BioOne.org will be down briefly for maintenance on 17 December 2024 between 18:00-22:00 Pacific Time US. We apologize for any inconvenience.
Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 August 2007 Display Size Preferences and Foraging Habits of High Andean Butterflies Pollinating Chaetanthera lycopodioides (Asteraceae) in the Subnival of the Central Chilean Andes
Mary T. K. Arroyo, Irene Till-Bottraud, Cristian Torres, Carolina A. Henríquez, Jaime Martínez
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

In our study, we asked whether butterflies visiting Chaetanthera lycopodioides (Asteraceae) in the subnival at 3450 m a.s.l. in the central Andes prefer larger floral displays. To answer this question, we compared the population distribution of display sizes with the distribution of visited display sizes at two sites. Six high elevation butterflies: Faunula leucoglene (Satyridae) (the dominant species), Hylephila sp. (Hesperiidae), Phulia nymphula (Pieridae), Vanessa terpsichore (Nymphalidae), Tatochila mercedis (Nymphalidae), and Yramea modesta (Nymphalidae) were reported as visitors. Butterflies tended to discriminate against plants with a single open head, preferring larger display sizes at both sites. Butterflies visited few plants per bout (mean: 3.1–4.5) and probed increasingly smaller proportions of the open heads per plant with increasing display size (overall mean: 45.8–48.4%). Results suggest that high elevation butterflies tend to prefer larger display sizes over smaller ones, but final choices are conditioned by the relative abundance of each display size in a population. Although previous studies show that butterflies prefer larger artificial individual flowers over smaller ones, our work appears to constitute the first report of butterfly preference for larger floral displays in any plant species or ecosystem.

Introduction

Pollinator preference for larger floral displays has been demonstrated on numerous occasions (Ohara and Higashi, 1994; Ohashi and Yahara, 1998, 1999; Ishii and Sakai, 2001; Elle and Carney, 2003; Mitchell et al., 2004; Harder et al., 2004; Grindeland et al., 2005; Kudo and Harder, 2005), and increased display size has been interpreted as a selective response on the part of plants for capturing pollinator visits. However, to date, most studies demonstrating preference for larger display sizes pertain to bee species, with a limited number of studies on flies and hummingbirds (Ohashi and Yahara, 2001). In particular, it remains unknown whether lepidopterans prefer larger displays when confronted with a variety of floral display sizes. One early experimental study at the individual flower level has shown that butterflies prefer larger individual artificial flowers over smaller artificial flowers when given a choice (Ilse, 1932), such that it would not be surprising that larger display sizes might also be preferred by this group of pollinators. Nevertheless, Kelber (1997) showed that hawkmoths preferred individual flowers of intermediate size.

Butterflies are important flower visitors for plant life at the highest elevations (i.e., in many alpine areas) (Arroyo et al., 1982). Yet, present knowledge of floral evolution in the alpine mostly comes from bee pollination studies (Galen and Newport, 1987; Galen, 1996). Along the altitudinal gradient, butterflies tend to replace bees at the higher elevations, or become relatively more common in relation to bees than at lower elevations, on account of their lower energy requirements and their ability to undergo passive thermoregulation (Arroyo et al., 1982). Butterflies have been questioned by some authors with regard to their efficiency as pollinators, on the grounds of limited pollen transport (e.g., Wiklund et al., 1979). However, this claim does not apply to all cases of butterfly pollination (e.g., Murphy 1984; Herrera, 1987). Moreover, several authors have pointed out that in traveling large distances (Waser, 1982; Herrera, 1987; Goulson et al., 1997), butterflies will tend to promote high levels of outcrossing. Given the importance of butterflies as visitors to flowers at the highest elevations, knowledge of whether butterflies prefer larger floral displays is critical for formulating hypotheses about floral evolution in high elevation floras and for understanding floral evolution in butterfly-pollinated plants in general. Large display sizes can be expected to emit stronger scents, and thus provide multiple cues (visual and olfactory) for attracting pollinators. Butterflies are characterized as having excellent long-range vision and an exceptionally wide visual field (Merry et al., 2006), strong color constancy (Kinoshita and Arikawa, 2000; Weiss and Papaj, 2003), and well-developed olfactory capacity (Andersson, 2003). Because larger floral displays will usually signify a high local concentration of floral resources, butterflies, like bees and other pollinator groups (Ohashi and Yahara, 2001), can also be expected to prefer larger display sizes when given a choice. However, this behavioral tendency, resulting in greater efficiency in nectar collection, might be even more marked in butterflies because they divide their time between nectaring and other activities such as searching for mates and defending territories (Goulson et al., 1997).

