Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
10 June 2022 Parasitism of the Katydid Neoconocephalus triops (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) by the Tachinid Flies Ormia lineifrons and Neomintho sp. (Diptera: Tachinidae)
Oliver M. Beckers
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Conspicuous mating signals of insects can be exploited by unintended predators and parasites to locate the signaler. Since these interactions can bear a substantial cost for the signaler, selection may cause changes in their signals, possibly contributing to the evolution of the communication system. Understanding the life history of the interacting species, especially that of the eavesdropper, is essential to better quantify the selective pressures in these interactions. The katydid, Neoconocephalus triops L. (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae), is parasitized by the lethal tachinid fly, Ormia lineifrons Sabrosky (Diptera: Tachinidae), in Florida. I collected N. triops in the field to characterize its parasitism by O. lineifrons and determine the efficiency of the fly's host use. The parasitism rate of N. triops was 48.2% and about half of the parasitized males (47.2%) were superparasitized. All parasitized katydids died and no larva that was the result of superparasitism survived the host's death. The average parasite load was 2.73 ± 1.20 larvae, and 49.5% of the fly pupae successfully developed into adult flies in 12.12 ± 0.60 d. Neoconocephalus triops also was parasitized by an undescribed species of Neomintho (Diptera: Tachinidae). The high superparasitism rate despite its low success suggests that O. lineifrons has not evolved traits to reliably distinguish between unparasitized and parasitized hosts. The high parasitism rate of N. triops suggests that O. lineifrons exerts substantial selective pressure on N. triops. However, the low developmental success of fly larvae may indicate that N. triops has evolved counter adaptations in its arms race with O. lineifrons, or N. triops might be a low-quality host.

Depredadores y parasitoides no intencionados pueden aprovechar las conspicuas señales de apareamiento de los insectos para localizar al emisor. Dado que estas interacciones pueden asumir un costo sustancial para el emisor de señales, la selección puede provocar cambios en sus señales, lo que posiblemente contribuya a la evolución del sistema de comunicación. Comprender la historia de vida de las especies que interactúan, especialmente la del espía, es esencial para cuantificar mejor las presiones selectivas en estas interacciones. La esperanza, Neoconocephalus triops L. (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae), es parasitado por la letal mosca taquínida, Ormia lineifrons Sabrosky (Diptera: Tachinidae) en la Florida. Recolecté N. triops en campo para caracterizar su parasitismo por O. lineifrons y determinar la eficiencia del uso de este como un hospedero de la mosca. La tasa de parasitismo de N. triops fue del 48,2% y aproximadamente la mitad de los machos parasitados (47,2%) estaban superparasitados. Todos las esperanzas parasitadas murieron y ninguna larva resultante del superparasitismo sobrevivió a la muerte del hospedero. El promedio de la razón de parasitismo fue de 2,73 ± 1,20 larvas y el 49,5 % de las pupas de mosca se convirtieron con éxito en moscas adultas en 12,12 ± 0,60 días. Neoconocephalus triops también fue parasitado por una especie no descrita del género Neomintho (Tachinidae). La alta tasa de superparasitismo a pesar de su bajo éxito sugiere que O. lineifrons no ha desarrollado rasgos para distinguir de manera confiable entre hopederos parasitados y no parasitados. La alta tasa de parasitismo de N. triops sugiere que O. lineifrons ejerce una presión selectiva sustancial sobre N. triops. Sin embargo, el bajo éxito en el desarrollo de las larvas de mosca puede sugerir que N. triops ha desarrollado adaptaciones defensivas en su carrera armamentista con O. lineifrons, o que N. triops podría ser un hospedero de baja calidad.

Acoustic insects produce conspicuous signals primarily to attract the opposite sex for mating (Gerhardt & Huber 2002). Evolution of these signals is complex and is driven typically by sexual selection based on receiver preferences or biases, and natural selection based on the signaling environment, signaling costs, and properties of receiver perception, to mention a few of the main factors (Endler & Basolo 1998). These signals also can be intercepted by illegitimate receivers (Cade 1975; Endler 1980, 1983; Zuk & Kolluru 1998; Hedwig & Robert 2014), such as eavesdropping parasites or predators, adding further selective pressures on the communication system, ultimately leading to its diversification (Belwood & Morris 1987; Zuk et al. 2006; Beckers & Wagner 2018; Tinghitella et al. 2018, 2021). Tachinid flies of the genus Ormia use orthopterans as hosts for their larvae (Lehmann 2003) and have caused a range of changes in the communication system of their hosts (Zuk et al. 2006; Beckers & Wagner 2018; Tinghitella et al. 2018, 2021), offering valuable opportunities to study fundamental questions of evolutionary ecology (Lehmann 2008). The extent to which the parasitoid exerts selection on its host is related substantially to its effectiveness of utilizing the host, which is the focus of this study.

