Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 December 2012 Social Interaction Influences Blood Cortisol Values and Brain Aromatase Genes in the Protandrous False Clown Anemonefish, Amphiprion ocellaris
Eri Iwata, Kyohei Mikami, Jun Manbo, Keiko Moriya-Ito, Hideaki Sasaki
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Anemonefish, Amphiprion spp., are socially controlled, protandrous sex changers with a monogamous mating system. Under certain conditions, sexually immature anemonefish with ambisexual gonads differentiate directly into males or females. Formation and maintenance of social rank in a group are considered key requirements for the induction of sex change or differentiation. Generally, each animal living in a social group experiences a different level of social stress in accordance with its social rank, and we hypothesize that the stress situation of individual anemonefish influences its sex determination. Groups of three sexually immature anemonefish were placed into each of five experimental tanks and kept for 10 days to allow for social rank formation and behavioral observation. The fish were then euthanized, and blood and brain samples were collected from each fish. The social rank of each individual was distinguishable from day 1 of the experiment. Aggressive behaviors were most frequent and blood Cortisol values were higher in dominant individuals. The transcription of mRNA for stress-related genes, i.e., those encoding for glucocorticoid and arginine vasotocin receptors, was higher in the brains of dominant individuals than in other social ranks. Furthermore, we detected higher transcription levels of gonad and brain aromatase genes, which encode the enzyme that converts androgens into estrogens, in the brains of dominant individuals. These results suggest that social rank reflects the blood Cortisol value, which in turn leads to sex differentiation by manipulating transcription of genes, including aromatase genes, in the brain.

INTRODUCTION

Plasticity in sex differentiation is common in teleost fish. In hermaphroditic teleost species, social interaction with conspecifics controls sex determination or sex change (Fishelson, 1970; Munday et al., 2006). Among sex-changing fish, anemonefish (genus Amphiprion) are unique in that they are socially controlled, protandrous sex changers with a monogamous mating system. They live symbiotically with sea anemones in the tropical waters of the Indo-Pacific region, forming a social unit consisting of a monogamous pair and several nonbreeders or juveniles. Females are the largest and dominant members of the social groups, displaying frequent aggressive behavior toward other group members. The second-ranked individuals become males, and the others remain as nonreproductive individuals. If a female disappears from a social unit, the male changes sex, and the largest of the nonbreeders becomes a functional male (Fricke and Fricke, 1977; Moyer and Nakazono, 1978). Meanwhile, under certain conditions, immature anemonefish with ambisexual gonads differentiate directly into males or females. For example, when juvenile anemonefish are raised together in captivity, the largest will become a female and the next largest a male, whereas the rest will remain sexually immature (Goldstein, 1989).

Anemonefish take about 45 d for a male-to-female sex change (Fricke, 1975; Godwin and Thomas, 1993) and several months or more for sex differentiation in an ambisexual pair (Iwata et al., 2008; Iwata et al., 2010a). Thus, the longterm social interaction in a group of anemonefish may be a key influence on the induction of sex change or differentiation (Iwata et al., 2008; Iwata et al., 2010a). In general, each animal living in a social group experiences a different level of social stress in accordance with its social rank (Goymann and Wingfield, 2004), and we hypothesize that the stress level of individual fish as determined by its social rank influences its sex determination.

The relationship between sex differentiation and physical stress, but not social stress, is well documented in gonochoristic teleost species with thermolabile sex determination (TSD), such as the Japanese flounder Paralichthys olivaceus and pejerrey Odontesthes bonariensis. High temperatures result in increased levels of the stress-related hormone cortisol in the blood, and high Cortisol levels suppress transcription of the aromatase gene, which leads to masculinization of larvae (Kitano et al., 2001; Hattori et al., 2009). Aromatase is the enzyme that converts testosterone to estradiol (E2), and is also considered to be a key enzyme of sex determination in vertebrates, including sex-changing fish (Gardner et al., 2005; Guiguen et al., 2010). Numerous studies have documented the relationship between aromatase and sex change. For example, aromatase inhibitors (Al) block the natural sex change and induce male function in the protandrous black porgy Acanthopagrus schlegeli (Lee et al., 2002; Wu et al., 2005) and increase plasma androgen and stimulate development of the testis in the protogynous honeycomb grouper Epinephelus merra (Bhandari et al., 2005). Aromatase also plays an important role in socially controlled sex-changing fish, including anemonefishes (Nakamura et al., 1994; Kobayashi et al., 2004; Kroon et al., 2005; Kobayashi et al., 2010).

