model. This model recapitulates some important features of the ARE model proposed for the imprinting (Bateson and Horn, 1994), and the functional network proposed for passive avoidance learning (Csillag, 1999).

Fig. 2

Biological implementation of the Analysis (A) – Recognition (R) – Execution (E) model proposed by Bateson and Horn (1994) with an emphasis on the execution of foraging behaviors. Units within the A-layer are multiply interconnected with the R-layer units, and the R-layer units further with the E-layer units. Connections shown in this figure (arrows) are based on hodological data in the domestic chicks (Csillag et al., 1994; Székely et al., 1994; Davies et al., 1997; Csillag, 1999). Note also that the optic tectum in the E-layer project massively to the ectostriatum in the A-layer (connection not indicated). Brain regions involved in the A-layer are responsible for coding elements of visual features (i.e., colors, movements, patterns and spatial relationships). Regions in the R-layer could act as an “attention filter” (IMHV) in which conspicuous novel / alerting object is separated, or as sites (AIv and LPO) in which “anticipation” of the future consequences is generated on the basis of memorized past. The caudal LPO is specifically involved in the anticipated proximity of reward, whereas the AIv could be responsible for other aspects (quantity and/or quality). The AIv and the LPO could exert opposing actions onto the E-layer units as has been suggested by Csillag (1999); the AIv activates the E-layer units and facilitates turning / orienting behavior to the cue object, whereas the LPO suppresses the E-layer units and blocks impulsive actions. Optic tectum and reticular formation are supposed to be responsible for the spatial localization and targeted movements, whereas the tegmental VTA/SN could be involved in characterizing the attributes such as appetitive or aversive reinforcements. Possible involvement of paleostriatal complex (or, lateral striatum) is not included in this diagram. See text for further explanations.

i0289-0003-20-4-395-f02.jpg

Basically, the system is composed of 3 layers; layer of Analysis modules (A-layer), Recognition modules (R-layer), and Execution modules (E-layer). The A-layer is composed of Wulst, neostriatum (particularly its caudo-lateral part), ectostriatum, and hippocampal complex; these regions are mutually interconnected. The R-layer is composed of IMHV, AIv, and LPO (particularly its caudal part, cLPO). The E-layer is composed of optic tectum, cerebellum, reticular formation, and dopaminergic system (accompanied by nondopaminergic SN subregions). Similarly to the ARE model of Bateson and Horn, the A-layer send signals to the E-layer directly and indirectly with a relay of the R-layer. As another important feature, dopaminergic system is incorporated as superviser for memory formation to be made in LPO, arch-istriatum (its dorsal part), and neostriatum (dorsolateral region, in particular).

For the filial imprinting, LPO and tegmental dopaminergic nuclei (VTA and SN) are not involved, and the memory formation is accomplished mainly by the Hebbian type synaptic plasticity within the IMHV (Matsushima and Aoki, 1995; Yanagihara et al., 1998). The role of the IMHV could be to associate visual features of the imprinting object scattered wide in the modules of the A-layer. Reciprocal connectivities between IMHV and Arch (Csillag et al., 1994; Davies et al., 1997; Csillag, 1999) could be responsible for the emotional control of imprinting (ten Cate, 1986), although the idea to equalize the whole Arch to the mammalian amygdala (Aoki et al., 2002) remains highly questionable. Most probably, the selective attachment and approaching behaviors could be executed by way of the direct descending system from Arch; actually a population of the Arch neurons proved to selectively code the cued movements, particularly those cued by the auditory stimuli (Aoki et al., 2003).

For the passive avoidance learning, the same set of modules in the A-layer and the R-layer are involved as in the imprinting. Additional process is that the gustatory inputs contribute to the memory formation, probably via the dopaminergic control from tegmental nuclei to LPO (Stewart et al., 1996). The memory formation might be performed by either (or both) of the plastic processes in IMHV and in LPO (Matsushima et al., 2001), though neither one of these regions could be the principal site for permanent storage of memory, as has been discussed above. Taste aversion (caused by delayed illness) might be performed in the same assembly of networks, however the relevant neural mechanisms are not yet evident. Execution of the passive avoidance should be accomplished by a selective suppression of visuo-motor responses within the optic tectum through the brainstem reticular formation.