In our research, we asked whether subnival butterflies pollinating Chaetanthera lycopodioides (Asteraceae), an abundant plant species between 3200 and 3600 m a.s.l. in the central Chilean Andes, preferentially visit larger floral displays. We also determined whether butterflies visit all of the open capitula head on a plant, taking advantage of all available floral resources offered by the plant. Studies focusing on pollinator visitation and display size preferences usually rely on comparisons of pollination visitation rates in artificial floral populations (e.g., Elle and Carney, 2003; Biernaskie and Cartar, 2004; Karron et al., 2004). Here we adopt an alternative approach in which we compare the frequency distribution of display sizes visited by pollinators with the frequency distribution of display sizes found in large, un-manipulated populations.

Study Species and Methods

Chaetanthera lycopodioides is a low-growing (3–4 cm tall), loosely branched, caespitose perennial herb endemic to the high Andes of central Chile and adjacent Argentina. Plants are characterized by showy sessile capitula (∼ 16 mm diameter) bearing white, female-sterile ray florets tinged red on the underside and yellow disc florets, which strongly contrast with the tightly adpressed brownish-green foliage. Chaetanthera lycopodioides has been reported to be pollinated by butterflies (Arroyo et al., 1982). At peak flowering, population display sizes represent approximately 40% (one head open) to 80% (six or more head open) of a plant's full number of capitula (M. T. K. Arroyo, unpublished data). Although genetically self-compatible (M. T. K. Arroyo, unpublished data), protandry at the level of the capitulum determines that C. lycopodioides requires external pollinators for full achene set.

Butterfly visitation with respect to display size and number of open heads per plant was investigated at peak flowering at two subnival sites located at 3450 m in the Cerro Tres Puntas area, La Parva-Valle Nevado ski complex area, Andes of central Chile, lat 33°S. Above 3000 m a.s.l. summer days tend to be sunny in the morning and cloudy as of the early mid-afternoon (Arroyo et al., 1981). Mean annual temperature is 1.2°C at 3500 m a.s.l. (Cavieres et al., 2000). At both sites C. lycopodioides was very abundant, continuously distributed, and the dominant species in the vegetation (∼ 90% of total plant cover), with no other showy species present in the area. The SE site was located on the upper east-southeastern side of a small dividing ridge, while the SW site was located on the southwestern side of the same ridge some 90 m away. Although the two sites are quite close, the SW side of the ridge tends to be wetter and supports higher plant cover. We chose to work at two sites in order to ascertain the generality of any patterns found.

At each site an observation plot measuring 8 m × 20 m ( =  160 m2) was marked off. Plot size followed the requirement that one be able to register long visitation sequences and obtain reliable data on display size frequencies in the population. Plants of C. lycopodioides occurring with a linear distance of 5 m from each of the four borders of the plots were weeded away so as to minimize carryover search images obtained outside of the patches.

Detailed observations of butterflies entering the plots (hereafter, bouts) were made by two or three observers working simultaneously over three contiguous sunny days per site (a total of six days of observation) during the period 10–19 February 2005. For as many separate butterfly bouts as we could handle (estimated at >95%), we recorded the identification of the butterfly, the display size of each plant (expressed as number of open heads) for the sequence of plants the butterfly visited in the plot on the particular bout, along with the number of open heads visited on the particular plant. Non-informative bouts, where the butterflies entered the plots but failed to make visits, were also recorded. Bouts were registered continuously from the time the first butterflies appeared (10:00–10:30 hrs) to the time they disappeared (usually 16:00–16:30 hrs), except on the third day of observations on SW when clouds appeared early in the afternoon and activity abruptly ceased. Butterfly behavior was further investigated by assessing the length of the visitation sequences and by determining the percentage of open heads visited per display size.