Teleosts are known to have two types of genes for aromatase, cyp19a1a and cyp19a1b, which are predominantly expressed in the ovary and brain, respectively (Tchoudakova and Callard, 1998). Gonadal cyp19a1a is involved in the sex differentiation or sex change of teleosts mentioned above, whereas the brain cyp19a1b gene is also involved in local E2 synthesis and is considered to be related to brain sexualization and plasticity (Le Page et al., 2010; Okubo et al., 2011), although the details of these functions remain unclear, especially as they relate to sex-changing fish.

To investigate the relationship between stress, social rank, and sex differentiation in anemonefish, we evaluated the early stages of social rank formation by examining blood Cortisol values and behavioral traits in groups of three individual false clown anemonefish A. ocellaris with ambisexual gonads kept in a tank for 10 d. We then determined transcription levels of cyp19a1a and cyp19a1b genes in the brain. We also evaluated the stress-related glucocorticoid receptor (GR) and arginine vasotocin receptor (AVTR) genes in the brain.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Animals

Captive-bred sexually immature A. ocellaris with ambisexual gonads at least 12 months post hatch (provided by Dr. T. Furuta, Environmental Science Research Laboratory, Central Research Institute of Electric Power Industry, Chiba, Japan) were kept in a 56– L tank with a recirculating water system at 25–26°C under natural light. The fish were held in groups of 30 to 50 to suppress sexual maturation until the experiment started. Fish were fed commercial pellets (Omega One Marine Flakes, Omega Sea, Ltd., Perry, OH, USA) daily throughout the experiment.

Three fish were moved to each of five 26-L experimental tanks and kept for 10 d for social rank formation and behavioral observation. A PVC pipe coupling (50 mm diameter) substituted for a host sea anemone and was placed at the center of each experimental tank as a shelter. Each individual fish in a tank was identified by differences in the white-striped pattern on its body, and each fish was classified as α(dominant), β(second ranked), or γ(subordinate) on the basis of behavioral observations on day 1 of the experiment. Specifically, an individual that occupied the shelter most of the time was identified as a, an individual with the second highest occupation time was identified as β, and the individual with the shortest occupation time was identified as γ (Iwata et al., 2008).

The experimental protocols used in this study followed the Iwaki Meisei University's Policies Governing the Use of Live Vertebrate Animals and the Japan Ethological Society's Guidelines for Research on Animal Behavior.

Behavioral analysis

Because anemonefish are diurnally active, we videotaped twelve 5-min observation periods during the light period, starting at 0800, 0830, 1000, 1030, 1200, 1230, 1400, 1430, 1600, 1630, 1800, and 1830 on days 1, 4, 7, and 10. On the video recordings, we observed each experimental fish for the following four behaviors: duration of shelter occupation, frequency of threatening other fish by lunging, frequency of being the target of lunging, and frequency of trembling, which is an appeasement behavior (Moyer and Bell, 1976). The time spent in the shelter was converted to a percentage of the total observation time.

Sample collection and measurements

On day 10 of the experiment, after the video recording was completed, fish were captured and euthanized with 300 ppm of MS222 (tricaine methanesulfonate; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA). All resident fish in a tank were captured at once using a hand net and then moved to a smaller tank with the dissolved anesthetic chemical. The anesthesia was introduced within 3 min of the start of capture. Total body length and weight were measured, and blood samples were collected from the caudal vessel of each fish by using a heparinized capillary tube and centrifuged immediately. The plasma was removed and stored at -20°C in a plastic tube until assay. The brains were extracted and soaked in 500 µL of RNA-stabilization solution (RNAlater; Applied Biosystems, Carlsbad, CA, USA) in a 1.5-mL microfuge tube and stored at -20°C for gene analysis.

Hormone assays

Ten microliters of each plasma sample was extracted using 2 µL diethyl ether and resuspended in 200 µL of enzyme immunoassay (EIA) buffer (Cayman Chemical, Ann Arbor, Ml, USA), and the concentration of Cortisol was measured following the manufacturer's instructions in using a commercially available EIA kit (Cayman Chemical). All concentrations were measured in triplicate. The interand intra-assay coefficients of variation were 7.6% and 8.4%, respectively.

