Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2008 The Feeding Behavior of the Black Kite (Milvus migrans) in the Rubbish Dump of Rome
Umberto De Giacomo, Gaspare Guerrieri
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

We studied the feeding behavior of Black Kites (Milvus migrans) in a rubbish dump in Rome, Italy, from April–September 2005. The earliest kites reached the rubbish dump at dawn and the last left just after dusk. The number of individuals foraging in the dump increased during the course of the day and also from April to August. The number of kites in the rubbish area of the dump was usually small and tended to show rapid increases and equally quick declines. Kites searched for food while directly standing on the rubbish or, more often, while flying low over the ground. The first strategy, which was never observed in April but was more common during the following months, was used when there were few foraging gulls or trucks in the dump. Cleptoparasitism was the technique most frequently used to acquire food (76% of all foraging events). It was directed toward conspecifics in 35% of the observed cases, toward Yellow-legged Gulls (Larus cachinnans) in 57% and toward Carrion Crows (Corvus corone) in 8%. The success rate of cleptoparasitic attempts was 32% against conspecifics, 73% against Yellow-legged Gulls and 66% against crows. The higher frequency of cleptoparasitic attempts against gulls may thus be explained by its higher success rate, although its efficiency declined with increasing gull numbers. The frequency of intraspecific cleptoparasitism paralleled variations in kite density and its effectiveness increased progressively from April–August.

The Black Kite (Milvus migrans) is a gregarious bird (Cramp and Simmons 1980, Donázar 1992) which frequently forms roosts and reproductive colonies where trophic resources are very abundant (Cramp and Simmons 1980, Viñuela 2000, Sergio et al. 2003b). However, because many individuals in this species typically gather over a spatially restricted amount of resources, the resulting high density of kites may exacerbate aggressiveness; thus, behavioral strategies to contend with this may be advantageous (Newton 1998, Donázar et al. 1999). Cleptoparasitism is a widespread aggressive strategy among birds, especially among predators/scavengers and has been frequently observed in Black Kites (Cramp and Simmons 1980, Davies and Cowlichaw 1996, Kabouche and Ventroux 1999). In Mediterranean areas, this species is attracted by the conspicuous availability of food at urban waste disposal sites as in Madrid or Marseilles, where it has established large breeding colonies (Blanco 1994, Blanco 1997, Kabouche and Ventroux 1999). The Black Kite population of Rome consists of 40–50 pairs, most of them feeding on the rubbish dump along with Yellow-legged Gulls (Larus cachinnans; approximately 10 000 individuals) and Carrion Crows (Corvus corone; 100–400 individuals; De Giacomo et al. 2004). We here examine the behavior of Black Kites foraging in the rubbish dump of Rome.

Study Area and Methods

Study Area

Kites were studied in a 190-ha rubbish dump, 12.5 km southwest of the urban center of Rome, Italy (41°51′N, 12°20′E). The area included two large pre-selection plants and a waste treatment area. The site supported a variety of patches of shrubs and trees, where several plant species (Eucalyptus sp., Robinia pseudoacacia, Phoenix canariensis, Olea europaea), mostly exotics, were planted in order to stabilize the slopes. Native plant communities (Salix alba, Prunus spinosa, Phragmites australis, Arundo pliniana, Inula viscosa) are developing on abandoned sites. Four thousand ± 158 tons of waste are deposited daily in the rubbish dump. The waste is transported in the treatment area with dump trucks and then levelled and compressed with compaction equipment. These activities begin at dawn and, after a break between 1200–1300 H, cease at approximately 1700 H. The compaction equipment works on a surface of 1000–3000 m2 per day. In the afternoon, a portion of the compressed waste is covered with sand.

Breeding Colonies

The range of the Black Kite in the lower reaches of the Tiber River near Rome comprises a mosaic of residual woodland patches and agricultural fields (mostly wheat [Triticum aestivum], maize [Zea mais] and forage) scattered within an extensive matrix of urban and industrial landscapes, and includes two airports and numerous busy roads. Adult kites return to the area between 10 March and the end of April (De Giacomo et al. 2004). Nonbreeding kites arrive at the site between May and early June, and form night roosts along the river (De Giacomo et al. 2004, Blanco et al. 2007, Sergio et al. 2007). Breeding pairs are clustered in three main colonies: one in the south of Rome in the Presidential Estate of Castelporziano (28–36 pairs) and the others within woodland patches in the western suburbs (Castel di Guido, 7–9 pairs; Tenuta dei Massimi, 4 pairs). These breeding colonies are 11.5, 5.7, and 4 km, respectively, from the rubbish dump. By the end of August, the kites have mostly migrated from the region and very few individuals are detected in September.