To establish the population distribution of display sizes, we surveyed the entire plots around midday on each observation day, scoring all plants with open heads and the number of open heads on each scored plant. Finally, as complementary information, records of air temperature were made at 5, 50, and 150 cm above ground level at 30-minute intervals.

Data Analysis

Over the three days of observation at each site, the total number of plants bearing open heads in the plots showed some variation because new heads were opening on some plants and old heads were closing on others. For statistical purposes we recognized five display sizes: plants with one, two, three, four, or five or more open heads. The frequency distribution of display sizes was indistinguishable over the three days at each site (SE: G  =  4.18, d.f.  =  8, p > 0.05; SW: G  =  7.01, d.f.  =  8, p > 0.05). This allowed us to pool the visitation data for the three days at each site. To detect display size preferences, the display size distribution of plants visited by the butterflies was compared with the population display size distribution using the G-test, where the expected frequencies for the visited plants were calculated from the population display size frequencies, reflecting the null hypothesis of no preference for larger-sized displays. In order to eliminate pseudo-replication in the comparisons of display size distributions, a single plant visited was randomly selected from the sequence of plants visited on an individual bout using a standard randomization procedure ( http://www.random.org/nform.html). This procedure reduced the number of data points from 578 to 187 on SE and from 1101 to 245 on SW. We generated the population display size distribution from the average number for each display size over the three sampling days in each population. In order to visualize differences in the distribution of display sizes in the visited plants and total population, the frequency of each display size for the full sets of visited plants was subtracted from the frequency of the corresponding display size in the population.

Results

During the six sampling days the skies were virtually cloudless, except for the afternoon of day three in SW. Temperature in the shade at 10 cm a.g.l. bordered on 19–20°C, at 50 cm a.g.l. it was 14–16°C, and at 150 cm a.g.l. the temperature was 13–14°C. The mornings were totally calm until around 11:00; thereafter light to strong winds characterized both sites until around 15:30.

Display size varied considerably, from one to 13 open capitula (Fig. 1), with larger displays in the denser and more productive SW site. In both populations, more plants had one open head than any other number, with few plants displaying five or more heads. Intermediate display sizes (two to four heads open per plant) constituted around 39% of plants at the SE site and 51% of the SW sites.

Figure 1

Display size distribution in Chaetanthera lycopodioides in SE and SW, 3450 m a.s.l. Vertical bars  =  2 SE for the three sampling days. See Table 1 for number of plants with open heads at each site.

i1523-0430-39-3-347-f01.gif

Table 1

Comparison of Chaetanthera lycopodioides in SW and SE, 3540 m, central Chilean Andes.

i1523-0430-39-3-347-t01.gif

Among the 904 butterfly bouts observed, a total of 432 were informative, giving 1589 plant and 1959 capitulum visits, respectively. The proportion of informative bouts was significantly higher on the denser SW site (54.1%) as compared with SE (41.5%) (Gadj  =  7.217, d.f.  =  1, p < 0.01). The dominant visiting butterfly was: Faunula leucoglene (Satyridae) (71–81% of bouts). Other species recorded were Hylephila sp. (Hesperiidae), Phulia nymphula (Pieridae), Tatochila mercedis (Nymphalidae), Vanessa terpsichore (Nymphalidae), and Yramea modesta (Nymphalidae). With the exception of Vanessa terpsichore, an occasional visitor, which was recorded on one occasion to visit an outstanding 55 plants in a single bout in SW, the number of plant visits made by a given butterfly species was proportional to its relative bout abundance (Fig. 2). For non-informative bouts, the butterflies flew over the sampling areas without visiting C. lycopodioides heads or any other plant species in the patches. Visits by Dipterans were so rare that we did not even consider quantifying them, and neither were they the object of study.