Partial gene sequencing

Total RNA was purified by using the RNeasy Mini Kit (Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). Reverse transcription of RNA was performed with a Thermo Script RT-PCR System (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA). cDNA was amplified by PCR with a Takara Ex Taq Reaction Kit (Takara Bio, Shiga, Japan). The total PCR reaction volume of 30 µL was composed of 3.0 µL 10× Ex Taq Buffer, 3.0 µL dNTP mixture, 2.1 pmol of each primer, 0.8 units Ex Taq, and 1.0 µL DNA solution containing 0.15 µg cDNA. The degenerate oligonucleotide primer sets used in the PCR are listed in Table 1. The PCR products were ligated into the pCR 2.1-TOPO vector by using TOPO TA Cloning (Invitrogen). Gene sequences were confirmed by DNA sequencing with an ABI PRISM 3730×l DNA Analyzer (Applied Biosystems). The gene sequences were compared with all other known gene sequences by using the Basic Local Alignment Search Tool (BLAST). Similar DNA sequences were downloaded from the DNA Data Bank of Japan (DDBJ) and aligned with our sequences. The data were registered in the DDBJ/EMBL (the European Molecular Biology Laboratory) /GenBank databases and assigned the accession numbers AB597954 (for GR), AB597955 (for AVTR), AB597952 (for cyp19a1b), and AB597953 (for cyp19a1a).

Real-time PCR analysis

For real-time PCR assay, gene-specific oligonucleotide primers were designed on ProbeFinder software (Roche Applied Science, Indianapolis, IN, USA). The specific oligonucleotide primer sets used in real-time PCR are listed in Table 2. β-actin was used as an internal control to normalize cDNA abundance. The specificity of the primer sets was confirmed by DNA sequencing.

Table 1.

Oligonucleotide sequences used for cloning, and their functions.

t01_849.gif

Table 2.

Oligonucleotide sequences used for quantitative PCR, and their functions.

t02_849.gif

Table 3.

Body length and mass of Amphiprion ocellaris kept in groups of three for 10 days. Values are means ± SE and significant differences between social ranks are indicated by different letters. Statistical significance is defined as P < 0.05 on the basis of ANOVA and Fisher's PLSD.

t03_849.gif

Real-time PCR was performed on a MiniOpticon Real-time PCR System (Bio-Rad, Hercules, CA, USA) with SYBR Green fluorescent labeling. Real-time analysis was performed with a final reaction volume of 25 µl Using <100 ng cDNA, 12.5 µl IQTM SYBR Green Super Mix (Bio-Rad), and 10 µM of each primer. The cycling parameters were 95°C for 3 min and then 34 cycles of 95°C for 10 s, 60°C for 30 s, and 72°C for 30 s, followed by a dissociation stage of 72°C for 7 min and 65°C for 1 min.

The cycle threshold (Ct) was calculated automatically with a manual baseline set at 3 to 15 cycles. The acceptability of each triplicate reaction was set at a Ct standard error (SE) of 0.85. Each set of data for social rank was normalized against the data from grouphoused control fish (GHC; reference gene, n = 5), which did not form social ranks due to the high stocking density. The normalized data were compared by using the equation of Jemiolo and Trappe (2004), which compares changes in the difference between the Ct of the gene of interest and a reference gene, expressed as:

e01_849.gif

Statistics

Statistical analyses were performed with StatView + Graphics 5.0J software (Abacus Concepts, Inc., Berkeley, CA, USA; no longer available); P < 0.05 was considered to indicate statistical significance. To compare behavioral elements, except for the frequency of lunging at other fish, we used a repeated two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Fisher's protected least significant difference (PLSD) test. Physical parameters, plasma Cortisol concentrations, and the frequency of lunging at other fish were compared by one-way ANOVA followed by Fisher's PLSD. Gene transcription levels were compared by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett's multiple comparison test.

RESULTS

Physical parameters of fish

ANOVA showed significant differences in body length (F2,11 = 4.089, P = 0.047) and mass (F2,11 = 4.229, P = 0.043); α individuals were significantly longer and heavier than γ ones, and β individuals were intermediate between the two (Table 3).

Behavioral observations

Social rank in groups of three individual A. ocellaris formed at the early stage of grouping (i.e., on day 1 of the experiment), and the ranks had not changed by the termination of the experiment. ANOVA revealed a significant (social rank) effect on behavior: F2,12 = 16.017, P < 0.001 for the amount of time spent in the shelter, F2,12 = 16.973, P < 0.001 for lunging, F2,12 = 10.363, P = 0.002 for being the target of lunging, and F2,12 = 8.996, P = 0.004 for trembling. However, there was no significant (time course × social rank) interaction effect. Thus, the behavioral parameters were compared using the total frequency during the entire period of observation.

Post hoc analysis revealed that α individuals spent significantly more time in the shelter than other individuals throughout the experimental period, whereas β and γ individuals rarely entered the shelter (Fig. 1A). Alpha individuals also displayed the most frequent lunging (Fig. 1B), the least trembling (Fig. 1D), and were the least likely to be a target of lunging (Fig. 1C). One-way ANOVA revealed that social rank had a significant effect on the frequency of lunging at other fish (F5,18 = 27.394, P < 0.01, Fig. 1E). Post hoc analysis revealed that α individuals lunged most frequently at β individuals. The frequencies of β fish lunging at α, γ lunging at α, and γ at β were the lowest, and the frequencies of α and β fish lunging at γ were intermediate.