For the reinforced GO/NOGO color discrimination task, the functional roles of LPO and the tegmental dopaminergic neurons could be most significant (Yanagihara et al., 2001; Izawa et al., 2001, 2002, 2003). Actually, our preliminary exploration revealed a neural code of reward within the VTA (Izawa E-I, Matsushima T, unpublished); these VTA neurons started to fire upon the presentation of food reward, and then started to fire at a high rate immediately after the chick actually gained food. Most probably, VTA neurons signal the reward, and gate the dopamine D1-receptor dependent synaptic plasticity within the LPO (Matsushima et al., 2001). As to the execution, however, the final motor regions are not yet identified in the telencephalon, except that some AIv neurons coded preparatory activities selectively for the cued turning movements toward the target. Despite our efforts, we are still unable to identify pecking-relevant command signals within archistriatum and striatum (Aoki et al., 2003); lateral striatum (or, paleostriatum augmentatum; homologous to the mammalian caudate-putamen) together with the pallidum (or, paleostriatum primitivum) might be involved (not shown in Fig. 2). Sensori-motor coordination of targeted movements at the bead could be accomplished within the optic tectum. Definitely, we need further intensive studies for fully understanding how the system works as a whole.

SCOPES

With these findings in the chick brain and behavior in hand, we can make a list of future research topics. To address these topics, we will have to find novel behavioral paradigms in novel bird models, other than the domestic chicks discussed in this review.

“Observation learning”: a social transmission

Chicks could learn also by observation. In addition to the own experiences of pecking and tasting as described above, the pecking preference can be socially transmitted from hens to day-old chicks (Suboski and Bartashunas, 1984). Even a motor-driven arrow-shaped paper model, that moved its taper pointing to a colored bead, could tell a chick which object to peck. The chick subsequently pecked at the “instructed” bead object, even after the arrow-operation was removed. Authors argued that “information about the visual characteristics of food objects” could be transmitted from hens to chicks by the same process. Similar transmission of pecking selectivity is reported in the one-trial passive avoidance (Johnston et al., 1998). Just by observing another individual pecking at a bitter bead, and subsequently showing disgust responses, day-old subject chicks learned not to peck at the same bead when tested afterwards. This finding is reminiscent of the finding in monkeys, in which a lasting phobia of snakes developed by observing another individual's fearful reactions to a snake (Mineka, Davidson, Cook and Carr, 1984; cited by Mazur, 2002). Beside the well-documented effects of social context (being observed by other individuals) on the re-cashing behavior in scrub jays (Emery and Clayton, 2001), chicks might also be endowed with a high ability to actively learn by observations. Development of a novel paradigm tractable for system neuroscience will enable us to penetrate into many interesting issues, such as how chicks observe others, how chicks convert the observed events into own behavioral rules, and what neural mechanisms are responsible for the conversion.

Deviations from optimal foraging: “naïve curiosity,” “contra-freeloading,” “Concorde fallacy,” and “altruism”

Chicks might be wise enough to actively “earn” information at the expense of immediate material benefits. In our controlled laboratory condition, week-old chicks are trained and tested under a limited diet so that the chick's motive toward food reward is maximized. Consequently, chicks quickly learn the association between cue colors and reward quantity, so that chicks reliably choose a color associated with a larger reward (Izawa et al., 2003). In this context, chicks behave in accordance with the most normative theory of the optimal foraging (Alcock, 2001) with a slight modification that anticipated reward in the future should weigh proportionately less than the immediate gain. Internal representation of the anticipated future plays a critical role.

The situation somewhat differs in day-old chicks. They are much more curious, pecking non-selectively at a variety of conspicuous objects they encounter. Within the initial 3–4 days post-hatch, chicks survive by the yolk reserve and do not depend on food. During this period, chicks have to make up an internal directory of edible foods and non-edible objects of similar but distinct appearance such as gravels or ground debris. “Naïve curiosity,” or an eagerness for information in the limited post-hatch period could play a critical role, serving a biological basis for the passive avoidance learning and the reinforcement tasks.

Similar deviation from the immediate optimization can be found in adult birds, which often work (i.e., pay behavioral “cost”) for food even when the same food items can be freely available; a process known as “contra-freeloading” (Inglis et al., 1997). The “contra-freeloading” has been reported in a variety of vertebrate species, including fish (Betta splendens), pigeons, domestic chicks, crows, star-lings, rats, monkeys, chimpanzee, and humans. In this context, it is argued that animals have “a hunger for information,” and a more information gain could offset the extra cost to be paid now, so long as the immediate need for food is not so great.