Figure 2

Percentage of the informative bouts (Inf) and percentage of total plant visits (Vis) made by each of the six species of butterflies visiting Chaetanthera lycopodioides in SE and SW, 3450 m a.s.l., central Chilean Andes. FL: Faunula leucoglene (Satyridae); HY: Hylephila sp. (Hesperiidae); YM: Yramea modesta (Nymphalidae); PN: Phulia nymphula (Pieridae); VT: Vanessa terpsichore (Nymphalidae); TM: Tatochila mercedis (Nymphalidae).

i1523-0430-39-3-347-f02.gif

The visited display size distribution was significantly different from the population display size distribution at both sites (SE: G  =  14.41, d.f.  =  4, p < 0.01; SW: G  =  17.53, d.f.  =  4, p < 0.01), there being a consistent trend in both populations for butterflies to discriminate strongly against the displays with one head in favor of larger-sized displays (Fig. 3). However, as the display sizes reached the exceptionally large sizes (> 5 heads) there was not evidence for exaggerated levels of visitation (Fig. 3); however, numbers for this display size are so small that it is difficult to make any firm statements.

Figure 3

Percent difference in the frequency of each display size visited in Chaetanthera lycopodioides in SE and SW, 3450 m a.s.l., with respect to the frequency of those display sizes at the population level. Negative percentage values indicate display sizes that are visited at lower frequencies in relation to their respective contributions in the populations, while positive values indicate display sizes that received proportionately more visits than expected from their frequencies in the populations. One of the plants visited in the sequence of plants visited in each butterfly bout was randomly selected, giving a final sample size of N  =  187 on SE and 245 on SW. Population sample sizes are given in Table 1.

i1523-0430-39-3-347-f03.gif

The number of plants visited per bout was exceedingly low given the literally hundreds of plants in the dense sampling areas, and significantly larger on the denser SW site (Table 1; Fig. 4). On average, fewer than half of the open heads per plant were probed (Table 1), with a notable decrease with increasing display size (Fig. 5).

Figure 4

Number of plants visited per bout by butterflies in SE and SW. Bouts where more than 20 plants were visited have been grouped into a single category. Lines have been used to facilitate distinguishing the tendencies in the two sites.

i1523-0430-39-3-347-f04.gif

Figure 5

Mean number of open heads visited in relation to total heads open per plant in Chaetanthera lycopodioides in SE and SW, 3450 m a.s.l. Vertical bars  =  2 SE of the mean for three days sampling. The line on the graph represents 100% of heads visited per plant (slope  =  1).

i1523-0430-39-3-347-f05.gif

Discussion

Butterflies visiting natural populations of C. lycopodioides at 3450 m a.s.l. in the subnival discriminated against the smallest floral displays. The fact that the same trend occurred in two separate populations on opposite-facing slopes suggests that the tendency for butterflies to prefer larger displays is a generalized trend. However, the relationship is not a simple one. The very large display sizes possibly did not receive exaggerated numbers of visits because of their lower frequencies in populations of C. lycopodioides. Classical optimal foraging theory (Stephens and Krebs, 1986) predicts that larger flowers (or displays) should be exploited once encountered, but if their relative abundance is very low, or if their exploitation signifies excessive costs (energetic cost or predation risk, among others), then pollinators should prefer to exploit second-class flowers. Significantly, in this context, most studies considering pollinator preference and display size rely on experiments where different display sizes are offered to pollinators in equal frequencies and densities. Our results suggest that offering various display sizes in their natural frequencies could alter the finer conclusions in such experimental studies. Of course, other potential pitfalls arise when using un-manipulated natural populations. For example, it has been shown that pollinators prefer taller plants (Donnelly et al., 1998). If taller plants naturally tend to have larger inflorescences (and displays), the relationship with display size could be spurious. This last situation, however, is unlikely to be the case in Chaetanthera lycopodioides, where the prostrate caespitose habit determines that plant height shows little variation with plant and display size.