Blood Cortisol values

ANOVA revealed a significant difference in blood Cortisol values according to social rank (F2.11 = 4.124, P = 0.046), and post hoc analysis showed that α individuals had significantly higher blood Cortisol than β and γ individuals, between which there was no significant difference (Fig. 1F).

Gene analysis

Specific primers were designed for real-time PCR, and gene transcription in the brains of fish of each social rank was compared (Fig. 2). ANOVA revealed significant differences among social ranks in the stress-related genes GR (F3,15 = 3.800, P = 0.039) and AVTR (F3,15 = 14.415, P < 0.001). Post hoc analysis revealed the highest transcription of GR and AVTR in α individuals.

Fig. 1.

Behavioral parameters and blood Cortisol values of individual α, β, and γ false clown anemonefish Amphiprion ocellaris kept together for 10 d. (A) Proportion of time spent in the shelter by individuals of each social level; (B) total frequency of lunging behavior; (C) frequency of lunging at other members of the social group; (D) total frequency of being the target of lunging; (E) frequency of trembling; and (F) blood Cortisol values. Values are means ± SE; *, P < 0.05; different letters above bars indicate statistically significant differences (Fisher's PLSD).

f01_849.jpg

Fig. 2.

Transcription of genes encoding for glucocorticoid receptor (GR), arginine vasotocin receptor (AVTR), brain aromatase (cyp19a1b), and gonad aromatase (cyp19a1a) in the brains of α, β, and γ Amphiprion ocellaris kept together for 10 d. Values are means ± SE. *, P < 0.05; *, P < 0.01, compared with a reference gene (i.e., value in group-housed control, GHC) (Dunnett's multiple comparison test).

f02_849.jpg

ANOVA indicated that there were significant differences in the transcription of cyp19a1b genes among social ranks (F3,15 = 5.004, P = 0.018), and post hoc analysis showed higher transcription of cyp19a1b in α individuals than in the other social ranks. Although there was a trend toward higher transcription of cyp19a1a in α individuals, ANOVA showed no significant differences between social ranks (F3,15 = 2.746, P = 0.089).

DISCUSSION

Our results showed that social rank in a group of three individual A. ocellaris formed at the early stage of grouping. On day 10 of the experiment, α individuals on average had a greater body mass than those of other social ranks. A previous study by our group showed that differences in body mass among social ranks occur because individuals of higher social rank hinder those of lower rank from feeding (Iwata et al., 2008), and it has also been reported that subordinate fish can adjust their growth strategically to avoid conflict among group members (Buston, 2003; Heg et al., 2004). However, the experimental period in the present study was short, and there is no evidence that the differences in body mass resulted only from differences in food intake. Body size is an important determinant of dominance relationships (Abbot et al., 1985; Beacham, 1987); therefore it seems likely that social rank was established by slight differences in body mass at the beginning of the experiment.

The social rank of each individual was clearly distinguishable by observing the frequency of lunging and being targeted by lunging; α individuals were lunged at least and lunged the most, displaying fierce aggressiveness, especially toward β individuals that tried to enter the shelter, β individuals lunged mostly at γ fish, and it was difficult for γ fish to get near the shelter because of the attacks by both α and β individuals. The α individuals seemed to monopolize the inside of the shelter, and their blood Cortisol levels were higher than those of the other group members.

It had been thought that social suppression from dominant individuals could elevate blood Cortisol levels and suppress reproductive activity in individuals with subordinate social rank (Blanchard et al., 1993). But recently it was found that for some simian species, the social rank having higher blood Cortisol values may vary with the species, which is a reflection of variations in social structures (Abbott et al., 2003). For example, male dominance rank in wild chimpanzees Pan troglodytes correlated positively with urinary Cortisol excretion in a stable dominance hierarchy, and Cortisol excretion also correlated positively with rates of male aggression (Muller and Wrangham, 2004). Similarly, high-ranking male Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata secreted significantly higher levels of Cortisol than low-ranking males (Barrett et al., 2002). These results suggest that there may be costs associated with dominance. Furthermore, recent studies revealed that dominant individuals of cooperative breeders, including the teleost species Neolamprologus pulcher (African cichlid), tended to show elevated glucocorticoids more often than subordinates (Creel, 2001; Mileva et al., 2009). Anemonefishes are not categorized as cooperative breeders, but they are highly social and have monogamous mating strategies; the male mainly cares for eggs and guards the nest (Moyer and Bell, 1976; Buston, 2004). High Cortisol values in dominant A. ocellaris in the present study may reflect such a social structure in anemonefishes.

