BioOne.org will be down briefly for maintenance on 17 December 2024 between 18:00-22:00 Pacific Time US. We apologize for any inconvenience.
Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
16 June 2007 CHEMICAL DISCRIMINATION OF FOOD, CONSPECIFICS AND PREDATORS BY APPARENTLY VISUALLY-ORIENTED DIURNAL GECKOS, NAULTINUS MANUKANUS
Joanne M. Hoare, Shirley Pledger, Nicola J. Nelson
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Animal life history strategies evolve in response to a range of selective pressures, and often are tightly linked to sensory cues. Squamate reptiles possess well-developed chemosensory systems, which evolved early in their history and remain highly conserved. However, chemosensory abilities are secondarily reduced in primarily visual agamid and chameleonid lizards, which casts doubt on the ubiquity of reliance on chemosensory cues among squamates. Diurnal geckos from New Zealand avoid predators using visual crypsis and are primarily ambush foragers; reliance on visual cues may be a product of strong avian predation pressure and minimal influence from mammals and snakes for the past 80 my. We used New Zealand's diurnal geckos to test the role of local adaptation versus evolutionary conservatism in defining chemosensory systems. Specifically, we tested whether Marlborough green geckos, Naultinus manukanus, use chemoreception to detect and behaviorally respond to (1) food, (2) opposite-sex conspecifics, and (3) native reptilian predators (tuatara, Sphenodon punctatus). Chemosensory cues mediated gecko interactions with all three stimuli tested: (1) the scent of fruit induced greater exploratory behavior, (2) male geckos responded to scent of female conspecifics by increasing lingual sampling and activity, and (3) tuatara fecal cues (but not skin secretions) elicited characteristic antipredator freeze behavior in geckos. Neither the primarily visual life history strategies nor the reduced range of predators of New Zealand's diurnal geckos appears to have reduced their chemosensory abilities relative to other noniguanid squamate reptiles. Instead, our findings support the notion that chemosensory traits remain highly conserved.

Many species display suites of morphological and behavioral characteristics influenced by the primary sensory mechanism that they use. Most squamate reptiles have highly developed chemosensory systems, which mediate a range of intra- and inter-specific interactions (Burghardt, 1970; Gabe and Saint Girons, 1976; Halpern, 1992; Schwenk, 1995). The evolution of chemosensory systems is considered conservative, and largely reflects suprafamilial attributes as opposed to local adaptation (Schwenk, 1993). However, agamid and chameleonid lizards are ambush foragers that rely on visual cues (Cooper et al., 2001; Schwenk, 1993). As a consequence, these families have secondarily reduced chemosensory abilities (Gabe and Saint Girons, 1976; Parsons, 1959, 1970). Secondary loss of chemosensory abilities in some squamate families prompts the question of which criteria are necessary to precipitate an evolutionary switch from primarily chemosensory to primarily visual antipredator and foraging strategies. We investigate the chemosensory abilities of gekkotans that appear to display primarily visual antipredator and prey detection strategies, perhaps as a result of their evolutionary isolation from many ground-dwelling predators and diurnal activity. Using this system, we explore the roles of local adaptation versus evolutionary conservatism in shaping chemoreception.

The highly developed nasal chemosensory systems of most squamate reptiles enable their discrimination of many chemicals (reviewed by Schwenk, 1995). Chemoreception in reptiles is known to be mediated by both the olfactory and vomeronasal systems, but other nasal chemical senses also may be involved (Halpern, 1992). Tongue flicking allows lingual sampling of both substrates and the air via the vomeronasal organ, which enables discrimination between chemical signals and mediates behavioral responses (‘vomerolfaction’; sensu Cooper, 1996; Halpern, 1992). Reptiles frequently rely on chemical cues to detect and avoid predators (Dial et al., 1989; Downes, 2002; Downes and Shine, 1998). Olfaction and vomerolfaction also can mediate interactions with conspecifics (Bull et al., 2000; Cooper and Pèrez-Mellado, 2002; Cooper and Steele, 1997) and be used to locate prey and other food (Cooper and Habegger, 2000; Cooper and Pèrez-Mellado, 2001).