Whether the ultimate cue determining the display size preferences of Chaetanthera lycopodioides butterfly pollinators is visual or olfactory remains to be seen. Butterflies use visual cues to locate floral resources from a distance, and a combination of visual and olfactory cues at short ranges (Andersson, 2003; Honda et al., 2004). However, one recent experimental study shows that even at short ranges visual cues (e.g., color) can be more important than scent (Ômura and Honda, 2005). These various behavioral studies suggest that visual cues are likely to be relevant, at least when a butterfly initially approaches a plant from a distance. However, what is visually perceived by a pollinator is likely to change with increasing proximity to the plant. At a large distance, overall display size and color are likely to be most important. However, at close range, the size of the individual flower in a display might be more important. The preference by hawkmoths for intermediate-sized individual flowers is thought to correspond to a need to perceive an image size that allows the animal to stabilize its position in space when hovering (Kelber, 1997). Individual flower size and inflorescence size tend to be seen as two sides of the same coin by pollination biologists, but this will not necessarily be the case (cf. Harder et al., 2004).

Though the butterflies visiting C. lycopodioides selected larger displays, counterintuitively, with respect to the expectations of optimal foraging, they avoided many of the open heads on a plant, moving quickly to the next plant. Moreover, with a few notable exceptions, there was a clear tendency for the butterflies to visit very few plants in the plots per bout (< 5). The decreasing proportion of heads probed per plant with increasing display size has been previously reported in other pollinator groups (Klinkhamer et al., 1989; Iwasa et el., 1995; Ohashi and Yahara, 2001, 2002) and has been related to the risk of a pollinator spending time on flowers that have already been handled. The proportional drop-off in butterfly-pollinated C. lycopodioides, however, seems to be greater than reported for bee-pollinated species. This trend could simply reflect high elevation environmental conditions as they affect pollinator behavior. In high elevations, foraging is strongly constrained by low temperatures (Arroyo et al., 1985; Bergman et al., 1996; Totland and Schulte-Herbrüggen, 2003), limiting the amount of time a pollinator can engage in active flight or foraging before resorting to passive thermoregulation. The outstanding weather conditions over the study period, however, make it unlikely that the short butterfly visits were conditioned by temperature conditions. Heads of C. lycopodioides are found at about 2–5 cm a.g.l., while butterflies, when foraging, tend to fly between plants at about 10–30 cm a.g.l. Temperatures at the height of butterfly flight during the study period, according to the measurements taken, were somewhere in the range of 13–14°C (50 cm a.g.l.) and 18–20°C (150 cm a.g.l.), which is not particularly cold.

Alternatively, butterflies might be coaxed into visiting the very attractive plants of C. lycopodioides using visual signals from a distance but fail to remain on the same plant once they are able to assess the quality of the floral resources in the first head(s) probed. Measurements of nectar are unavailable in C. lycopodioides, but amounts are so small as to be imperceptible to the human eye. Due to strong intra-head protandry, self-compatible C. lycopodioides requires pollinator visitors to obtain a full seed set. However, some intra-capitulum geitonogamy and considerable inter-capitulum geitonogamy will occur on a plant with more than one open head (cf. Galloway et al., 2002; Karron et al., 2004). Combining attractive displays with low amounts of floral resources would tend to promote outcrossing by discouraging inter-capitulum geitonogamy. Harder and Barrett (1995) have suggested that traditional interpretations of floral design and display need to be broadened to recognize their role in reducing the effect of geitonogamous pollen transfer.