In contrast, in another study of olive baboons Papio anubis, a male being challenged for his more dominant position tended to display higher basal Cortisol concentrations (Sapolsky, 1992). In the nonmammalian vertebrate lizard Anolis carolinensis, chronically elevated plasma glucocorticoids reliably inhibit aggressive behavior, but acute elevation of plasma glucocorticoids may either promote an actively aggressive response or be permissive to escalated aggression or activity (Summers et al., 2005). The higher blood Cortisol in dominant A. ocellaris compared to other members of the group in the present study might be due to the dominant individual's struggle to establish dominant status at the early stage of social rank formation of the group.

Stress-related genes in the brain (i.e., GR and AVTR), the transcription of which is associated with blood Cortisol values, also showed higher transcription in α individuals compared to other group members. As in other vertebrates, upregulation of GR mRNA in teleost fish is induced by an acute stress response as a result of elevation of blood Cortisol values (Acerete et al., 2007). In the green molly Poecilia latipinna, arginine vasotocin (AVT) neurons innervate the corticotroph cells of the pituitary (Batten et al., 1990) and influence adrenocorticotropin, and thus Cortisol secretion, in the rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss (Baker et al., 1996). In socially controlled protogynous bluehead wrasse Thalassoma bifasciatum, aromatase-immunoreactive fibers are closely associated with AVT-immunoreactive neurons in the preoptic area, indicating the interaction between local estrogen synthesis and signaling systems that subserve social behavior (Marsh et al., 2006). Moreover, our previous studies revealed that brain AVT neurons were modulated by social rank formation in A. ocellaris (Iwata et al., 2010a; Iwata et al., 2010b). Taken together, these findings suggest that the α individuals in the current experiment were acutely stressed.

In the brains of ambisexual A. ocellaris, transcription of the cyp19a1b gene, which encodes brain-type P450 aromatase, was higher in α individuals than in other social ranks. There was also a trend of higher cyp19a1a levels, which encodes gonad-type P450 aromatase, in α individuals. During the social rank formation period, the dominant individual is in a stressful situation and behaves aggressively toward other group members, possibly because of the need to establish and maintain its dominant status. This stressful social situation may influence aromatase activity, possibly via the hypothalamo—pituitary—adrenal axis.

It is well known that gonochoristic teleost species with TSD demonstrate changes in sex ratio in response to changes in environmental temperature. For example, high temperatures during the sex differentiation period in Japanese flounder produce a male-dominant population because cyp19a1a transcription is suppressed (Kitano et al., 1999; Kitano et al., 2001). Cortisol treatment mimics the effects of high temperature in inducing masculinization in larval-stage pejerrey; this induction is associated with the suppression of cyp19a1a transcription (Hattori et al., 2009). In Japanese flounder, the cAMP-responsive element (CRE) in the cyp19a1a promoter region reportedly binds to the cortisol—GR complex and suppresses gene transcription (Yamaguchi et al., 2010). Moreover, in sex-changing fish, sex-changing dominant females of the protogynous blue banded goby Lythpnus dalli, which displays an increased number of aggressive acts, have lower brain aromatase activity (Black et al., 2005).

In contrast, cyp19a1a and cyp19a1b in the brain of A. ocellaris in the current experiment seemed to be upregulated by the elevation of blood Cortisol. However, in several TSD teleost species, such as those of the genus Sevastes, high temperature induces the development of a femaledominant rather than a male-dominant population (Omoto et al., 2010). Furthermore, high blood Cortisol values are detected in male-to-female sex-changing A. melanopus 20 d after female removal (Godwin, 1994). It was also reported that polymorphisms in the promoter region of cyp19a1a of Japanese flounder are associated with blood E2 level (He et al., 2009). These results suggest the existence of a different regulatory mechanism that leads to feminization in certain stress situations.

It seem improbable at present that elevation of aromatase gene transcription in the brain is directly linked to sex determination in α individuals, as sex determination in ambisexual fish takes several months (Hattori and Yanagisawa, 1991; Iwata et al., 2008; Iwata et al., 2010a). Moreover, persistently elevated Cortisol levels would downregulate GR, which would in turn reduce blood Cortisol values (Pottinger, 1990; Maule and Schreck, 1991). A previous study by our group revealed that after six months in group living, blood Cortisol values tended to be higher in β individuals of A. ocellaris, which displayed submissive behavior (i.e., trembling) more frequently than α or γ individuals (Iwata et al., 2008). These results suggest that the social stress levels of each individual change over time; thus, more research is needed to reveal the relationship between endocrine changes associated with changes in social interactions in a group and sex determination mechanisms of A. ocellaris. However, it appears that aromatase genes may play a role in the brain during sex differentiation in A. ocellaris because social rank, which in turn leads to gonadal sex differentiation, is determined at the very early stages of social group formation.