Reptiles often use chemoreception in combination with other sensory cues, such as vision (Head et al., 2005). Under certain local conditions, use of visual cues may assume greater importance relative to chemosensory cues (e.g., in agamid and chameleonid lizards; Schwenk, 1993). Although gekkotans have highly developed chemosensory abilities (Cooper, 1995; Dial et al., 1989; Downes and Shine, 1999; Schwenk, 1993), some have evolved secondary diurnality, which may be associated with a switch to using primarily visual strategies. For example, diurnal green geckos, Naultinus spp., from New Zealand display visual crypsis and secretive behavior as antipredator strategies and appear to ambush their mostly invertebrate prey (Hare et al., in press; Hitchmough, 1978; Whitaker and Gaze, 1999).

The reliance of New Zealand diurnal geckos on visual cues to avoid predators may be a consequence of evolution in isolation from major predatory groups. On a global scale, major lizard predators are birds, reptiles (especially snakes) and mammals. New Zealand had few terrestrial mammalian predators for ca. 80 my prior to the introduction of Polynesian rats, Rattus exulans, by Polynesian voyagers ca. 1800 ya (Holdaway, 1999; Worthy et al., 2006), and today mammalian predators remain absent from many offshore islands. Furthermore, there are no confirmed reports of terrestrial snakes in the New Zealand fossil record. Birds are the primary native predators on New Zealand lizards, and tuatara (Sphenodon spp.) also prey upon lizards (Walls, 1981). Because birds usually detect their prey from a distance using visual cues, crypsis may assume a greater role in predator avoidance than the ability of prey to detect their predators using chemical cues. Similarly, tuatara appear to use primarily visual foraging cues to detect their prey (Meyer-Rochow, 1988; Meyer-Rochow and Teh, 1991; Walls, 1981). In contrast, other reptiles (especially snakes) and mammals often use chemical cues to detect prey at close range, thus chemoreception may play a key role in predator detection and avoidance by prey (Downes and Shine, 1998). The visual antipredator strategy used by New Zealand's diurnal geckos may reflect an avian dominated predatory fauna, in contrast with the range of predators that tend to elicit chemosensory responses among reptiles on a global scale.

The apparent lack of selection pressure to detect and avoid predators using chemoreception in New Zealand lizards may be countered to some degree by use of chemosensory cues in foraging and in mediating intraspecific interactions. New Zealand lizards (Diplodactylidae and Scincidae) consume fruits and nectar (Lawrence, 1997; Whitaker, 1987; Wotton, 2002), which indicates likely use of chemoreception to find food sources. However, records of frugivory by the diurnal gecko genus Naultinus are sparse (Whitaker, 1987), which leaves both the importance of fruit as a food source and the mechanism involved in finding it unresolved. Chemosensory conspecific recognition and predator detection have not been examined in New Zealand geckos, but many possess distinctive femoral pores (e.g., Gill and Whitaker, 1996; Hitchmough, 1982), which may indicate the use of chemical signals to mediate conspecific interactions.

Adaptation to a reduced suite of predators and associated use of a visually-oriented strategy by New Zealand's diurnal geckos provides an opportunity to test the role of local adaptation versus evolutionary conservatism in squamate chemosensory systems. We specifically address whether chemosensory capabilities are reduced in lizards that use primarily visual antipredator and foraging strategies by investigating chemosensory-mediated responses of diurnal, Marlborough green geckos, Naultinus manukanus, to fruit, conspecifics and predators.

Materials and Methods

Study Animals

Naultinus manukanus are small (up to 81 mm snout–vent length; Hare et al., in press) green, diurnal geckos which are restricted to the Marlborough region of New Zealand and occur in forest and scrub habitat (Gill and Whitaker, 1996). These arboreal geckos exhibit limited movement patterns and appear to rely primarily on visual crypsis and secretive behavior to avoid predators (Hare et al., in press; Hitchmough, 1978; Whitaker and Gaze, 1999).

Eighteen adult geckos (snout–vent length 62–81 mm; mass 5.7–11.9 g) captured from Takapourewa (Stephens Island), Cook Strait, New Zealand were held at Victoria University of Wellington between March and October 2003. Because Takapourewa has no introduced mammals or snakes (Brown, 2000), lizards experienced a natural predation regime including only birds and reptiles (other than snakes) prior to being taken into captivity. All 18 adult geckos (nine of each sex, all females pregnant) were tested for behavioral responses to scent stimuli (fruit, conspecifics and native predators) over a 3 wk period in October 2003 during the normal mating season (Gill and Whitaker, 1996). Additionally, 10 predator-naïve juvenile geckos (SVL 43–49 mm; mass 1.8–2.6 g), born to captive-held mothers, were tested for behavioral responses to the scent of their native tuatara predators.