Males of the conehead katydid, Neoconocephalus triops (Linnaeus) (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae), call at night to attract females. These mating calls are intercepted by the eavesdropping parasitoid fly, Ormia lineifrons Sabrosky (Diptera: Tachinidae) (Sabrosky 1953), that uses these mating calls to locate and parasitize N. triops (Burk 1982). The deposited fly larvae develop inside the host and emerge after 7 to 9 d, pupate, and metamorphose into adult flies (Burk 1982). Parasitized N. triops males continue to call for up to 5 d (Burk 1982), allowing for multiple parasitism events (i.e., superparasitism) during subsequent nights. The emergence of the parasitoid larvae from the host is lethal to the katydid and can reduce drastically the reproductive lifetime of the katydid from 2 to 3 mo to 1 to 2 wk (Burk 1982). Besides N. triops, the fly also uses other Neoconocephalus species, such as Neoconocephalus robustus (Scudder), Neoconocephalus velox Rehn & Hebard, and Neoconocephalus nebrascensis Bruner (all Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) as hosts in Kentucky (Rogers & Beckers 2022) as well as meadow katydids of the genus Orchelimum (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) (Feaver 1983; Shapiro 1995). Parasitism of silent females by this fly has not been observed.

In northern Florida, N. triops has 2 reproductive generations per yr, 1 in early spring and 1 in late summer (Whitesell 1974). Both generations are parasitized heavily by O. lineifrons, sometimes reaching parasitism rates of up to 100% (Burk 1982). However, besides the parasitism rate, not much else is known about the life history and host use efficiency of O. lineifrons. To further develop this understudied evolutionary arms race, data is presented here on the characteristics and efficiency of parasitism of N. triops by O. lineifrons. Additionally, a first-time report of parasitism of N. triops by an undescribed tachinid fly species is provided.

Material and Methods

ANIMAL HUSBANDRY

Neoconocephalus triops males (n = 86) were collected along roads and in fields within 15 Km (10 miles) west of Gainesville, Florida, USA, between 19 Jul and 21 Jul 2021. This time window corresponds to the population peak of the summer generation in this location (Burk 1982). The mating calls of male N. triops were used to find the katydids, which were collected by hand. The animals were transported to my lab at Murray State University, Murray, Kentucky, USA (USDA Permit #P526P-19-00002) for the study. Each male was placed in a separate cage (9 cm W × 16 cm L × 11 cm H) (Lee's Aquarium and Pet products, San Marcos, California, USA) with its lid lined with insect screen (small bug screen #14151, M-D Building Products, Inc., Oklahoma City, Oklahoma, USA) on the underside to prevent roaming fly larvae from escaping. The individual cages were sprayed daily with water and apple, and rolled oats were provided as food for the katydids ad libitum. The individual cages were placed inside an incubator (Model #PR505755L, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Marietta, Ohio, USA) with a light/dark cycle of 15:9 h (L:D) and high/low temperatures at 26.0 ± 1.0 °C and 22.0 ± 1.0 °C, respectively, which approximates a long summer d in northern Florida. The katydids were kept in their individual cages for at least 11 d after collection, which corresponded to 3 d after the last larva emerged, before they were used for breeding to maintain the laboratory stock. Note that O. lineifrons larvae emerge from N. triops within 9 d in Florida (Burk 1982), and the breeding enclosures were monitored for an additional 3 d after transferring the males and no dead male katydids were found in these enclosures.

The individual cages were checked daily for the presence of fly larvae or pupa and the date of their appearance was noted. On the day of emergence, pupae were placed on a piece of cotton (#3166, Dynarex Corporation, Orangeburg, New York, USA) inside a centrifuge tube (50 mL, Corning Science, Reynosa, Tamaulipas, Mexcio), and were sprayed with a saturated Methylparaben solution to restrict fungal growth and provide moisture. The lid of the tube had holes to allow for gas exchange. Larvae of the same host were placed in the same centrifuge tube and kept under the same conditions as the katydid hosts (see above). The tubes were checked daily for adult flies and the dates of appearance were noted. The hosts were kept inside their cages for 24 h after death to allow for additional larvae to emerge. After 24 h, the katydid was dissected under a dissecting scope (Stemi 1000, Carl Zeiss, Oberkochen, Germany) to search for larvae inside the host.