Here, we have demonstrated for the first time that the transcription of P450 aromatase genes in the brain of A. ocellaris is upregulated by social stress. However, at present it is unclear whether Cortisol directly affects gene transcription. Thus, further experiments, for example evaluation of gene transcription in fish subjected to Cortisol administration, will be needed. Further examination of the time course of gene transcription changes in the gonad and in the brain as well as the analysis of upstream regions of these genes may reveal the mechanisms of sex differentiation or sex change in protandrous anemonefish.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This study was supported by a Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (no. 22570069) from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science. The authors thank Dr. T. Furuta of the Central Research Institute of Electric Power Industry for providing the anemonefish used in this study. We also thank Dr. M. Ichikawa of the Tokyo Metropolitan Institution of Neuroscience and Dr. M. Yokosuka of Nippon Veterinary and Life Science University for their valuable advice.

REFERENCES

1.

JC Abbot , RL Dunbrack , CD Orr ( 1985) The interaction of size and experience in dominance relationships of juvenile steelhead trout (Salmo gairdneri). Behaviour 92: 241–253 Google Scholar

2.

DH Abbott , EB Keverne , FB Bercovitch , CA Shively , SP Mendoza , W Saltzman , et al. (2003) Are subordinates always stressed? A comparative analysis of rank differences in Cortisol levels among primates. Horm Behav 43: 67–82 Google Scholar

3.

L Acerete , JC Balasch , B Castellana , B Redruello , N Roher , AV Canario ,et al. (2007) Cloning of the glucocorticoid receptor (GR) in gilthead seabream (Sparus aurata). Differential expression of GR and immune genes in gilthead seabream after an immune challenge. Comp Biochem Physiol B Biochem Mol Biol 148: 32–43 Google Scholar

4.

BI Baker , DJ Bird , JC Buckingham (1996) In the trout, CRH and AVT synergize to stimulate ACTH release. Regul Pept 67: 207–210 Google Scholar

5.

GM Barrett, K Shimizu, M Bardi, S Asaba, A Mori (2002) Endocrine correlates of rank, reproduction, and female-directed aggression in male Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) Horm Behav 42: 85–96 Google Scholar

6.

TF Batten , ML Cambre , L Moons , F Vandesande (1990) Comparative distribution of neuropeptide—immunoreactive systems in the brain of the green molly, Poecilia latipinna. J Comp Neurol 302: 893–919 Google Scholar

7.

JL Beacham (1987) The relative importance of body size and aggressive experience as determinations of dominance in pumpkinseed sunfish, Lepomis gibbosus. Anim Behav 36: 621–623 Google Scholar

8.

RK Bhandari , MA Alam , M Higa , K Soyano , M Nakamura (2005) Evidence that estrogen regulates the sex change of honeycomb grouper (Epinephelus merra), a protogynous hermaphrodite fish. J Exp Zoolog A Comp Exp Biol 303: 497–503 Google Scholar

9.

MP Black , J Balthazart , M Baillien , MS Grober (2005) Socially induced and rapid increases in aggression are inversely related to brain aromatase activity in a sex-changing fish, Lythpnus dalli. Proc R Soc B 272: 2435–2440 Google Scholar

10.

DC Blanchard , RR Sakai , B McEwen , SM Weiss , RJ Blanchard (1993) Subordination stress: behavioral, brain, and neuroendocrine correlates. Behav Brain Res 58: 113–121 Google Scholar

11.

P Buston (2003) Social hierarchies: size and growth modification in clownfish. Nature 424: 145–146 Google Scholar

12.

PM Buston ( 2004) Does the presence of non-breeders enhance the fitness of breeders? An experimental analysis in the clown anemonefish Amphiprion percula. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 57: 23–31 Google Scholar

13.

S Creel (2001) Social dominace and stress hormones. Trend Ecol Evol 16: 491–497 Google Scholar

14.

L Fishelson (1970) Protogynous sex reversal in the fish Anthias squamipinnis (Teleostei, Anthiidae) regulated by the presence or absence of a male fish. Nature 227: 90–91 Google Scholar

15.

H Fricke , S Fricke (1977) Monogamy and sex change by aggressive dominance in coral reef fish. Nature 266: 830–832 Google Scholar

16.

HW Fricke (1975) Selektives feinderkennen bei dem anemonefisch Amphiprion bicinctus. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 19: 1–7 Google Scholar

17.

L Gardner , T Anderson , AR Place , B Dixon , A Elizur (2005) Sex change strategy and the aromatase genes. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 94: 395–404 Google Scholar

18.