In European starlings, it is further reported that the cost that had been paid for gaining food reward increased the preference in choice condition (Kacelnik and Marsh, 2002), in a clear contrast to the consequence predicted by the optimization theory. The authors claim that they can relate their finding of the behavioral “perversity” in birds to a phenomenon known as “Concorde fallacy,” in which a behavioral choice is biased toward a recipient of big efforts in previous history, just as the maladaptive investments by developers to the supersonic airplanes Concorde that simply did not pay. Though it is difficult to separate the effects of investment in the past and the effects of anticipated gain in the future, a plausible explanation is that the past record of investment is a reliable measure for estimation of future gains in most of the ecologically realistic circumstances, and the fallacy could represent a maladaptive side effect.

Some cases of “altruism” could constitute still another example of deviation from the optimal foraging. When an indirect fitness gain is available, animals often invest material benefits to genetically related individuals as has been demonstrated in the Florida scrub jays (Woolenfenden 1974, cited by Wilson, 1975). The choice by helpers in this context is the one between giving food to others and ingesting it by oneself. We can assume a similar proximate mechanism for the “altruistic” choice, to the one found in the anticipation codes of chick LPO. The scrub jay helpers could suppress the option of own ingesting, probably after developing an internal representation of the benefits available by the alternative option of giving. Future researches by system neuroscience might be successful in revealing the internal representation, a mental representation comparable to our ethical self-control or the Freudian super-ego.

In summary, evolution of these behavioral variations such as “naïve curiosity,” “contra-freeloading,” “Concorde fallacy,” and “altruism” should be examined, in concert with the accounts by behavioral ecology, toward understanding the responsible brain mechanisms as targets of the selection pressures. Definitely, the telencephalic structures (limbic system and striatal complex) involved in cognitive processes (memory, evaluation, anticipation, and decision making) should be the sites for the future researches. These processes could be understood as deviations from the gain optimization, rather than assuming distinct centers of “instincts.” The issue of “animal mind” could be agued most fruitfully, if approaches of the system neuro-science are thus synthesized with the evolutionary perspectives.

Acknowledgments

This series of studies have been supported by grants-in-aid for scientific research to T.M. from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, from Japanese Ministry of Education, Science, Culture, Sports, Science and Technologies, from Takeda Science Foundation, and from Daiko Foundation. Encouraging and highly intriguing discussions with Drs. Andras Csillag (Semmelweis University, Hungary), Peter Kabai (St Istvan University, Hungary), Kazuo Okanoya (Chiba University, Japan), Giorgio Vallortigara (University of Trieste, Italy) and Onur Güntürkün (University of Bochum, Germany) should also be appreciated.

Notes

[1] 1. Analogy simply implies that animals with distinct phylogenetic histories share a similar trait. For example, wings of birds and insects are analogous; evolutionary origins are not relevant in this context. The similarity can either be functional or morphological in nature. Homoplasy implies, on the other hand, that similar traits emerged from a common ancestor, but the phylogenetic development occurred independently in these animals under comparison. For example, wings of birds and bats are homoplastic, since both have similar function as “flying organ”, but emerged independently from forelimbs of the tetrapoda. Homology indicates that a trait is shared because the trait was inherited from the common ancestor. For example, wings of birds and Archeopteryx (a Mesozoic bird-like reptile) are homologous because they derived from a group of extinct feathered therapod dinosaurs. The disctinction between homology and homoplasy can be made only on the basis of cladistic analysis of related animals groups, which enables us to reconstruct features of the extinct ancestors. These basic concepts are perfectly applicable also for the structures and functions of brain and behaviors; for further discussions, see Shimizu (2001).

REFERENCES

1.

J. Alcock 2001. Animal Behavior: an evolutionary approach, 7th ed.. Sinauer Associates. Sunderland, MA, USA. Google Scholar

2.

M. Aoki, E-I. Izawa, K. Koga, S. Yanagihara, and T. Matsushima . 2000. Accurate visual memory of colors in controlling the pecking behavior of quail chicks. Zool Sci 17:1053–1059. Google Scholar

3.

N. Aoki, E-I. Izawa, J. Naito, and T. Matsushima . 2002. Representation of memorized color in the intermediate ventral archistriatum (amygdala homologue) of domestic chicks. Abstract: in the Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, November 2002. Orland. FL. USA. Google Scholar

4.