Adult geckos were kept individually in transparent plastic containers (330 × 215 × 110 mm, L × W × H) with 1 × 1 mm wire mesh (165 × 120 mm) in the lids. Food and water were provided ad libitum; all other husbandry details are outlined by Hare et al. (2004). Juveniles were held under the same conditions as adults, but were housed in their birth groups (usually two individuals; three individuals in one case when one juvenile had no siblings).

Experimental Design

We tested behavioral responses of geckos to chemical cues from fruit, conspecific geckos and native reptilian predators to gauge the extent to which chemoreception mediated interactions. Because lizards use tongue flicks to sample substrates and airborne volatile compounds (vomerolfaction), the rate of tongue flicks provides an objective index of chemosensory investigation (Cooper, 1998; Cooper and Pèrez-Mellado, 2002). We also recorded a range of other behaviors to provide context for any changes observed in the rate of tongue flicks (Downes and Shine, 1998), including maintenance behavior (eye-licking), activity and exploratory behaviors (walking, running, climbing, moving limbs and nudging the wall of the arena) and ‘freeze’ behavior.

The experimental arena was a clear plastic enclosure (280 × 250 × 210 mm, L × W × H) containing a plain cotton cloth of the same dimensions on the base of the enclosure and covered with the scent to be tested. For juvenile geckos, the basal area of the experimental arena was reduced (to 250 × 130 mm) using a divider of the same material as the enclosure walls.

Scent Treatments and Controls

The cotton scent cloth was washed between trials to remove any scent residue by soaking the cloth in the disinfectant Napisan™ (active ingredient Sodium Percarbonate) for 2 h, rinsing thoroughly, air-drying and steam ironing. Behaviors of geckos presented with a washed cloth previously covered in each scent stimulus did not differ from a negative control (washed cloth which had not been in contact with any of the scents tested; F(21, 51.1)  =  0.510, P  =  0.954), confirming that the washing methodology was effective at removing all scents.

Scent treatments and controls were: (1) negative control: unscented cloth; (2) positive control: cloth sprayed with commercial cologne (‘Smiley’ Second Edition No. 42; Liberty Cosmetics Ltd, England); (3) pear: cloth sprayed with a 1:1 solution of natural pear juice and distilled water; (4) conspecific: cloth placed in an enclosure with a conspecific adult gecko of the opposite sex for 4–7 d; (5) native predator skin secretions: cloth placed in an enclosure with an adult tuatara, Sphenodon punctatus, for a period of 16 to 24 h; (6) native predator feces: cloth covered in a slurry of tuatara faeces and distilled water.

Experimental Procedure

To avoid handling immediately prior to the experiment, geckos were placed into a plastic jar ca. 2 min prior to the experiment. The top one-third of the inner surface of the plastic jar was coated with Fluon® (Tetrafluoroethylene co-polymer), a paint with low surface tension to which geckos cannot adhere (Losos et al., 2001). Geckos were gently tipped into the enclosure at the start of the experiment.

Observations of behavior commenced ca. 10 s after transfer of the gecko to the experimental enclosure. Behavior of each gecko was observed continuously for 12 min from behind a cardboard blind by a single observer. The observational data-logging programme ODLog™ (Macropod Software) was used to directly record behavior onto the computer.

Behaviors Recorded

Either the frequency (behaviors 1 and 2 below) or the duration (3 to 8) of the following behavioral acts and locomotor patterns was recorded: (1) tongue flick: tongue is extruded and either waved in the air or lowered to touch the substrate; (2) eye lick: tongue is slowly extruded and passed over an eye; (3) slow walk: gecko proceeds by very slow, stalking or continuous directional movement; (4) run: very fast horizontal locomotion on base of enclosure; (5) climb: directional movement on a wall; (6) non-locomotory movement: gecko moves body parts but does not alter its location; (7) nudge wall: gecko presses its snout against a wall; (8) freeze: gecko remains motionless.

Relative to a negative control, we predicted that scent from fruit and conspecifics would elicit elevated tongue flicking rates and greater exploratory behaviors by geckos, including either climbing or slow, deliberate walking. In contrast, because visual crypsis is the usual mechanism by which these geckos avoid native predators, we predicted that tuatara scent would elicit a higher incidence of freeze behavior and decreased movements.