J Godwin (1994) Bahavioural aspects of protandrous sex change in the anemonefish, Amphiprion melamopus, and endocrine correlates. Anim Behav 48: 551–567 Google Scholar

19.

J Godwin , P Thomas (1993) Sex change and steroid profiles in the protandrous anemonefish Amphiprion melanopus (Pomacentridae, Teleostei). Gen Comp Endocrinol 91: 144–157 Google Scholar

20.

R Goldstein (1989) Breeding marine clownfish. Aquarium Fish Magazine 1: 32 Google Scholar

21.

W Goymann , JC Wingfield (2004) Allostatic load, social status and stress hormones: the costs of social status matter. Anim Behav 67: 591–602 Google Scholar

22.

Y Guiguen , A Fostier , F Piferrer , CF Chang (2010) Ovarian aromatase and estrogens: a pivotal role for gonadal sex differentiation and sex change in fish. Gen Comp Endocrinol 165: 352– 366 Google Scholar

23.

A Hattori , Y Yanagisawa (1991) Sex change of the anemonefish Amphiprion clarkii in a habitat of high host density: a removal study. Jpn J Ecol 41: 1–8 Google Scholar

24.

RS Hattori, JI Fernandino, A Kishii, H Kimura, T Kinno, M Oura, et al. (2009) Cortisol-induced masculinization: does thermal stress affect gonadal fate in pejerrey, a teleost fish with temperaturedependent sex determination? PLoS One 4: e6548 Google Scholar

25.

F He , HS Wen , SL Dong , B Shi , CF Chen , LS Wang , al. et (2009) Polymorphisms within promotro of Japanese flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus) ovary cytochrome P450-c19 (CYP19a) gene associated with reproductive traits. Fish Physiol Biochem 35: 333–340 Google Scholar

26.

D Heg, N Bender, Hamilton (2004) Strategic growth decisions in helper cichlids. Proc R Soc B 271 : s505–s508 Google Scholar

27.

E Iwata , Y Nagai , M Hyoudou , H Sasaki (2008) Social environment and sex differentiation in false clown anemonefish, Amphiprion ocellaris. Zool Sci 25: 123–128 Google Scholar

28.

E Iwata , Y Nagai , H Sasaki (2010a) Immunohistochemistry of brain arginine vasotocin and isotocin in false clown anemonefish Amphiprion ocellaris. The Open Fish Science Journal 3: 147– 153 Google Scholar

29.

E Iwata, Y Nagai, H Sasaki (2010b) Social rank modulates brain arginine vasotocin immunoreactivity in false clown anemonefish (Amphiprion ocellaris) Fish Physiology and Biochemistry 36: 337–345 Google Scholar

30.

B Jemiolo , S Trappe (2004) Single muscle fiber gene expression in human skeletal muscle: validation of internal control with exercise. Biochem Biophys Res Commun 320: 1043–1050 Google Scholar

31.

T Kitano , K Takamune , T Kobayashi , Y Nagahama , SI Abe (1999) Suppression of P450 aromatase gene expression in sexreversed males produced by rearing genetically female larvae at a high water temperature during a period of sex differentiation in the Japanese flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus). J Mol Endocrinol 23: 167–176 Google Scholar

32.

T Kitano , K Takamune , Y Nagahama , A Shin-ichi (2001) Role of P450 aromatase in gonadal sex differentiation in Japanese flounder (Paralichthys olivaceus). Environmental Sciences 8: 1–11 Google Scholar

33.

Y Kobayashi , T Kobayashi , M Nakamura , T Sunobe , CE Morrey , N Suzuki , Y Nagahama (2004) Characterization of two types of cytochrome P450 aromatase in the serial-sex changing gobiid fish, Trimma okinawae. Zool Sci 21: 417–425 Google Scholar

34.

Y Kobayashi , R Horiguchi , S Miura , M Nakamura (2010) Sex- and tissue-specific expression of P450 aromatase (cyp19a1a) in the yellowtail clownfish, Amphiprion clarkii. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol lntegr Physiol 155: 237–244 Google Scholar

35.

FJ Kroon , PL Munday , DA Westcott , JA Hobbs , NR Liley (2005) Aromatase pathway mediates sex change in each direction. Proc R Soc Lond B 272: 1399–1405 Google Scholar

36.

YH Lee , WS Yueh , JL Du , LT Sun , CF Chang (2002) Aromatase inhibitors block natural sex change and induce male function in the protandrous black porgy, Acanthopagrus schlegeli Bleeker: possible mechanism of natural sex change. Biol Reprod 66: 1749–1754 Google Scholar

37.

Page Y Le , N Diotel , C Vaillant , E Pellegrini , I Anglade , Y Merot , O Kah (2010) Aromatase, brain sexualization and plasticity: the fish paradigm. Eur J Neurosci 32: 2105–2115 Google Scholar

38.