CALCULATIONS

The proportion of parasitized males was calculated by dividing the number of all males from which O. lineifrons larvae emerged and/or were found inside the host by the total number of collected males. In the closely related fly, Ormia ochracea (Bigot) (Diptera: Tachinidae) that uses field crickets as hosts, superparasitism after 24 h led to the death of the second clutch of larvae, whereas the first clutch emerged and killed the host (Adamo et al. 1995). Similarly, it was found that 24 h after the emergence-related death of N. triops hosts, all remaining larvae inside the hosts (n = 17 hosts) were dead at the time of dissection. The rate of superparasitism was estimated by determining the proportion of parasitized katydids that had both larvae emerged from their body and at least 1 dead larva inside their body relative to all parasitized katydids (n = 36). Five katydids that died before the larvae could emerge and had more than 1 larva inside were not included in this calculation since their superparasitism status could not be determined.

The developmental success rate from pupa to adult fly was determined by calculating for each host (n = 33) the proportion of larvae that pupated and developed into adult flies relative to all larvae that emerged from the host, i.e., each host contributed 1 data point ranging from 0 to 1. Eight pupae were damaged and were not used to calculate the success rate.

The pupal development time was calculated as the time span between emergence from the host and metamorphosis into the adult fly. Fly larvae pupate within a few h on the same d they emerge from the host (Adamo et al. 1995; personal observation). Note that the temperatures used to rear the pupae (see above) approximated summer conditions in northern Florida. However, data on habitat conditions used for pupation in the field (e.g., temperature, moisture) are not available and the reported development times are an approximation based on lab conditions. The average development time of all pupae that emerged from each host (n = 16) was calculated, i.e., each host contributed 1 data point. The developmental time of larvae from 3 hosts could not be determined because the indistinguishable pupae were stored in the same centrifuge tube but emerged and matured into adults on different d. Note that flies that pupated but did not develop into adult flies contributed data to the developmental success rate but not development time.

The sex of each adult fly was identified using the placement of the compound eyes, i.e., the eyes of O. lineifrons males almost touch on the dorsal side of the head, whereas the eyes of females clearly are separated (Sabrosky 1953), and the proportion of males and females was determined. One N. triops male was parasitized by a different fly species. The 2 flies were reared to adulthood as described above and were sent to James O'Hara at the Canadian National Collection of Insects, Arachnids, and Nematodes (Ottawa, Ontario, Canada) for identification. Data from this host were not used for any of the calculations outlined above.

Results

Out of 85 collected N. triops males, 41 were parasitized by O. lineifrons, which corresponds to a parasitism rate of 48.2%. None of the hosts that were parasitized survived the emergence of the larvae from their bodies. The values reported below are averages (± SD). The parasite load of N. triops males was 2.73 ± 1.21 larvae, of which 1.85 ± 1.35 larvae emerged from their hosts and pupated, and 0.88 ± 0.90 larvae were found inside their hosts. All larvae found inside a host were dead 24 h after the host's death. The development time from pupa to adult fly was 12.12 ± 0.60 d. The developmental success of O. lineifrons pupae to adult flies across all host individuals was 49.54 ± 44.65%. Of those pupae that successfully developed into adults, 21 were females (65.6%) and 11 were males (34.4%); however this ratio was not significantly different from a 1:1 sex ratio (Chi square test: χ2 = 3.125; P = 0.077). Of all parasitized males, 47.2% were superparasitized, which corresponded to 20.0% of all collected males.

One N. triops male was parasitized by an undescribed Neomintho sp. (Tachinidae), and this is the first report of parasitism by this fly. The host contained 3 larvae, 2 of which pupated and developed into 1 male and 1 female adult Neomintho fly after 18 d, whereas the third larvae was found dead inside the expired host, suggesting a case of superparasitism.