Experimental Conditioning and Scent Tests

All geckos were conditioned to the experiment by undergoing three practice tests with a negative control prior to scent trials to minimize the influence of the novel experimental procedure on observed behaviors (Hare et al., 2004). The role of conditioning in minimizing behavioral variation was tested using data from the three successive conditioning trials recorded for nine adults and eight juveniles.

We constructed a Wilks Lambda multivariate test to investigate whether the novel environment of the test arena influenced the behavior of geckos and, if so, whether successive conditioning trials reduced behavioral variation associated with the novel test environment. Individual and trial numbers were included as factors in the MANOVA. We compared a complete model (three levels of trial number) with reduced models (merged trial effects) using Wilks Lambda and approximate F tests, based on appropriate sums of squares and cross-products matrices. Significant behavioral variation was observed between the first conditioning trial and subsequent trials (F21,63.72  =  3.615, P < 0.001), but not between the second and third conditioning trials (F21,63.72  =  1.454, P  =  0.128), suggesting that the novel test environment influenced behavior during the first conditioning trial only. Our methodology involving three conditioning trials prior to experimental trials therefore was sufficient to minimize the influence of a novel environment on gecko behavior, and this result is consistent with findings from conditioning prior to physiological experiments in this species (Hare et al., 2004).

All 18 adult N. manukanus were tested for behavioral responses to the negative control and to conspecific scent. From these, 10 adults were randomly selected to perform all other scent tests. All 10 juvenile geckos were tested for behavioral responses to tuatara fecal cues against a negative control. Geckos were tested a maximum of one time on any day, and the order of scent tests was randomly allocated. All tests were conducted within an ambient temperature range (18.0–20.3 C) at which geckos are active in nature (Werner and Whitaker, 1978) and during the normal activity phase of this species (0900 to 2000 h NZDT; Hare et al., in press).

Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed using the statistical programme ‘R’ (version 1.9.1; R Development Core Team, 2004). Count data were transformed using square roots to normalize data for analyses. Statistical significance was assigned at α  =  0.05. All data are presented as mean ± SE, unless stated otherwise. All data satisfied the assumptions of the statistical tests used.

The abilities of adult geckos to detect chemical cues of fruit, conspecifics, predator skin secretions and predator feces were assessed based on behavioral responses to these in relation to negative and positive controls. Additionally, any ontogenetic change in chemosensory-mediated antipredator response was investigated by comparing adult and juvenile N. manukanus responses to tuatara fecal cues relative to a negative control.

Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs), using the F-approximation to the Wilks test, enabled us to assess the overall importance of the scent stimulus (as an independent variable), in explaining variation in behavioral activities (the correlated dependent variables) and to provide protection against multiple testing of several response variables. Temperature, time of trial (early: 0900–1200 h; mid: 1200–1600 h; late: 1600–2000 h), sex, ontogenetic stage (adult or juvenile) and individual were added as independent variables in the relevant MANOVAs, and interactions between these variables and scent stimuli were assessed. We assessed correlations between behavioral activities using a Bonferroni-adjusted Spearman's correlation analysis.

Significant MANOVA effects were explored via detailed testing using linear mixed effects models (Hair et al., 2006). One univariate ANOVA was performed for each behavioral activity (as separate dependent variables) to assess the relative importance of significant variables (from MANOVA) in determining behavior. Individual was included as a random factor in these analyses to account for it as a source of variation (Sih et al., 2004; Stapley and Keogh, 2004), as is necessary with the repeated measures experimental design.

Results

Behavioral Responses to Scent Stimuli

The positive control (cologne) induced geckos to elevate their rate of tongue flicks (t48  =  2.27, P  =  0.03) and increase overall activity (t48  =  2.08, P  =  0.04), but other behavioral activities were not affected (P > 0.05 for all). Encountering a novel scent stimulus, therefore, resulted in greater vomerolfactory response and exploratory behavior, but did not alter other components of behavior. The observed response to the positive control demonstrates that our experimental protocol is valid for N. manukanus, and sets a standard from which to explore recognition of fruit, conspecific and predator scents.