M. Aoki, E-I. Izawa, S. Yanagihara, and T. Matsushima . 2003. Neural correlates of memorized associations and cued movements in archistriatum of the domestic chick. Eur J Neurosci in press. Google Scholar

5.

C. U. Ariens-Kappers, G. C. Huber, and E. C. Crosby . 1936. The comparative anatomy of the nervous system of vertebrates, including man. Vol.1–3.Hafner Publishing Co. New York. reprinted in 1967. Google Scholar

6.

P. Bateson 1979. How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? Anim Behav 27:470–486. Google Scholar

7.

P. P. G. Bateson 1982. Preferences for cousins in Japanese quail. Nature 295:236–237. Google Scholar

8.

P. Bateson and G. Horn . 1994. Imprinting and recognition memory: a neural net model. Anim Behav 48:695–715. Google Scholar

9.

A. B. Butler and W. Hodos . 1996. Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy, evolution and adaptation. Wiley-Liss Inc. New York. Google Scholar

10.

J. J. Bolhuis 1999. Early learning and the development of filial preferences in the chick. Behav Brain Res 98:245–252. Google Scholar

11.

J. J. Bolhuis and R. C. Honey . 1998. Imprinting, learning and development: from behaviour to brain and back. Trends Neurosci 21:306–311. Google Scholar

12.

J. K. Bowmaker, L. A. Heath, S. E. Wilkie, and D. M. Hunt . 1997. Visual pigments and oil droplets from six classes of photoreceptors in the retinas of birds. Vision Res 37:2183–2194. Google Scholar

13.

P. Bradley, D. C. Davies, and G. Horn . 1985. Connection of hyperstriatum ventrale of the domestic chick (Gallus domesticus). J Anat 140:577–589. Google Scholar

14.

C. Carr 2000. Locating an error correction signal for adult birdsong. Nature Neurosci 3:419–421. Google Scholar

15.

R. L. Carrol 1988. Vertebrate paleontology and evolution. W.H. Freeman and Co. New York. Google Scholar

16.

J. Cipolla-Neto, G. Horn, and B. J. McCabe . 1982. Hemispheric asymmetry and imprinting: the effect of sequential lesions to the hyperstriatum ventrale. Exp Brain Res 48:22–27. Google Scholar

17.

A. Cherkin 1969. Kinetics of memory consolidation: role of amnesic treatment parameters. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 63:1094–1101. Google Scholar

18.

J. J. Cherfas 1977. Prior exposure to light improves avoidance learning in day-old domestic chicks. Anim Behav 25:732–735. Google Scholar

19.

N. S. Clayton and A. Dickinson . 1998. Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395:272–274. Google Scholar

20.

C. Cozzutti and G. Vallortigara . 2001. Hemispheric memories for the content and position of food caches in the domestic chick. Behav Neurosci 115:305–313. Google Scholar

21.

A. Csillag 1999. Striato-telencephalic and striato-tegmental circuits: relevance to learning in domestic chicks. Behav Brain Res 98:227–236. Google Scholar

22.

A. Csillag, A. D. Székely, and D. C. Davies . 1994. Termination pattern of medial hyperstriatum ventrale efferents in the archistriatum of the domestic chick. J Comp Neurol 348:394–402. Google Scholar

23.

A. Csillag, P. Kabai, and J. K. Kovach . 1995. Effects of diencephalic lesions on approach responses and color preferences in quail. Physiol Behav 58:659–667. Google Scholar

24.

D. C. Davies, D. A. Taylor, and M. H. Johnson . 1988. The effects of hyperstriatal lesions on one-trial passive avoidance learning in the chick. J Neurosci 8:4662–4666. Google Scholar

25.

D. C. Davies, A. Csillag, A. D. Székely, and P. Kabai . 1997. Efferent connections of the domestic chick archistriatum: a Phaseolus lectin anterograde tracing study. J Comp Neurol 389:679–693. Google Scholar

26.

C. R. Dermon, B. Zikopoulos, L. Panagis, E. Harrison, C. L. Lancashire, R. Mileusnic, and M. G. Steward . 2002. Passive avoidance training enhances cell proliferation in 1-day-old chicks. Eur J Neurosci 16:1267–1274. Google Scholar

27.