Data Collection

From April–August 2005, the activity of Black Kites was observed for half a day, once a week. Every morning observation period (0400–1300 H) was followed by an afternoon observation (1300–2000 H) the following day and vice versa. Observations were made from elevated sites, 100 or 150 m from the waste treatment area using 8 × 30 and 10 × 42 binoculars. Individuals resting on perches were counted using a 20–60× spotting scope. We made observations only when visibility was good and wind speed did not exceed 4 m/s.

A timed-count method was employed to estimate kite abundance in various regions of the dump. Every 5 min a count was made of: (1) the number of individuals observed inside an area of one hectare (100 × 100 m) where the rubbish is being dumped (“rubbish area”); (2) the number of individuals flying outside the rubbish area, and (3) the number of individuals at rest, i.e., those perched on the pylons or trees located about 400 m from the rubbish area.

We distinguished food collecting strategies as follows: (1) food collected directly from the rubbish by an individual standing on the ground; (2) food collected while flying over the rubbish; (3) food obtained via intraspecific cleptoparasitism; (4) food obtained via interspecific cleptoparasitism. Because the kites could not be individually identified, we recorded all the observed feeding events. In case of cleptoparasitic events, kite activity was monitored until success or failure of the attempt. We also recorded the frequency and success of cleptoparasitic attempts by kites. Cleptoparasitic attempts that lasted for more than one minute were analysed separately.

We estimated the number of Yellow-legged Gulls and Carrion Crows feeding on 100 m2 (10 × 10 m) of rubbish in front of compaction equipment from photographs (Bibby et al. 2000). Photographs were taken every 30 min with a 300 mm lens, from a position of known distance to the subject area. Every 30 min we counted the number of rubbish trucks and compaction equipment moving in the area in order to evaluate their potential effect on the feeding behavior of kites. We related the behavior within the rubbish dump to the potential breeding stage of the kites, based on local phenological data (De Giacomo et al. 1999, Battisti et al. 2003).

Statistical Analysis

To test for differences between mean values, we used one-way ANOVAs and subsequent Tukey tests on log-transformed or arcsin-square-root-transformed values of the data. A χ2 test was performed to test for differences among the frequencies of feeding strategies and success rates of cleptoparasitism, and Yates correction applied when necessary. Spearman's correlation and, when possible, Pearson's correlation coefficients were used to examine the relationship between mean kite abundance and time of the day. All analyses were performed with SPSS 12.0 software.

Results

Activity Patterns

We observed for a total of 160 hr. The mean number of kites per 5-min count for the whole study period, including individuals at rest, those present in the rubbish area, and those flying outside the rubbish area, was 24.1 ± 21.3 (SD; total number of 5-min counts  =  1920), and varied significantly among months (ANOVA: F4,1915  =  162.0, P < 0.001). Abundance was lowest in April (6.68 ± 3.54, number of 5-min counts in April  =  384; post hoc Tukey test, P < 0.05) and increased during the summer (Fig. 1). The highest mean abundance per 5-min count was observed in August (42.90 ± 29.20, N  =  384), when we observed the highest mean abundance for any single hour (1600–1700 H: 90.1 ± 20.7, N  =  24) or for any single count (1700–1800 H; 108 individuals). A second abundance peak was also observed in June (mean abundance per 5-min count  =  33.40 ± 14.30, N  =  384, with a maximum value recorded in a count of 71 individuals between 1000–1100 H). Daily, the first kite arrived at the rubbish dump 1–51 min before dawn (mean: 28.2 ± 17.9 min, N  =  6) in April, May and June. In July and August, the earliest kite reached the rubbish dump 1–22 min after dawn (mean: 11.5 ± 9.1 min, N  =  4). The last kite of the day to depart from the rubbish dump left 5–55 min after dusk (mean: 34.0 ± 17.5 min, N  =  6) in April, May, and June, and 5–15 min before dusk (mean: 10.5 ± 4.2 min, N  =  4) in July and August. Kite abundance increased progressively until 1100 H (rs  =  1.000, N  =  7 hr, P < 0.01), then remained stable until 1700 H, and after 1800 H, the birds progressively left the area.