Average tongue flick rate for all scent stimuli pooled was 2.75 flicks/min (SE  =  0.13, range 0.08–6.42), whereas average eye lick rate was only 0.33 licks/min (SE  =  0.04, range 0–1.75). Geckos spent almost one-half (45.8%) of their time during trials motionless, although this varied according to the scent stimuli presented (Fig. 1). The most common activity (34.1% of time) was climbing, followed by movements of head, limbs or tail (8.2%), nudging the wall (7.2%) and slow walking (4.7%; Fig. 1). Not surprisingly, climbing was negatively associated with slow walking, movements of head, limbs or tail, and freeze behavior (Table 1). Exploratory slow walking was positively correlated with tongue flicking rate and movements of head, limbs or tail (Table 1). Running (flight response) accounted for less than 0.1% of time (Fig. 1).

Figure 1

Percent time spent in alternate behavior patterns when adult Naultinus manukanus were presented with different scent treatments. Time spent running was negligible and thus does not show on the graph.

i0018-0831-63-2-184-f01.gif

Table 1

Correlations between activities of Marlborough green geckos and their significance based on Bonferroni-adjusted Spearman's correlation analysis. Significant Spearman's coefficients (bold) indicate one behavior increasing or decreasing (as indicated by the sign) the likelihood of another. See Methods and Materials for definitions of behaviors.

i0018-0831-63-2-184-t01.gif

Adult geckos responded to the chemical cues of fruit, conspecifics and native predators (Table 2). Individual variation in behavior was highly significant (Table 2). The only intersexual difference in behavioral patterns (Table 2) was that females spent more time in slow, deliberate walking than males (t16  =  2.93, P  =  0.01). Because pregnancy is expected to decrease mobility in female geckos (Downes and Bauwens, 2002), increased walking suggests that reproductive condition did not influence female behavior. We observed an interactive effect between sex and scent, whereas there were no effects of temperature or time of trial (Table 2).

Table 2

Factors influencing behavior of Naultinus manukanus. ‘F’ is the F-approximation to the Wilks test.

i0018-0831-63-2-184-t02.gif

Fruit Scent

Geckos did not show an elevated tongue flicking rate in response to fruit scent (Fig. 2a). However, fruit scent elicited more climbing behavior (t48  =  2.57, P  =  0.01; Fig. 2b) and a decrease in two correlated behaviours (Table 2): slow, deliberate walking (t48  =  −3.33, P < 0.01) and time spent motionless (t48  =  −2.17, P  =  0.04). Males spent more time in slow, deliberate walking in response to the scent of fruit than females (t48  =  3.02, P < 0.01).

Figure 2

Behavioral responses of Naultinus manukanus to the scent of pear (a and b), opposite-sex conspecific scent (c and d) and tuatara feces (e and f) during a 12-min trial. Females are represented by filled circles; males are represented by open squares. Data are mean values corrected for individual variation (±1.0 SE).

i0018-0831-63-2-184-f02.gif

Conspecific Scent

Male geckos showed a higher tongue flicking response to conspecific scent than did females (t48  =  2.83, P < 0.01; Fig. 2c). Males also increased time spent in slow, deliberate walking in response to the scent of conspecific females, relative to a negative control (t48  =  2.33, P  =  0.02; Fig. 2d).

Predator Scent

The response of adult geckos to native predator (tuatara) scent was dependent upon the cue provided. Tuatara skin secretions elicited only a higher rate of eye licking (t48  =  2.89, P < 0.01), but their feces reduced both tongue flicks (t48  =  −2.21, P  =  0.03; Fig. 2e) and slow, deliberate walking (t48  =  −3.43, P < 0.01) which are correlated behaviors (Table 1). Tuatara fecal cues also induced geckos to spend more time motionless (t48  =  2.11, P  =  0.04; Fig. 2f). There was a trend for a disproportionate reduction in tongue flicks in response to tuatara scent by females relative to males (Fig. 2), but there was no statistically significant difference (t48  =  1.86, P  =  0.07). We detected no interaction between ontogenetic stage and reaction to tuatara fecal scent (F(7, 11)  =  1.44, P  =  0.28). However, adults showed reduced activity (t17  =  −2.22, P  =  0.04) and exploratory behavior (nudging walls; t17  =  −3.39, P < 0.01) relative to juveniles, suggesting overall ontogenetic differences in behavior.