KE Marsh , LM Creutz , MB Hawkins , J Godwin (2006) Aromatase immunoreactivity in the bluehead wrasse brain, Thalassoma bifasciatum: immunolocalization and co-regionalization with arginine vasotocin and tyrosine hydroxylase. Brain Res 1126: 91–101 Google Scholar

39.

AG Maule , CB Schreck (1991) Stress and Cortisol treatment changed affinity and number of glucocorticoid receptors in leukocytes and gill of coho salmon. Gen Comp Endocrinol 84: 83– 93 Google Scholar

40.

VR Mileva , JL Fitzpatrick , S Marsh-Rollo , KM Gilmour , CM Wood , S Balshine (2009) The stress response of the highly social African cichlid Neolamprologus pulcher. Physiol Biochem Zool 82: 720–729 Google Scholar

41.

JT Moyer , LJ Bell (1976) Reproductive behavior of the anemonefish Amphiprion clarkii at Miyake-Jima, Japan. Jpn J Ichthyol 23: 23–32 Google Scholar

42.

JT Moyer , A Nakazono (1978) Protandrous hermaphroditism in six species of the anemonefish genus Amphiprion in Japan. Jpn J Ichthyol 25: 101–106 Google Scholar

43.

M Muller, R Wrangham (2004) Dominance, Cortisol and stress in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55: 332–340 Google Scholar

44.

PL Munday , AM Cardoni , C Syms (2006) Cooperative growth regulation in coral-dwelling fishes. Biol Lett 2: 355–358 Google Scholar

45.

T Naito , Y Saito , J Yamamoto , Y Nozaki , K Tomura , M Hazama , et al. (1998) Putative pheromone receptors related to the Ca2+sensing receptor in Fugu. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95: 5178– 5181 Google Scholar

46.

K Nakamura , T Mariko , Y Nagahama (1994) Ultrastructure and in vitro steroidogenesis of the gonads in the protandrous anemonefish Amphiprion frenatus. Jpn J Ichthyol 41: 47–56 Google Scholar

47.

K Okubo , A Takeuchi , R Chaube , B Paul-Prasanth , S Kanda , Y Oka , Y Nagahama (2011) Sex differences in aromatase gene expression in the medaka brain. J Neuroendocrinol 23: 412– 423 Google Scholar

48.

N Omoto , Y Koya , B Chin , Y Yamashita , M Nakagawa , T Noda (2010) Gonadal sex differentiation and effect of rearing temperature on sex ratio in black rockfish (Sebastes schlegeli). Ichthyol Res 57: 133–138 Google Scholar

49.

TG Pottinger (1990) The effect of stress and exogenous Cortisol on receptor-like binding of Cortisol in the liver of rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. Gen Comp Endocrinol 78: 194–203 Google Scholar

50.

RM Sapolsky (1992) Cortisol concentrations and the social significance of rank instability among wild baboons. Psychoneuroendocrinology 17: 701–709 Google Scholar

51.

CH Summers , MJ Watt , TL Ling , GL Forster , RE Carpenter , WJ Korzan , al. et (2005) Glucocorticoid interaction with aggression in non-mammalian vertebrates: reciprocal action. Eur J Pharmacol 526: 21–35 Google Scholar

52.

A Tchoudakova , GV Callard (1998) Identification of multiple CYP19 genes encoding different cytochrome P450 aromatase isozymes in brain and ovary. Endocrinology 139: 2179–2189 Google Scholar

53.

GC Wu , JL Du , YH Lee , MF Lee , CF Chan (2005) Current status of genetic and endocrine factors in the sex change of protandrous Black Porgy, Acanthopagrus schlegeli (Teleostean). Ann NY Acad Sci 1040: 206–214 Google Scholar

54.

T Yamaguchi , N Yoshinaga , YazawaT , K Gen , T Kitano (2010) Cortisol is involved in temperature-dependent sex determination in the Japanese flounder. Endocrinology 151: 3900–3908 Google Scholar
© 2012 Zoological Society of Japan
Eri Iwata, Kyohei Mikami, Jun Manbo, Keiko Moriya-Ito, and Hideaki Sasaki "Social Interaction Influences Blood Cortisol Values and Brain Aromatase Genes in the Protandrous False Clown Anemonefish, Amphiprion ocellaris," Zoological Science 29(12), 849-855, (1 December 2012). https://doi.org/10.2108/zsj.29.849
Received: 10 January 2012; Accepted: 1 June 2012; Published: 1 December 2012
KEYWORDS
anemonefish
aromatase
sex change
social behavior
stress
Back to Top