Figure 1

Mean number of individuals/5 min (±1 SE) observed in a rubbish dump of Rome in April–August 2005. The location of individuals was classified as: (1) near the main refuse-area, (2) within the rubbish dump but outside the immediate surroundings of the refuse-area, or (3) perched. Horizontal arrows indicate breeding stages. (Number of monthly observations  =  384).

i0892-1016-42-2-110-f01.gif

The percentage of individuals at rest on perches differed over the months (8 ± 6% in April, 22 ± 23% in May, 43 ± 23% in June, 26 ± 17% in July, and 56 ± 14% in August; ANOVA: F4,1915  =  133.0, P < 0.001; Fig. 2). In particular, the percentage of individuals present in the rubbish area was not correlated with the percentage of individuals in flight outside the area (r  =  0.021, P  =  0.85, N  =  80; 16 hourly counts per day over 5 mo), but increased as the percentage of individuals at rest diminished (r  =  −0.573, N  =  80, P  =  0.01). The activity in the rubbish area rose progressively during the day and was greatest around noon. The greatest number of individuals was observed during the time periods 1000–1100 H and 1100–1200 H in July and August, respectively. Crowding peaks normally lasted for 5–10 min and were followed by rapid, marked declines (Fig. 3). The mean percentage of individuals per 5-min count flying outside the rubbish area was 37 ± 27% (N  =  1920); this varied over the months (ANOVA: F4,1915  =  81.3, P < 0.001; Fig. 2), and was inversely correlated with the percentage of individuals on perches (r  =  −0.474, N  =  80, P < 0.01). The first juveniles were observed in the dump during the last third of June and some of them were still being fed by parents.

Figure 2

Mean percentage of individuals/hr (±1 SE) observed in the rubbish area, outside the rubbish area, and on daytime perches (Rome rubbish dump, April–August 2005).

i0892-1016-42-2-110-f02.gif

Figure 3

Number of individuals observed in the rubbish area during the whole study period during counts taken every 5 min from 0400–2000 H (total number of counts  =  1920, Rome rubbish dump, April–August 2005).

i0892-1016-42-2-110-f03.gif

Feeding Behaviors

During the study period we did not observe any predation on live animals. On the contrary, we observed 3214 episodes of food collection from the rubbish. Of these, 783 were autonomous food collection episodes and the other 2431 were cleptoparasitism acts. Of the overall 783 autonomous collection episodes, food items were collected by birds inspecting the waste on the ground on 298 occasions (38.1% of the episodes) and by individuals flying low over the rubbish mass in 485 instances (61.9%; Fig. 4). Food collection by birds standing on the ground was never observed before 1000 H and this behavior increased during the day (ground collection observations per hr vs. time of day: rs  =  0.778, N  =  16, P < 0.001). This behavior was more often observed when gull densities were lower (ground collections per hr vs. mean number of gulls per hr: rs  =  −0.539, N  =  16, P < 0.031) and when the compaction equipment was inactive for the lunch period (1100–1200H), or ceased at the end of the daily work shift (ground collections per hr vs. mean number of compactors per hr: rs  =  −0.805; N  =  16, P < 0.001). No relationship was observed between the time of the day and the number of episodes of food collection by flying kites (observations of collections per hr vs. time of day: rs  =  0.238, N  =  16, P  =  0.37). The ground collection behavior, not observed in April, differed in frequency among the later study months (χ23  =  93.4, P < 0.001; Table 1).

Figure 4

Number of observations/hr of birds collecting food while standing on the ground (N  =  298), or collecting food while flying low over the ground (N  =  485), Rome rubbish dump, April–August 2005.

i0892-1016-42-2-110-f04.gif

Table 1

Number of cleptoparasitic attempts and frequency (%) of two food collection techniques used by Black Kites in a rubbish dump in Rome (April–August 2005).

i0892-1016-42-2-110-t01.gif

The mean numbers of gulls and crows in the photographs of the rubbish in front of compaction equipment were, respectively, 425 ± 212 (range: 44–845) and 9 ± 4 (range: 2–15). However, there was no relationship between the number of gulls and the episodes of food collection by flying kites (observations of collections per hr vs. mean number of gulls per hr: rs  =  0.071, N  =  16, P  =  0.80). Collection of food from the ground by kites was not correlated with the mean number of gulls or compactors (P ≥ 0.47).