Another relevant factor in Chaetanthera lycopodioides concerns the fact that butterflies intersperse nectaring with other activities, such as searching for mates and defending territories (Goulson et al., 1997). Over altitudinal gradients, butterflies tend to emerge later in the season and have shorter flight periods at the higher altitudes (Gutíerrez and Menéndez, 1998), such that the aforementioned tasks must necessarily be completed over a relatively shorter period of time. On numerous occasions, we witnessed intense social interactions between butterflies, which led to the abandonment of foraging. Indeed, for about half of the bouts, the butterflies flew completely over the plots (presumably searching for mates), failing to make any visits at all. Such behavioral patterns and strong social interactions make the interpretation of butterfly foraging patterns (and their implications for floral morphology) more complex than in well-studied bee pollination systems.

Finally, the showiness of alpine plants has long drawn the attention of alpine ecologists (cf. Körner, 1999). Using allometric models, Fabbro and Körner (2004) demonstrated that although individual flowers of European alpine species are not significantly larger than those of lowland species, proportionately more of an alpine plant's resources are invested in flowers in relation to vegetative components. In addition, more of a plant's flowers remain open simultaneously, resulting in comparatively large displays. These authors suggested that the relatively higher investment in flowers together with longer-lived flowers in alpine species might be the result of strong selection favouring outcrossing under pollinator-impoverished conditions in the alpine.

Here we have documented that butterflies discriminate against the smallest floral displays. All other things being equal, butterflies would therefore tend to favour population variants that allocate relatively more of their resources to the production of flowers and larger displays. This validates a critical supposition underlying Fabbro and Körner's (2004) explanation for altitudinal changes in resource allocation in an important group of high elevation pollinators. Future ecological studies in butterfly plants might focus on the relative contribution of individual flower size versus flower number as they affect total display, and aim to determine the limits at which increases in individual flower size and inflorescence size make a difference to butterflies.

Acknowledgments

Work supported by FONDECYT Grant Nos. 1020956 and 7020956, Chilean Millennium Science Initiative Grant No. P02-051, Ecos Chile-France Grant C01B03, and the BBVA-2004 Prize in Conservation Biology. Cristian Torres acknowledges support of a Mecesup-Chile doctoral fellowship, and Jaime Martinez acknowledges support of an IEB masters fellowship. The authors thank an anonymous reviewer for drawing their attention to the stronger drop-off in percentage visitation with increasing display size in our butterfly-pollinated plant.

References Cited

1.

S. Andersson 2003. Foraging responses in the butterfly Inachis io, Aglais urticae (Nymphalidae) and Gonepteryx rhamni (Pieridae) to floral scents. Chemoecology 13:1–11. Google Scholar

2.

M. T. K. Arroyo, J. J. Armesto, and C. Villagrán . 1981. Plant phenological patterns in the high Andean cordillera of central Chile. Journal of Ecology 69:205–233. Google Scholar

3.

M. T. K. Arroyo, R. B. Primack, and J. J. Armesto . 1982. Community studies in pollination ecology in the high temperate Andes of central Chile. I. Pollination mechanisms and altitudinal variation. American Journal of Botany 69:82–97. Google Scholar

4.

M. T. K. Arroyo, J. J. Armesto, and R. B. Primack . 1985. Community studies in pollination ecology in the high temperate Andes of central Chile. II. Effect of temperature on visitation rates and pollination possibilities. Plant Systematics and Evolution 149:187–203. Google Scholar

5.

P. Bergman, U. Molau, and B. Holmgren . 1996. Micrometeorological impacts of insect activity and plant reproductive success in an alpine environment, Swedish Lapland. Arctic, Antarctic, and Alpine Research 28:196–202. Google Scholar

6.

J. M. Biernaskie and R. V. Cartar . 2004. Variation in rate of nectar production depends on floral display size: a pollinator manipulation hypothesis. Functional Ecology 18:125–129. Google Scholar

7.

L. A. Cavieres, A. Peñaloza, and M. T. K. Arroyo . 2000. Altitudinal vegetation belts in the high Andes of central Chile (33°S). Revista Chilena de Historia Natural 73:331–344. Google Scholar

8.