Discussion

Behavioral responses of N. manukanus to the scents of fruit, conspecifics and native predators demonstrate chemosensory recognition of a variety of chemical cues, and the ability to respond accordingly. Diurnal geckos from New Zealand are under strong selection from native predators to adopt visual antipredator strategies and, as a consequence, limited ability to use chemical cues was expected. However, N. manukanus do not appear to have greatly reduced chemosensory abilities or exhibit the visual-chemosensory trade off seen in agamid and chameleonid lizards.

Increased activity in response to fruit scent implies recognition and use of chemical cues to locate fruit. Because observed behavioral changes were not accompanied by a change in frequency of lingual sampling, it is possible that the behaviours were elicited primarily by olfactory cues rather than vomerolfactory cues (Cooper and Pèrez-Mellado, 2001; Whitaker, 1987; Wotton, 2002). Although New Zealand lizards are primarily insectivorous, frequent natural observations of lizards consuming fruit (Whitaker, 1987; Wotton, 2002) together with high capture rates of lizards using traps baited with fruit (Whitaker, 1967) both imply that these lizards use chemical cues to locate fruit. Diurnal New Zealand congeners (N. grayi) are known to feed on fruit and nectar, whereas reports of frugivory and nectivory in nocturnal geckos are numerous (Whitaker, 1987; Wotton, 2002). Our observations on the use of chemoreception to recognize fruit in N. manukanus support the recent hypothesis that location of fruit using chemical cues is widespread among omnivorous and herbivorous reptiles (Cooper et al., 2001).

Sex-specific vomerolfactory and behavioral responses to conspecific scent in N. manukanus suggests that chemoreception mediates intraspecific interactions, which is consistent with findings in other species. For example, the lingual response by male Podarcis hispanica was greater to nongravid than gravid females, and conspecific than heterospecific females (Cooper and Pèrez-Mellado, 2002). Increased response of males to female scent may indicate that males use chemical cues to take an active role in searching for mates. However, investigating whether these geckos produce chemical secretions in different seasons and use scent marks on the substrate is necessary for a complete interpretation of the use of chemosensory cues in mediating conspecific interactions.

Naultinus manukanus displayed antipredator responses to fecal cues (but not skin secretions) produced by their native predators (tuatara). Tuatara are generalist, medium-sized predators that include geckos in their diet (Walls, 1981) and share both vine and ground habitats with N. manukanus on Takapourewa. The decreased activity and tongue flick rate by both adult and juvenile geckos in response to tuatara fecal cues may reflect an antipredator freeze response appropriate to this primarily visual, sit-and-wait predator (Meyer-Rochow and Teh, 1991). Such chemosensory-mediated antipredator freeze responses are common among reptilian prey (Greene, 1988) and can function to increase survival of lizards (Downes, 2002).

Evolution under a reduced suite of selection pressures in isolation from major predators seems not to have decreased chemosensory capabilities of New Zealand's apparently visually-oriented, diurnal geckos. Instead, use of chemosensory cues by N. manukanus appears to play a functional role in their biotic interactions and are consistent with other squamates. Our findings support the hypothesis that chemosensory traits remain highly conserved among squamates, reflecting suprafamilial attributes rather than local adaptation.

Acknowledgments

We thank C. Daugherty for comments on the manuscript and K. Hare for logistical support. Financial support was provided by a Foundation for Research, Science and Technology Top Achiever Doctoral Scholarship (to JMH). Our research was conducted with Victoria University of Wellington Animal Ethics Committee (permits 2003R16 and 2003R20), New Zealand Department of Conservation (permits 9/375 ROA and LIZ0301) and Ngāti Koata Trust approval.

References

1.

D. Brown 2000. Stephens Island: Ark of the Light. Blenheim, New Zealand Blenheim Print. Google Scholar

2.

C. M. Bull, C. L. Griffin, E. J. Lanham, and G. R. Johnston . 2000. Recognition of pheromones from group members in a gregarious lizard, Egernia stokesii. Journal of Herpetology 34:92–99. Google Scholar

3.

G. M. Burghardt 1970. Chemical perception in reptiles. In J. W. Johnston, D. G. Moulton, and A. Turk , editors. Advances in Chemoreception. 241–308.New York, New York, U.S.A Appleton-Century-Crofts. Google Scholar

4.

W. E. Cooper 1995. Prey chemical discrimination and foraging mode in gekkonid lizards. Herpetological Monographs 9:120–129. Google Scholar

5.

W. E. Cooper 1996. Preliminary reconstructions of nasal chemosensory evolution in Squamata. Amphibia-Reptilia 17:395–415. Google Scholar

6.