Of the 2431 cleptoparasitism events, 854 (35.1%) were intraspecific, 1379 (56.7%) were against Yellow-legged Gulls and 198 (8.1%) against Carrion Crows. The rate of success was 32.4% for intraspecific cleptoparasitism, but was significantly higher for cleptoparasitism attempts against Yellow-legged Gulls (73.2%; χ21  =  358, P < 0.001) and against Carrion Crows (65.7%; χ21  =  73.8, P < 0.001). The effectiveness of intraspecific cleptoparasitism increased during the season, ranging from 21.4% success in April (N  =  70 attempts) to 37.8% in August (N  =  196; χ24  =  13.10, P < 0.02), and the frequency of intraspecific cleptoparasitism increased with the overall abundance of kites feeding in the rubbish area (rs  =  0.834, N  =  16, P < 0.01; Fig. 5). Cleptoparasitic attempts lasting >1 min (maximum  =  4.2 min) were less successful than shorter ones (χ21  =  8.61, P < 0.01).

Figure 5

Mean hourly increase in intraspecific cleptoparasitic attempts in relation to the mean number of kite individuals in the rubbish area of the Rome rubbish dump, April–August 2005 (mean number of observations per hr  =  160; line: y  =  1.7303 x + 3.0317, r2  =  0.6281).

i0892-1016-42-2-110-f05.gif

The success rate of cleptoparasitism against Yellow-legged Gulls and Carrion Crows did not differ among the months (P > 0.05). When directed against the Yellow-legged Gull, the effectiveness of cleptoparasitism increased with the time of day (rs  =  0.944, N  =  16, P <0.001; Fig. 6) and declined with the number of gulls present on the rubbish (rs  =  −0.615, N  =  16, P < 0.05). Against Carrion Crows, the efficiency of cleptoparasitism increased progressively throughout the day (rs  =  0.811, N  =  16, P < 0.001), but was not related to the local abundance of crows (rs  =  0.362, N  =  16, P < 0.17; Fig. 6). Cleptoparasitism against Yellow-legged Gulls was significantly more successful than that against Carrion Crows (χ21  =  4.50, P < 0.03). Cleptoparasitism attempts by Yellow-legged Gulls against Black Kites were never successful (N  =  47), whereas Carrion Crows succeeded on 2 of 196 attempts (1.0%).

Figure 6

Success rate of intraspecific (N  =  854) and of interspecific cleptoparasitic attempts against Yellow-legged Gulls (N  =  1379) and Carrion Crows (N  =  198). Success rate is expressed as the percentage of the attempts which were successful (Rome rubbish dump, April–August 2005).

i0892-1016-42-2-110-f06.gif

Discussion

The Black Kite population of Rome regularly visits the refuse dump that we studied (De Giacomo et al. 2004). Some individuals in this population are known to feed also elsewhere, and the exploitation of such alternative food sources seems to have a seasonal character (Castaldi and Guerrieri 2006). In contrast to observations from Madrid (Blanco 1997), where kites also hunted for live food items, kites in the Rome dump collected food primarily from the refuse, which may not meet all the nutrients needs of the young (Veiga and Hiraldo 1990, Viñuela 1991).

In April, the number of kites in the rubbish area was high. In May and June, the percentage of kites present in the refuse processing area tended to decline. In May, individuals that probably were nonbreeders or had arrived at the area later in the season (Blanco et al. 2007, Sergio et al. 2007) remained on perches longer. In June, the first juveniles were observed at the dump-site (Glutz von Blotzheim et al. 1971, Newton 1979, Blanco 1994, Ortlieb 1998); in this month, most kites arrived at the dump at dawn and likely remained on perches until they were successful at feeding. In August, kites spent most of the day resting (i.e., perching) within the rubbish dump. At dusk, they moved some kilometers from the rubbish dump to form night roosts, whereas in Marseilles they stayed near the rubbish dump overnight (Kabouche and Ventroux 1999). This suggests that locations of roosting sites were likely determined by woodland availability and structure as reported elsewhere (Sergio and Boto 1999, Sergio et al. 2003c, Sergio et al. 2003a, Battisti and Zocchi 2004).

At the Rome dump, the concentrations of individuals were lower than those recorded in Madrid and Marseilles dumps (Blanco 1994, Kabouche and Ventroux 1999). Given the fact that the number of individuals present in the rubbish area usually remained low and that high concentrations occurred only for brief periods, it is possible that coexistence of a relatively large number of individuals was promoted by temporal segregation and consequently lower aggression rates, as previously reported for other carrion- and refuse-feeders (Wallace and Temple 1987, Gómez-Tejedor 1998, Newton 1998). The same mechanism, working on a seasonal basis, has been proposed for Red Kites (Milvus milvus) and Black Kites in Spanish rubbish dumps (Donázar 1992). In this scenario, the observed increase in intraspecific cleptoparasitism rates associated with increasing kite density might cause some kites to refrain from feeding on the refuse until the abundance of potential cleptoparasites declines. The progressive increase in the efficiency of intraspecific cleptoparasitism observed from April to August might be influenced by (1) the progressively higher conspecific density within the rubbish dump, (2) a progressively higher proportion of young birds employing or suffering cleptoparasitic attempts, and/or (3) the increasing motivation by breeders later in the season when the energy demands of feeding nestlings are higher.