S. E. Donnelly, J. Christopher, C. J. Lortie, and L. W. Aarssen . 1998. Pollination in Verbascum thapsus (Scrophulariaceae): the advantage of being tall. American Journal of Botany 85:1618–1625. Google Scholar

9.

E. Elle and R. Carney . 2003. Reproductive assurance varies with flower size in Collinsia parviflora (Scrophulariaceae). American Journal of Botany 90:888–896. Google Scholar

10.

T. Fabbro and C. Körner . 2004. Altitudinal differences in flower traits and reproductive allocation. Flora 199:70–81. Google Scholar

11.

C. Galen 1996. Rates of floral evolution: Adaptation to bumblebee pollination in an alpine wildflower, Polemonium viscosum. Evolution 50:120–125. Google Scholar

12.

C. Galen and M. E. Newport . 1987. Bumble bee behavior and selection on flower size in the sky pilot, Polemonium viscosum. Oecologia 74:20–23. Google Scholar

13.

L. F. Galloway, T. Cirigliano, and K. Gremski . 2002. The contribution of display size and dichogamy to potential geitonogamy in Campanula americana. International Journal of Plant Sciences 163:133–139. Google Scholar

14.

D. Goulson, J. Ollerton, and C. Sluman . 1997. Foraging strategies in the small skipper butterfly, Thymelicus flavus: when to switch?. Animal Behaviour 53:1009–1016. Google Scholar

15.

J. M. Grindeland, N. Sletvold, and R. A. Ims . 2005. Effects of floral display size and plant density on pollinator visitation rate in a natural population of Digitalis purpurea. Functional Ecology 19:383–390. Google Scholar

16.

D. Gutíerrez and R. Menéndez . 1998. Phenology of butterflies along an altitudinal gradient in northern Spain. Journal of Zoology 244:249–264. Google Scholar

17.

L. D. Harder and S. C. H. Barrett . 1995. Mating cost of large floral displays in hermaphrodite plants. Nature 373:512–515. Google Scholar

18.

L. D. Harder, C. Y. Jordan, W. E. Gross, and M. B. Routley . 2004. Beyond floricentrism: the pollination function of inflorescences. Plant Species Biology 19:137–148. Google Scholar

19.

C. M. Herrera 1987. Components of pollinator ‘quality’: comparative analysis of a diverse insect assemblage. Oikos 50:79–90. Google Scholar

20.

K. Honda, H. Ômura, and N. Hayashi . 2004. Identification of floral volatiles from Ligustrum japonicum that stimulate flower-visiting by cabbage butterfly, Pieris rapae. Journal of Chemical Ecology 24:2167–2180. Google Scholar

21.

D. Ilse 1932. Zur Formwahrnehmung der Tagfalter. I. Spontane Bevorzugung von Formmerkmalen durch Vanessen. Zeitschrift für Vergleichende Physiologie 8:658–692. Google Scholar

22.

H. S. Ishii and S. Sakai . 2001. Effects of display size and position on individual floral longevity in racemes of Narthecium asiaticum (Liliaceae). Functional Ecology 15:396–405. Google Scholar

23.

Y. Iwasa, T. J. de Jong, and P. G. L. Kinkhamer . 1995. Why do pollinators visit only a fraction of the open flowers on a plant? The plant's point of view. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 8:439–453. Google Scholar

24.

J. D. Karron, R. J. Mitchell, J. M. Bell, and B. Funk . 2004. The influence of floral display size on selfing rates in Mimulus ringens. Heredity 92:242–248. Google Scholar

25.

A. Kelber 1997. Innate preferences for flower features in the hawkmoth Macroglossum stellatarum. The Journal of Experimental Biology 200:827–836. Google Scholar

26.

M. Kinoshita and K. Arikawa . 2000. Colour constancy in the swallowtail butterfly Papilio xuthus. The Journal of Experimental Biology 203:3521–3530. Google Scholar

27.

P. G. Klinkhamer, T. J. de Jong, and G. J. De Bruyn . 1989. Plant size and pollinator visitation in Cynoglossum officinale. Oikos 54:201–204. Google Scholar

28.