Discussion

The parasitism rate determined for Jul (48.2%) was similar to those of N. triops from the same location and mo in 1980 and 1981 (38–43%) (Burk 1982) and was comparable to the parasitism rate of another tachinid fly, Therobia leonidei (Mesnil) (Diptera:Tachinidae), that parasitizes the katydid Poecilimon marianne Willemse & Heller (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) at a rate of 50 to 57% (Lehmann 2008). In contrast, parasitism rates of O. ochracea that uses field crickets as hosts are lower and range between 3% in Gryllus rubens (Scudder) (Orthoptera: Gryllidae) (Walker & Wineriter 1991) to 28% in Teleogryllus oceanicus (Le Guillo) (Orthoptera: Gryllidae) (Zuk et al. 1993). The high parasitism rate of N. triops males by O. lineifrons indicates substantial selection pressure on male traits to reduce the adverse effects of parasitism, potentially leading to male satellite behavior (Cade 1975; Bertram et al. 2004), reduced calling (Zuk et al. 2006), change of call features (Tinghitella et al. 2021), calling activity (Cade et al. 1996), or calling in aggregations to reduce the risk of detection (Goodale et al. 2019; Lehmann & Lakes-Harlan 2019) to mention a few possible adaptations documented in other Ormia host species. Such changes related to the eavesdropping parasitoids can introduce changes in signals and signal preferences, ultimately having the potential for the communication or mating system to evolve.

About half of the parasitized N. triops males were superparasitized (47.2%) by O. lineifrons. For reference, O. ochracea superparasitism of parasitized crickets ranges between 4.7% (Kolluru & Zuk 2001) and 25% (Adamo et al. 1995), and that of Therobia leonidei using the katydid Poecilimon thessalicus Brunner & Wattenwyl (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) is 17.6% (Lehmann 2008). The high rate of superparasitism in N. triops was unexpected, considering that a substantial proportion (> 50%) of unparasitized N. triops males was available as hosts. Potential preferences of O. lineifrons for particular call features (Wagner 1996; Lehmann et al. 2001) may explain why some katydid males were parasitized more than once, i.e., superparasitized, while others were not parasitized. Superparasitism, despite its ineffectiveness (see below), suggests that O. lineifrons, like other tachinid parasitoids (Adamo et al. 1995; Lehmann 2008), may not to be able to discriminate between unparasitized and parasitized hosts. In contrast to parasitic Hymenoptera, tachinids do not have a piercing ovipositor and typically do not contact their hosts during larviposition (Lehmann 2008), providing little opportunity to directly determine the parasitism status of the host. Also, changes in the calls (Lehmann & Lehmann 2006) that could have indicated the host's parasitism status likely did not occur or were too small to be detected by O. lineifrons.

Dissections of the hosts 24 h after death indicate that larvae that did not synchronize their emergence with the larvae that emerged before them had a very low chance of survival. Evolutionary theory suggests that the direct benefits to the mother and indirect benefits to genetic siblings are expected to exert strong selection on synchronous emergence of related larvae. Along this line of reasoning, the stragglers would represent larvae placed on the host in separate infestation events, i.e., superparasitism. However, superparasitism may be beneficial to the fly if it occurs within the same night because this would allow for such synchronization with larvae of an earlier infestation to happen. Note that if superparasitism occurred during the same night, these instances could not be distinguished from regular parasitism, suggesting that the superparasitism rates of N. triops in Florida could be even higher than reported here. Overall, superparasitism of N. triops by O. lineifrons was mostly ineffective, which represents a pattern that was shared with the parasitoid flies T. leonidei (Lehmann 2008) and O. ochracea (Adamo et al. 1995).

The developmental success of emerged fly pupae, with about half of them not developing to adulthood, was surprisingly low. This suggests that N. triops might be a low-quality host (e.g., poor nutritional resource) (Mackauer et al. 1996) for O. lineifrons larvae. Note that O. lineifrons parasitizes other Neoconocephalus species in Kentucky (Rogers & Beckers 2022) and Florida (Theodore Burk, personal communication), as well as Orchelimum species (Feaver 1983; Shapiro 1995), and it may be better adapted to these, potentially higher-quality hosts. It also may be possible that in the arms race between the host and its parasitoid, N. triops is currently ahead and has evolved better defenses (e.g., a more efficient immune response) (Thomson et al. 2012; Wilson & Cotter 2013) that interfere with the development of the fly. However, further research is necessary to better understand the low developmental success rate of the flies in N. triops.

The collection of N. triops indicated that another previously unknown tachinid fly species of the genus Neomintho parasitizes this katydid in Florida. Currently, nothing is known about the ecology and life history of this fly. In contrast to O. lineifrons and other members of Ormiini, members of Euthelarini, to which Neomintho belongs, typically do not have specialized sterna with tympana (Wood & Zumbado 2010) indicating that this fly is not using N. triops mating calls to find its hosts. Further study on this species is necessary to understand how Neomintho finds its host(s) and what evolutionary impact it may have on N. triops.