Kites seemed to prefer finding food while flying low over the ground to foraging from the ground. Food collection from the ground was observed only in the late morning, after the searching activity of gulls made food more readily available. Kites locate food by sight and apparently are not able to break through plastic seals. Searching for food on the ground was the technique used more often when the number of gulls was low and the opportunities for cleptoparasitism more limited. The same behavior was observed when compaction equipment activity was low or absent. This strategy was more frequent in July and August, and may have been associated with the appearance of recently fledged young at the rubbish dump.

Finally, Yellow-legged Gulls were the most frequent victims of cleptoparasitic attempts by kites. This was expected and probably adaptive because gulls were more abundant than crows and cleptoparasitised more efficiently. The increase in success rate during the day was probably associated with a progressive decline in gull abundance. In fact, large gull flocks seemed to disorient the kites when gull numbers were extremely high.

Acknowledgments

We thank G. Blanco, F. Sergio, J. Viñuela, and an anonymous referee for their criticism and constructive suggestions. We are also very grateful to Manlio Cerroni, Piero Giovi, and Francesco Rando, who gave us permission to work at the rubbish dump, and to all the dump personnel for their collaboration during fieldwork.

Literature Cited

1.

C. Battisti, C. Mari, M. Tomassetti, and A. Zocchi . 2003. La colonia suburbana di Nibbio bruno, Milvus migrans, a Roma: attività riproduttiva e roosting premigratorio. Riv. Ital. Ornitol 73:97–103. Google Scholar

2.

C. Battisti and A. Zocchi . 2004. Nesting habitat structure of the Black Kites, Milvus migrans, in a suburban area (Rome, Central Italy). Riv. Ital. Ornitol 74:97–106. Google Scholar

3.

C. J. Bibby, N. D. Burgess, D. A. Hill, and S. H. Mustoe . 2000. Bird census techniques. Academic Press. London, U.K. Google Scholar

4.

G. Blanco 1994. Seasonal abundance of Black Kite associated with the rubbish dump of Madrid. J. Raptor Res 28:242–245. Google Scholar

5.

G. Blanco 1997. Role of refuse as food for migrant, floater and breeding Black Kite (Milvus migrans). J. Raptor Res 31:71–76. Google Scholar

6.

G. Blanco, J. A. Lemus, O. FrÍas, J. Grande, B. Arroyo, F. MartÍnez, and N. Baniandrés . 2007. Contamination traps as trans-frontier contamination challenges: new research on the impact of refuse dumps on the conservation of migratory avian scavengers. 153–204. in A. M. Cato , editor. Environmental research trends. Nova Publishers. New York, NY U.S.A. Google Scholar

7.

A. Castaldi and G. Guerrieri . 2006. Ritmi d'attività e uso dell'habitat trofico nella popolazione romana di Nibbio bruno in Italia centrale. 42–43. in S. Allavena, A. Andreotti, J. Angelini, and M. Scotti , editors. Atti del Convegno “Status e conservazione del Nibbio reale (Milvus milvus) e del Nibbio bruno (Milvus migrans) in Italia e in Europa meridionale. Serra S. Quirico. AN, Italy. Google Scholar

8.

S. C. Cramp and K. E. L. Simmons . 1980. The birds of the western palearctic, Vol. 2. Oxford Univ. Press. Oxford, U.K. Google Scholar

9.

J. G. Davies and G. Cowlichaw . 1996. Baboon carnivory and raptor interspecific competition in the Namib Desert. J. Arid Environ 34:247–249. Google Scholar

10.

U. De Giacomo, C. Battisti, J. G. Cecere, S. Ricci, F. Borlenghi, and A. Tinelli . 2004. La popolazione romana di Nibbio bruno Milvus migrans: aspetti ecologici. 95–124. in L. Corsetti , editor. Atti del Convegno. Uccelli rapaci nel Lazio: status e distribuzione, strategie di conservazione. Ed. Belvedere. Latina, Italy. Google Scholar

11.