A. Doupe and P. Kuhl . 1999. Birdsong and human speech: common themes and mechanisms. Annu Rev Neurosci 22:567–631. Google Scholar

28.

D. Durstewitz, S. Kröner, and O. Güntürkün . 1999. The dopaminergic innervation of the avian telencephalon. Prog Neurobiol 59:161–195. Google Scholar

29.

N. J. Emery and N. S. Clayton . 2001. Effects of experience and social context on prospective cashing strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414:443–446. Google Scholar

30.

A. S. Fernandez, C. Pieau, J. Repérant, E. Boncinelli, and M. Wassef . 1998. Expression of the Emx-1 and Dlx-1 homeobox genes define three molecularly distinct domains in the telencephalon of mouse, chick, turtle and frog embryos: implications for the evolution of telencephalic subdivisions in amniotes. Development 125:2099–2111. Google Scholar

31.

D. B. Gilbert, T. A. Patterson, and S. P. R. Rose . 1991. Dissociation of brain sites necessary for registration and storage of memory for a one-trial passive avoidance task in the chick. Behav Neurosci 105:553–561. Google Scholar

32.

M. D. Hauser 2000. Wind Minds, what animals really think. Henry Holt & Co. New York. Google Scholar

33.

I. Hayashi, Y. Ono, and T. Matsushima . 2001. Visual cues for suppressing isolation-induced distress calls in quail chicks. Zool Sci 18:1065–1071. Google Scholar

34.

K. L. Hollis, C. ten Cate, and P. Bateson . 1991. Stimulus representation: a subprocess of imprinting and conditioning. J Comp Psychol 105:307–317. Google Scholar

35.

G. Horn 1985. Memory, Imprinting, and the Brain. Oxford Psychology Series No.10. Clarendon Press. Oxford. Google Scholar

36.

G. Horn 1998. Visual imprinting and the neural mechanisms of recognition memory. Trends Neurosci 21:300–305. Google Scholar

37.

G. Horn, B. J. McCabe, and P. P. G. Bateson . 1979. An autoradiographic study of the chick brain after imprinting. Brain Res 168:361–367. Google Scholar

38.

G. Horn, A. U. Nicol, and M. W. Brown . 2001. Tracking memory's trace. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:5282–5287. Google Scholar

39.

B. B. Houx and C. ten Cate . 1999. Song learning from playback in zebra finches: Is there an effect of operant contingency? Anim Behav 57:837–845. Google Scholar

40.

I. R. Inglis, B. Forkman, and J. Lazarus . 1997. Free food or earned food? A review and fuzzy model of contrafreeloading. Anim Behav 53:1171–1191. Google Scholar

41.

E-I. Izawa, S. Yanagihara, K. Koga, and T. Matsushima . 2000. Representation of memory, attention, expectancy in chick telencephalon (IMHV/LPO): Single unit analysis. Abstract: in the Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, November 2000. New Orleans. USA. Google Scholar

42.

E-I. Izawa, S. Yanagihara, T. Atsumi, and T. Matsushima . 2001. The role of basal ganglia in reinforcement learning and imprinting in domestic chicks. NeuroReport 12:1743–1747. Google Scholar

43.

E-I. Izawa, G. Zachar, N. Aoki, K. Koga, and T. Matsushima . 2002. Lesions of the ventro-medial basal ganglia impair the reinforcement but not the recall of memorized color discrimination in domestic chicks. Behav Brain Res 136:405–414. Google Scholar

44.

E-I. Izawa, G. Zachar, S. Yanagihara, and T. Matsushima . 2003. Localized lesion of caudal part of lobus parolfactorius caused impulsive choice in the domestic chick: evolutionarily conserved function of ventral striatum. J Neurosci 23:1894–1902. Google Scholar

45.

A. N. B. Johnston, T. H. J. Burne, and S. P. R. Rose . 1998. Observation learning in day-old chicks using a one-trial passive avoidance learning paradigm. Anim Behav 56:1347–1353. Google Scholar

46.

A. Kacelnik and B. Marsh . 2002. Cost can increase preference in star-lings. Anim Behav 63:245–250. Google Scholar

47.

H. J. Karten and W. Hodos . 1967. A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Brain of the Pigeon (Columba livia). Johns Hopkins University Press. Baltimore. Google Scholar

48.

S. Kohsaka, K. Takamatsu, E. Aoki, and Y. Tsukada . 1979. Metabolic mapping of chick brain after imprinting using [14C] 2-deoxyglucose technique. Brain Res 172:539–544. Google Scholar

49.