W. E. Cooper 1998. Evaluation of swab and related tests as a bioassay for assessing responses by squamate reptiles to chemical stimuli. Journal of Chemical Ecology 24:841–866. Google Scholar

7.

W. E. Cooper and L. J. Steele . 1997. Pheromonal discrimination of sex by male and female leopard geckos (Eublepharis macularius). Journal of Chemical Ecology 23:2967–2977. Google Scholar

8.

W. E. Cooper and J. J. Habegger . 2000. Lingual and biting responses to food chemicals by some eublepharid and gekkonid geckos. Journal of Herpetology 34:360–368. Google Scholar

9.

W. E. Cooper and V. Pèrez-Mellado . 2001. Location of fruit using only airborne odor cues by a lizard. Physiology and Behavior 74:339–342. Google Scholar

10.

W. E. Cooper and V. Pèrez-Mellado . 2002. Pheromonal discriminations of sex, reproductive condition, and species by the lacertid lizard Podarcis hispanica. Journal of Experimental Zoology 292:523–527. Google Scholar

11.

W. E. Cooper, G. W. Ferguson, and J. J. Habegger . 2001. Responses to animal and plant chemicals by several iguanian insectivores and the tuatara, Sphenodon punctatus. Journal of Herpetology 35:255–263. Google Scholar

12.

B. E. Dial, P. J. Weldon, and B. Curtis . 1989. Chemosensory identification of snake predators (Phyllorhynchus decurtatus) by banded geckos (Coleonyx variegatus). Journal of Herpetology 23:224–229. Google Scholar

13.

S. J. Downes 2002. Does responsiveness to predator scents affect lizard survivorship? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 52:38–42. Google Scholar

14.

S. J. Downes and D. Bauwens . 2002. Does reproductive state affect a lizard's behavior toward predator chemical cues? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 52:444–450. Google Scholar

15.

S. J. Downes and R. Shine . 1998. Sedentary snakes and gullible geckos: predator-prey coevolution in nocturnal rock-dwelling reptiles. Animal Behaviour 55:1373–1385. Google Scholar

16.

S. J. Downes and R. Shine . 1999. Do incubation-induced changes in a lizard's phenotype influence its vulnerability to predators? Oecologia 120:9–18. Google Scholar

17.

M. Gabe and H. Saint Girons . 1976. Contribution à la morphologie comparée des fosses nasales et de leurs annexes chez les Lépidosauriens. Mémoires du Muséum d'Histoire Naturelle Nouvelle Série A 98:1–106. Google Scholar

18.

B. J. Gill and A. H. Whitaker . 1996. New Zealand Frogs and Reptiles. Auckland, New Zealand David Bateman Limited. Google Scholar

19.

J. F. Hair, W. C. Black, B. J. Babin, R. E. Anderson, and R. L. Tatham . 2006. Multivariate data analysis. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey, U.S.A Pearson Prentice Hall. Google Scholar

20.

M. Halpern 1992. Nasal chemical senses in reptiles: structure and function. In C. Gans and D. Crews , editors. Biology of the Reptilia. 423–523.Chicago, Illinois, U.S.A University of Chicago Press. Google Scholar

21.

K. M. Hare, J. M. Hoare, and R. Hitchmough . In press. Investigating natural population dynamics of Naultinus manukanus to inform conservation management of New Zealand's cryptic diurnal geckos. Journal of Herpetology 41:80–92. Google Scholar

22.

K. M. Hare, S. Pledger, M. B. Thompson, J. H. Miller, and C. H. Daugherty . 2004. Conditioning reduces metabolic rate and time to steady-state in the lizard Naultinus manukanus (Reptilia: Gekkonidae). Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A 139:245–250. Google Scholar

23.

M. L. Head, J. S. Keogh, and P. Doughty . 2005. Male southern water skinks (Eulamprus heatwolei) use both visual and chemical cues to detect female sexual receptivity. Acta Ethologica 8:79–85. Google Scholar

24.

R. A. Hitchmough 1978. Report to Wildlife Service on observations of Heteropholis manukanus on Stephens Island. Auckland, New Zealand New Zealand Wildlife Service. Google Scholar

25.