U. De Giacomo, M. Stazi, G. Pavan, A. Tinelli, and A. Fanfani . 1999. Il Nibbio bruno Milvus migrans nella Tenuta di Castelporziano. Alula 4:137–149. Google Scholar

12.

J. A. Donázar 1992. Muladares y basureros en la biología y conservación des las aves en España. Ardeola 39:29–40. Google Scholar

13.

J. A. Donázar, A. Travaini, O. Ceballos, A. Rodriguez, M. Delibes, and F. Hiraldo . 1999. Effects of sex-associated competitive asymmetries on foraging group structure and despotic distribution in Andean condors. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 45:55–65. Google Scholar

14.

U. N. Glutz von Blotzheim, K. M. Bauer, and E. Bezzel . 1971. Handbuch der Vogel Mitteleuropas. Band 4 Falconiformes. Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft. Frankfurt am Main, Germany. Google Scholar

15.

U. Gómez-Tejedor 1998. Comportamiento Cleptopárasitico del Milano Real Milvus milvus en un vertedero. 173–176. in R. D. Chancellor, B-U. Meyburg, and J. J. Ferrero , editors. Holarctic birds of prey: proceedings of an international conference. Actas del Congreso Internacional sobre Rapaces del Holártico. ADENEX, Mèrida, España, and WWGBP. Berlin, Germany. Google Scholar

16.

B. Kabouche and J. Ventroux . 1999. Évolution journalière de l'abondance des Milans noirs Milvus migrans sur la décharge d'ordures de Marseille. Alauda 67:63–67. Google Scholar

17.

I. Newton 1979. Population ecology of raptors. Berkhamsted, U.K T. and A.D. Poyser. Google Scholar

18.

I. Newton 1998. Population limitation in birds. San Diego, U.S.A Academic Press. Google Scholar

19.

R. Ortlieb 1998. Der Schwarzmilan: Milvus migrans. Hohenwarsleben: Westarp-Wissenschaften. Hohenwarsleben, Germany. Google Scholar

20.

F. Sergio, J. Blas, M. G. Forero, J. A. Donázar, and F. Hiraldo . 2007. Sequential settlement and site-dependence in a migratory raptor. Behav. Ecol 18:811–821. Google Scholar

21.

F. Sergio and A. Boto . 1999. Nest dispersion, diet, and breeding success of Black Kites (Milvus migrans) in the Italian pre-Alps. J. Raptor Res 33:207–217. Google Scholar

22.

F. Sergio, L. Marchesi, and P. Pedrini . 2003a. Spatial refugia and the coexistence of a diurnal raptor with its intraguild owl predator. J. Anim. Ecol 72:232–245. Google Scholar

23.

F. Sergio, P. Pedrini, and L. Marchesi . 2003b. Reconciling the dichotomy between single species and ecosystem conservation: Black Kites (Milvus migrans) and eutrophication in pre-alpine lakes. Biol. Conserv 110:101–111. Google Scholar

24.

F. Sergio, P. Pedrini, and L. Marchesi . 2003c. Adaptive selection of foraging and nesting habitat by Black Kites (Milvus migrans) and its implications for conservation: a multi-scale approach. Biol. Conserv 112:351–362. Google Scholar

25.

J. P. Veiga and F. Hiraldo . 1990. Food habits and survival and growth of nestlings in two sympatric kites (Milvus milvus and Milvus migrans). Holarct. Ecol 13:62–71. Google Scholar

26.

J. Viñuela 1991. Ecología reproductiva del Milano Negro en el Parque Nacional de Doñana. Madrid, Spain Universidad Complutense de Madrid. Ph.D. dissertation,. Google Scholar

27.

J. Viñuela 2000. Opposing selective pressures on hatching asynchrony: egg viability, brood reduction, and nestling growth. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 48:333–343. Google Scholar

28.

M. P. Wallace and S. A. Temple . 1987. Competitive interactions within and between species in a guild of avian scavengers. Auk 104:290–295. Google Scholar
Umberto De Giacomo and Gaspare Guerrieri "The Feeding Behavior of the Black Kite (Milvus migrans) in the Rubbish Dump of Rome," Journal of Raptor Research 42(2), 110-118, (1 June 2008). https://doi.org/10.3356/JRR-07-09.1
Received: 13 February 2007; Accepted: 1 January 2008; Published: 1 June 2008
KEYWORDS
Black Kite
cleptoparasitism
dump
feeding behavior
Milvus migrans
urban
Back to Top