J. K. Kovach 1980. Mendelian units of inheritance control visual preferences in quail chicks (C. coturnix japonica). Science 207:549–551. Google Scholar

50.

J. K. Kovach 1990. Nonspecific imprintability of quail to colors: response to artificial selection. Behav Genet 20:91–96. Google Scholar

51.

J. K. Kovach and P. Kabai . 1993. Effects of bilateral hemispherectomy on genetically variable stimulus preferences and imprinting in quail chicks. Brain Res 629:181–186. Google Scholar

52.

W. J. Kuenzel and M. Masson . 1988. A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Brain of the Chick (Gallus domesticus). Johns Hopkins University Press. Baltimore. Google Scholar

53.

M. Lowndes and D. C. Davies . 1994. The effects of archistriatal lesions on one-trial passive avoidance learning in the chick. Eur J Neurosci 6:525–530. Google Scholar

54.

M. Lowndes, D. C. Davies, and M. H. Johnson . 1994. Archistriatal lesions impair the acquisition of filial preferences during imprinting in the domestic chick. Eur J Neurosci 6:1143–1148. Google Scholar

55.

O. Marín, W. J. A. J. Smeets, and A. González . 1998. Evolution of the basal ganglia in tetrapods: a new perspective based on recent studies inamphibians. Trends Neurosci 21:487–494. Google Scholar

56.

T. Matsushima and K. Aoki . 1995. Potentiation and depotentiation of DNQX-sensitive fast excitatory synaptic transmission in telencephalone of the quail chick. Neurosci Lett 185:179–182. Google Scholar

57.

T. Matsushima, E-I. Izawa, and S. Yanagihara . 2001. D1-receptor dependent synaptic potentiation in the basal ganglia of quail chicks. NeuroReport 12:2831–2837. Google Scholar

58.

J. E. Mazur 2002. Learning and Behavior, 5th ed.. Prentice Press. Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA. Google Scholar

59.

B. J. McCabe 1991. Hemispheric asymmetry of learning-induced changes. In “Neural and Behavioural Plasticity”. Ed by R. J. Andrew Oxford University Press. Oxford. pp. 262–276. Google Scholar

60.

B. J. McCabe, G. Horn, and P. P. G. Bateson . 1981. Effects of restricted lesion of the chick forebrain on the acquisition of filial preferences during imprinting. Brain Res 205:29–37. Google Scholar

61.

B. J. McCabe, J. Cipolla-Neto, G. Horn, and P. P. G. Bateson . 1982. Amnestic effects of bilateral lesions placed in the hyperstriatum ventrale of the chick after imprinting. Exp Brain Res 48:187–193. Google Scholar

62.

L. Medina and A. Reiner . 2000. Do birds possess homologues of mammalian primary visual, somatosensory and motor cortices? Trends Neurosci 23:1–12. Google Scholar

63.

A. U. Nicol, M. W. Brown, and G. Horn . 1995. Neurophysiological investigation of a recognition memory system for imprinting in the domestic chick. Eur J Neurosci 7:766–776. Google Scholar

64.

A. U. Nicol, M. W. Brown, and G. Horn . 1998. Neural encoding of subject-object distance in a visual recognition system. Eur J Neurosci 10:34–44. Google Scholar

65.

A. Nieder and H. Wagner . 1999. Perception and neuronal coding of subjective contours in the owl. Nature Neurosci 2:660–663. Google Scholar

66.

M. A. Norell, P. Makovicky, and J. M. Clark . 1997. A Velociraptor wishbone. Nature 389:447. Google Scholar

67.

K. Okanoya 2003. Finite-state syntax in Bengalese finch songs: Timbergen's four questions asked. In “Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 32”. Ed by J. B. Slater, et al Academic Press. New York. Google Scholar

68.

Y. Ono, I. Hayashi, and T. Matsushima . 2002. Visual memory of shapes in quail chicks: discrimination among 2-dimensional objects. Zool Sci 19:719–725. Google Scholar

69.

D. Osorio, C. D. Jones, and M. Vorobyev . 1999. Accurate memory for colour but not pattern contrast in chicks. Curr Biol 9:199–202. Google Scholar

70.

T. A. Patterson and S. P. R. Rose . 1992. Memory in the chick: multiple cues, distinct brain locations. Behav Neurosci 106:465–470. Google Scholar

71.