R. A. Hitchmough 1982. The ecology of a population of Naultinus grayi Bell near Kaitaia. In D. G. Newman , editor. New Zealand Herpetology. 399–407. Wellington, New Zealand New Zealand Wildlife Service Occasional Publication No. 2. Proceedings of a symposium held at the Victoria University of Wellington 29–31 January 1980.  Google Scholar

26.

R. N. Holdaway 1999. A spatio-temporal model for the invasion of the New Zealand archipelago by the Pacific rat Rattus exulans. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand 29:91–105. Google Scholar

27.

M. H. Lawrence 1997. The importance of lizards to seed dispersal of native montane fleshy fruits, Canterbury, New Zealand. Christchurch, New Zealand M.Sc. Plant and Microbial Sciences, University of Canterbury. Google Scholar

28.

J. B. Losos, T. W. Schoener, K. I. Warheit, and D. Creer . 2001. Experimental studies of adaptive differentiation in Bahamian Anolis lizards. Genetica 112–113:399–415. Google Scholar

29.

V. B. Meyer-Rochow 1988. Behaviour of young tuatara (Sphenodon punctatus) in total darkness. Tuatara 30:36–38. Google Scholar

30.

V. B. Meyer-Rochow and K. L. Teh . 1991. Visual predation by tuatara (Sphenodon punctatus) on the beach beetle (Chaerodes trachyscelides) as a selective force in the production of distinct colour morphs. Tuatara 31:1–8. Google Scholar

31.

T. S. Parsons 1959. Nasal anatomy and the phylogeny of reptiles. Evolution 13:175–187. Google Scholar

32.

T. S. Parsons 1970. The nose and Jacobson's organ. In C. Gans and T. S. Parsons , editors. Biology of the Reptilia. 99–191.New York, New York, U.S.A Academic Press. Google Scholar

33.

K. Schwenk 1993. The evolution of chemoreception in squamate reptiles: a phylogenetic approach. Brain Behavior and Evolution 41:124–137. Google Scholar

34.

K. Schwenk 1995. Of tongues and noses: chemoreception in lizards and snakes. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 10:7–12. Google Scholar

35.

A. Sih, A. Bell, and J. C. Johnson . 2004. Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19:372–378. Google Scholar

36.

J. Stapley and J. S. Keogh . 2004. Exploratory and antipredator behaviours differ between territorial and nonterritorial male lizards. Animal Behaviour 68:841–846. Google Scholar

37.

G. Y. Walls 1981. Feeding ecology of the tuatara (Sphenodon punctatus) on Stephens Island, Cook Strait. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 4:89–97. Google Scholar

38.

Y. L. Werner and A. H. Whitaker . 1978. Observations and comments on the body temperatures of some New Zealand reptiles. New Zealand Journal of Zoology 5:375–393. Google Scholar

39.

A. H. Whitaker 1967. Baiting pitfall traps for small lizards. Herpetologica 23:309–310. Google Scholar

40.

A. H. Whitaker 1987. The roles of lizards in New Zealand plant reproductive strategies. New Zealand Journal of Botany 25:315–328. Google Scholar

41.

A. H. Whitaker and P. Gaze . 1999. Conservation of lizards in the Nelson/Marlborough Conservancy. Nelson, New Zealand Occasional Publication 44, New Zealand Department of Conservation. Google Scholar

42.

T. H. Worthy, A. J. D. Tennyson, M. Archer, A. M. Musser, S. J. Hand, C. Jones, B. J. Douglas, J. A. McNamara, and R. M. D. Beck . 2006. Miocene mammal reveals a Mesozoic ghost lineage on insular New Zealand, southwest Pacific. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103:19419–19423. Google Scholar

43.

D. M. Wotton 2002. Effectiveness of the common gecko (Hoplodactylus maculatus) as a seed disperser on Mana Island, New Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Botany 40:639–647. Google Scholar
Joanne M. Hoare, Shirley Pledger, and Nicola J. Nelson "CHEMICAL DISCRIMINATION OF FOOD, CONSPECIFICS AND PREDATORS BY APPARENTLY VISUALLY-ORIENTED DIURNAL GECKOS, NAULTINUS MANUKANUS," Herpetologica 63(2), 184-192, (16 June 2007). https://doi.org/10.1655/0018-0831(2007)63[184:CDOFCA]2.0.CO;2
Received: 22 January 2007; Published: 16 June 2007
KEYWORDS
chemoreception
Evolutionary conservatism
local adaptation
Sensory mode
squamate
Back to Top