C. Körner 1999. Alpine plant life—Functional plant ecology of high mountain ecosystems. New York, Berlin Springer Verlag. Google Scholar

29.

G. Kudo and L. D. Harder . 2005. Floral and inflorescence effects on variation in pollen removal and seed production among six legume species. Functional Ecology 19:245–254. Google Scholar

30.

J. W. Merry, N. I. Morehouse, K. Yturralde, and R. L. Rutowski . 2006. The eyes of a patrolling butterfly: visual field and eye structure in the Orange Sulphur, Colias eurytheme (Lepidoptera, Pierdae). Journal of Insect Physiology 52:240–248. Google Scholar

31.

R. J. Mitchell, J. D. Karron, K. G. Holmquist, and J. M. Bell . 2004. The influence of Mimulus ringens floral display size on pollinator visitation patterns. Functional Ecology 18:116–124. Google Scholar

32.

D. D. Murphy 1984. Butterflies and their nectar plants: the role of the checkerspot butterfly Euphydryas editha as a pollen vector. Oikos 43:113–117. Google Scholar

33.

M. Ohara and S. Higashi . 1994. Effects of inflorescence size on visits from pollinators and seed set of Corydalis ambigua (Papaveraceae). Oecologia 98:25–30. Google Scholar

34.

K. Ohashi and T. Yahara . 1998. Effects of variation in flower number on pollinator visits in Cirsium purpuratum (Asteraceae). American Journal of Botany 85:219–224. Google Scholar

35.

K. Ohashi and T. Yahara . 1999. How long to stay on, and how often to visit a flowering plant?—A model for foraging strategy when floral display vary in size. Oikos 86:386–392. Google Scholar

36.

K. Ohashi and T. Yahara . 2001. Behavioral responses of pollinators to variation in floral display size and their influences on the evolution of floral traits. In L. Chittka and J. D. Thomson , editors. eds. Cognitive ecology of pollination Cambridge Cambridge University Press. 274–296. Google Scholar

37.

K. Ohashi and T. Yahara . 2002. Visit large displays but probe proportionately fewer flowers: counterintuitive behaviour of nectar-collecting bumble bees achieves an ideal free distribution. Functional Ecology 16:492–503. Google Scholar

38.

H. Ômura and K. Honda . 2005. Priority of color over scent during flower visitation by adult Vanessa indica butterflies. Oecologia 142:588–596. Google Scholar

39.

D. W. Stephens and J. R. Krebs . 1986. Foraging theory Princeton Princeton University Press. Google Scholar

40.

Ø Totland and B. Schulte-Herbrüggen . 2003. Breeding system, insect flower visitation, and floral traits of two alpine Cerastium species in Norway. Arctic, Antarctic, and Alpine Research 35:242–247. Google Scholar

41.

N. M. Waser 1982. A comparison of distances flown by different visitors to flowers of the same species. Oecologia 55:251–257. Google Scholar

42.

M. W. Weiss and D. R. Papaj . 2003. Colour learning in two behavioural contexts: how much can a butterfly keep in mind?. Animal Behaviour 65:425–434. Google Scholar

43.

C. Wiklund, T. Ericksson, and H. Lundberg . 1979. The wood white butterfly Leptidea sinapsis and its nectar plants: a case of mutualism of parasitism?. Oikos 33:358–362. Google Scholar
Mary T. K. Arroyo, Irene Till-Bottraud, Cristian Torres, Carolina A. Henríquez, and Jaime Martínez "Display Size Preferences and Foraging Habits of High Andean Butterflies Pollinating Chaetanthera lycopodioides (Asteraceae) in the Subnival of the Central Chilean Andes," Arctic, Antarctic, and Alpine Research 39(3), 347-352, (1 August 2007). https://doi.org/10.1657/1523-0430(06-017)[ARROYO]2.0.CO;2
Accepted: 1 December 2006; Published: 1 August 2007
Back to Top