Acknowledgments

I want to thank James O'Hara (Ottawa, Ontario, Canada) for his help with identifying the newly discovered fly. I thank the 2 anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on the manuscript. I am indebted to Tom Walker, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA, for his advice on N. triops, identifying collection sites in Gainesville, and providing space for storage of the animals during my collection. I also want to thank Stephen Shey for supporting my collections on his property. This research was supported by an award of the National Science Foundation to OMB (NSF #1755118).

References Cited

1.

Adamo SA, Robert D, Perez J, Hoy RR. 1995. The response of an insect parasitoid, Ormia ochracea (Tachinidae), to the uncertainty of larval success during infestation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 36: 111–118. Google Scholar

2.

Beckers OM, Wagner Jr WE. 2018. Males and females evolve riskier traits in populations with eavesdropping parasitoids. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 72: 17. doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2588-1 Google Scholar

3.

Belwood JJ, Morris GK. 1987. Bat predation and its influence on calling behavior in Neotropical katydids. Science 238: 64–67. Google Scholar

4.

Bertram SM, Xochitl Orozco S, Bellani R. 2004. Temporal shifts in conspicuousness: mate attraction displays of the Texas field cricket, Gryllus texensis . Ethology 110: 963–975. Google Scholar

5.

Burk T. 1982. Evolutionary significance of predation on sexually signaling males. Florida Entomologist 65: 90–104. Google Scholar

6.

Cade WH. 1975. Acoustically orienting parasitoids: fly phonotaxis to cricket song. Science 190: 1312–1313. Google Scholar

7.

Cade WH, Ciceran M, Murray AM. 1996. Temporal patterns of parasitoid fly (Ormia ochracea) attraction to field cricket song (Gryllus integer). Canadian Journal of Zoology 74: 393–395. Google Scholar

8.

Endler JA. 1980. Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia reticulata. Evolution 34: 76–91. Google Scholar

9.

Endler JA. 1983. Testing causal hypotheses in the study of geographical variation, pp. 424–443 In Felsenstein J [Ed.], Numerical Taxonomy. Springer, Berlin, Germany. Google Scholar

10.

Endler JA, Basolo AL. 1998. Sensory ecology, receiver biases and sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13: 415–420. Google Scholar

11.

Feaver M. 1983. Pair formation in the katydid Orchelimum nigripes (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae), pp. 205–239 In Gwynne DT, Morris GK [Eds.], Orthopteran Mating Systems: Sexual Competition in a Diverse Group of Insects. West-view Press, Boulder, Colorado, USA. Google Scholar

12.

Gerhardt HC, Huber F. 2002. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and Diverse Solutions. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Illinois, USA. Google Scholar

13.

Goodale E, Ruxton GD, Beauchamp G. 2019. Predator eavesdropping in a mixed-species environment: how prey species may use grouping, confusion, and the cocktail party effect to reduce predator detection. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 7: 141. doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2019.00141 Google Scholar

14.

Hedwig B, Robert D. 2014. Auditory parasitoid flies exploiting acoustic communication of insects, pp. 45–63 In Insect Hearing and Acoustic Communication. Animal Signals and Communication, Vol 1. Springer, Berlin, Germany. Google Scholar

15.

Kolluru GR, Zuk M. 2001. Parasitism patterns and the size-fecundity relationship in the acoustically orienting dipteran parasitoid Ormia ochracea. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79: 973–979. Google Scholar

16.

Lehmann GU. 2003. Review of biogeography, host range and evolution of acoustic hunting in Ormiini (Insecta, Diptera, Tachinidae), parasitoids of night-calling bushcrickets and crickets (Insecta, Orthoptera, Ensifera). Zoologischer Anzeiger-A Journal of Comparative Zoology 242: 107–120. Google Scholar

17.

Lehmann GU. 2008. How different host species influence parasitism patterns and larval competition of acoustically-orienting parasitoid flies (Tachinidae: Ormiini), pp. 93–132 In Krause L, Weber E [Eds.], Animal Behavior: New Research. Nova Science Publishers, New York, USA. Google Scholar

18.

Lehmann GU, Lakes-Harlan R. 2019. Adaptive strategies in life-history of bushcrickets (Orthoptera) and cicadas (Homoptera) to parasitoids pressure on their acoustic communication systems–a case for sociality? Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution 7: 295. doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2019.00295 Google Scholar

19.