I. M. Pepperberg 2002. In search of King Solomon's ring: Cognitive and communicative studies of Grey parrots. Brain Behav Evolut 59:54–67. Google Scholar

72.

L. Puelles, E. Kuwana, E. Puelles, A. Bulfone, K. Shimamura, J. Keleher, S. Smiga, and J. L. R. Rubenstein . 2000. Pallial and subpallial derivatives in the embryonic chick and mouse telencephalon, traced by the expression of the genes Dlx-2, Emx-1, Nkx-2.1, Pax-6, and Tbr-1. J Comp Neurol 424:409–438. Google Scholar

73.

J. Qiang, P. J. Currie, M. A. Norell, and J. Shu-an . 1998. Two feathered dinosaurs from northeastern China. Nature 393:753–761. Google Scholar

74.

L. Regolin and G. Vallortigara . 1995. Perception of partly occluded objects by young chicks. Percept Psychophys 57:971–976. Google Scholar

75.

L. Regolin, L. Tommasi, and G. Vallortigara . 2000. Visual perception of biological motion in newly hatched chicks as revealed by an imprinting procedure. Anim Cogn 3:53–60. Google Scholar

76.

A. Reiner, S. E. Brauth, and H. J. Karten . 1987. Evolution of the amniote basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci 7:320–325. Google Scholar

77.

A. Reiner, L. Medina, and C. L. Veenman . 1998. Structural and functional evolution of the basal ganglia in vertebrates. Brain Res Rev 28:235–285. Google Scholar

78.

L. J. Rogers 1997. Minds of Their Own, thinking and awareness in animals. Allen & Unwin. Google Scholar

79.

S. P. R. Rose 1991. How chicks make memories: the cellular cascade from c-fos to dendritic remodeling. Trends Neurosci 14:390–397. Google Scholar

80.

S. Rose 1993. The Making of Memory, from molecules to mind. Bantam Press. London. Google Scholar

81.

S. P. R. Rose 1995. Cell-adhesion molecules, glucocorticoids and long-term memory formation. Trends Neurosci 18:502–506. Google Scholar

82.

S. P. R. Rose and A. Csillag . 1985. Passive avoidance training results in lasting changes in deoxyglucose metabolism in left hemisphere regions of chick brain. Behav Neural Biol 44:315–324. Google Scholar

83.

S. P. R. Rose and M. Stewart . 1999. Cellular correlates of stages of memory formation in the chick following passive avoidance training. Behav Brain Res 98:237–243. Google Scholar

84.

S. Sakai, S. Yanagihara, P. Kabai, K. Koga, and T. Matsushima . 2000. Pre-disposed visual memory of shapes in quail chicks. Zool Sci 17:1045–1051. Google Scholar

85.

S. J. Shettleworth 1998. Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior. Oxford University Press. Oxford. pp. 155–165. Google Scholar

86.

T. Shimizu 2001. Evolution of the forebrain in tetrapods. In “Brain Evolution and Cognition”. Ed by G. Roth and M. F. Wullimann . John-Wiley. New York. pp. 135–184. Google Scholar

87.

M. G. Stewart, A. Csillag, and S. P. R. Rose . 1987. Alterations in synaptic structure in the paleostriatal complex of the domestic chick, Gallus domesticus, following passive avoidance training. Brain Res 426:69–81. Google Scholar

88.

M. G. Stewart and D. A. Rusakov . 1995. Morphological changes associated with stages of memory formation in the chick following passive avoidance training. Behav Brain Res 12:21–28. Google Scholar

89.

M. G. Stewart, P. Kabai, E. Harrison, R. J. Steele, M. Kossut, M. Giedalski, and M. Csillag . 1996. The involvement of dopamine in the striatum in passive avoidance training in the chick. Neurosci 70:7–14. Google Scholar

90.

M. D. Suboski and C. Bartashunas . 1984. Mechanisms for social transmission of pecking preferences to neonatal chicks. J Exp Psychol Anim B 10:182–194. Google Scholar

91.

A. A. D. Székely, M. I. Boxer, M. G. Steward, and A. Csillag . 1994. Connectivity of the lobus parolfactorius of the domestic chicken (Gallus domesticus): an anterograde and retrograde pathway tracing study. J Comp Neurol 348:374–393. Google Scholar

92.