Lehmann GU, Lehmann AW. 2006. Potential lifetime reproductive success of male bushcrickets parasitized by a phonotactic fly. Animal Behaviour 71: 1103–1110. Google Scholar

20.

Lehmann GU, Heller KG, Lehmann AW. 2001. Male bushcrickets favoured by parasitoid flies when acoustically more attractive for conspecific females (Orthoptera: Phanopteridae/Diptera: Tachinidae). Entomologia Generalis 25: 135–140. Google Scholar

21.

Mackauer M, Michaud JP, Völkl W. 1996. Host choice by aphidiid parasitoids (Hymenoptera: Aphidiidae): host recognition, host quality, and host value. The Canadian Entomologist 128: 959–980. (Invitation paper, CP Alexander Fund) Google Scholar

22.

Rogers KJ, Beckers OM. 2022. Parasitism of Neoconocephalus katydids by the parasitoid fly, Ormia lineifrons. Ethology 128: 111–118. Google Scholar

23.

Sabrosky CW. 1953. Taxonomy and host relations of the tribe Ormiini in the Western Hemisphere (Diptera, Larvaevoridae). Proceedings of the Entomological Society of Washington 55: 167–183. Google Scholar

24.

Shapiro L. 1995. Parasitism of Orchelimum katydids (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) by Ormia lineifrons (Diptera: Tachinidae). Florida Entomologist 78: 615–616. Google Scholar

25.

Thomson IR, Vincent CM, Bertram SM. 2012. Success of the parasitoid fly Ormia ochracea (Diptera: Tachinidae) on natural and unnatural cricket hosts. Florida Entomologist 95: 43–48. Google Scholar

26.

Tinghitella RM, Broder ED, Gallagher JH, Wikle AW, Zonana DM. 2021. Responses of intended and unintended receivers to a novel sexual signal suggest clandestine communication. Nature Communications 12: 1–10. Google Scholar

27.

Tinghitella RM, Broder ED, Gurule-Small GA, Hallagan CJ, Wilson JD. 2018. Purring crickets: the evolution of a novel sexual signal. The American Naturalist 192: 773–782. Google Scholar

28.

Wagner Jr WE. 1996. Convergent song preferences between female field crickets and acoustically orienting parasitoid flies. Behavioral Ecology 7: 279–285. Google Scholar

29.

Walker TJ, Wineriter SA. 1991. Hosts of a phonotactic parasitoid and levels of parasitism (Diptera: Tachinidae: Ormia ochracea). Florida Entomologist 74: 554–559. Google Scholar

30.

Whitesell JJ. 1974. Geographic Variation and Dimorphisms in Song, Development, and Color in a Katydid; Field and Laboratory Studies (Tettigoniidae, Orthoptera). PhD dissertation, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida, USA. Google Scholar

31.

Wilson K, Cotter SC. 2013. Host-parasite interactions and the evolution of immune defense. Advances in the Study of Behavior 45: 81–174. Google Scholar

32.

Wood DM, Zumbado MA. 2010. Tachinidae (tachinid flies, parasitic flies), pp. 1343–1417 In Brown BV, Borkent A, Cumming JM, Wood DM, Woodley NE, Zumbado MA [Eds.], Manual of Central American Diptera. Vol. 2. NRC Research Press, Ottawa, Canada. Google Scholar

33.

Zuk M, Kolluru GR. 1998. Exploitation of sexual signals by predators and parasitoids. The Quarterly Review of Biology 73: 415–438. Google Scholar

34.

Zuk M, Rotenberry JT, Tinghitella RM. 2006. Silent night: adaptive disappearance of a sexual signal in a parasitized population of field crickets. Biology Letters 2: 521–524. Google Scholar

35.

Zuk M, Simmons LW, Cupp L. 1993. Calling characteristics of parasitized and unparasitized populations of the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 33: 339–343. Google Scholar
Oliver M. Beckers "Parasitism of the Katydid Neoconocephalus triops (Orthoptera: Tettigoniidae) by the Tachinid Flies Ormia lineifrons and Neomintho sp. (Diptera: Tachinidae)," Florida Entomologist 105(2), 133-136, (10 June 2022). https://doi.org/10.1653/024.105.0205
Published: 10 June 2022
KEYWORDS
eavesdropping
escuchas clandestinas
parasitoid
parasitoide
superparasitism
superparasitismo
Back to Top