C. ten Cate 1986. Does behavior contingent stimulus movement enhance filial imprinting in Japanese quail? Dev Psychobiol 19:607–614. Google Scholar

93.

C. ten Cate 1989. Stimulus movement, hen behavior, and filial imprinting in Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica). Ethology 82:287–306. Google Scholar

94.

T. Tömböl 1995. Golgi structure of telencephalon of chicken. Semmelweis Univeristy Medical School. Budapest, Hungary. Google Scholar

95.

T. Tömböl, A. Csillag, and M. Steward . 1988a. Cell types of the hyperstriatum ventrale of the domestic chicken (Gallus domesticus): a Golgi study. J Hirnforschung 29:319–334. Google Scholar

96.

T. Tömböl, A. Csillag, and M. Steward . 1988b. Cell types of the paleostriatal complex of the domestic chicken (Gallus domesticus): a Golgi study. J Hirnforschung 29:493–507. Google Scholar

97.

L. Tommasi, G. Vallortigara, and M. Zanforlin . 1997. Young chickens learn to localize the centre of a spatial environment. J Comp Physiol A 180:567–572. Google Scholar

98.

L. Tommasi and G. Vallortigara . 2000. Searching for the center: spatial cognition in the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). J Exp Psychol 26:477–486. Google Scholar

99.

L. Tommasi and G. Vallortigara . 2001. Encoding of geometric and landmark information in the left and right hemispheres of the avian brain. Behav Neurosci 115:602–613. Google Scholar

100.

L. Tommasi, A. Gagliardo, R. J. Andrew, and G. Vallortigara . 2003. Separate processing mechanisms for encoding of geometric and landmark information in the avian hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci in press. Google Scholar

101.

G. Vallortigara and M. Zanforlin . 1986. Position learning in chicks. Behav Process 12:23–32. Google Scholar

102.

G. Vallortigara, M. Zanforlin, and M. Cailotto . 1988. Right-left asymmetry in position learning of male chicks. Behav Brain Res 27:189–191. Google Scholar

103.

G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin, G. Bortolomiol, and L. Tommasi . 1996. Lateral asymmetries due to preferences in eye use during visual discrimination learning in chicks. Behav Brain Res 74:135–143. Google Scholar

104.

G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin, M. Rigoni, and M. Zanforlin . 1998. Delayed search for a concealed imprinting object in the domestic chick. Anim Cogn 1:17–24. Google Scholar

105.

J. Vauclair 1996. Animal Cognition, an introduction to modern comparative psychology. Harvard University Press. Cambridge. Google Scholar

106.

S. Watanabe 2001. Van Gogh, Chagall and pigeons: picture discrimination in pigeons and humans. Anim Cogn 4:147–151. Google Scholar

107.

S. Watanabe, M. Wakita, and J. Sakamoto . 1995. Discrimination of Monet and Picasso in pigeons. JEAB 63:165–174. Google Scholar

108.

E. O. Wilson 1975. Sociobiology, the new synthesis. Harvard University Press. Cambridge. Google Scholar

109.

S. Yanagihara, T. Yagi, and T. Matsushima . 1998. Distinct mechanisms for expression of Fos-like immunoreactivity and synaptic potentiation in telencephalic hyperstriatum of the quail chick. Brain Res 779:240–253. Google Scholar

110.

S. Yanagihara, K. Koga, and T. Matsushima . 2000. Expression of transcription factor ZENK (zif/268) in telencephalon of quail chicks after induced seizure and passive avoidance training. Zool Sci 17:1221–1229. Google Scholar

111.

S. Yanagihara, E-I. Izawa, K. Koga, and T. Matsushima . 2001. Reward-related neuronal activities in basal ganglia of domestic chicks. NeuroReport 12:1431–1435. Google Scholar

112.

Z. Zhou, J. A. Clarke, and F. Zhang . 2002. Archaeoraptor's better half. Nature 420:285. Google Scholar
Toshiya Matsushima, Ei-Ichi Izawa, Naoya Aoki, and Shin Yanagihara "The Mind Through Chick Eyes : Memory, Cognition and Anticipation," Zoological Science 20(4), 395-408, (1 April 2003). https://doi.org/10.2108/zsj.20.395
Received: 20 February 2003; Published: 1 April 2003
KEYWORDS
basal ganglia
evolution
limbic system
optimal foraging
Back to Top