Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 August 2007 Cebus paraguayanus in Zoos: The Spontaneous Expression of Species-Specific Behaviors
Aldo M. Giudice, Romina Pavé
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Captive Cebus paraguayanus were observed to express spontaneously a wide variety of species-specific behaviors in different zoos from Argentina. The monkeys were kept in old-style wire mesh cages with cement floors, as well as island enclosures. We collected qualitative data on nine species-specific behaviors, and all the sex-age classes analyzed expressed some of these behaviors. The results indicated that in spite of living under predictable captive conditions without environmental enrichment, Cebus paraguayanus were able to react to the incidental opportunities that were presented in these environments.

Introduction

Capuchin monkeys, genus Cebus (Platyrrhini: Cebidae), are omnivorous primates with a diet consisting mainly of fruit, and in lesser quantity, insects and other plant parts (Freese and Oppenheimer, 1981). Terborgh (1983) has pointed out that capuchins use a destructive feeding strategy; by means of their strength, they can exploit food resources not available to other animals. Both in natural and captive conditions, capuchin monkeys use a wide variety of manipulative skills. They have been observed to crack open hard-shelled fruit (i.e.: Izawa and Mizuno, 1977; Izawa, 1979), hunt different vertebrates and invertebrates (i.e.: Izawa, 1979; Robinson and Janson, 1987; Rose, 1997; Ferreira et al., 2002), use tools like stones and branches (i.e: Anderson, 1990; Visalberghi, 1990; Ottoni and Mannu, 2001; Fragaszy et al., 2004a; Moura and Lee, 2004), tap scan for presence of hidden animals (Izawa, 1978; Phillips et al., 2003), and fur rub with different plant and animal materials (Baker, 1996; Gilbert et al., 1998).

Cebus paraguayanus (also called Cebus apella paraguayanus Fisher, 1829, or Cebus libidinosus paraguayanus) finds its southern limit of distribution in Argentina (Mudry, 1990; Martinez et al., 2002). Its natural habitat is shrinking and, in addition, commercial traffic is affecting its populations (Giudice and Mudry, 1995), and is increasing their vulnerability to local extirpation (Diaz and Ojeda, 2000). In Argentina, C. paraguayanus is one of the Neotropical primates most frequently found in zoos (Giudice, 2000). Zoos in Argentina can be defined as “old-style zoos,” following Crockett (1998), with conditions that lack furnishings and bedding materials. Here we use “old style” zoos to refer to zoos that have enclosures of either old or modern architecture but that lack the physical, biological and social stimuli necessary to favor the acclimatization of the animals. These zoos often practice highly deficient management procedures. In extremely predictable enclosures, with little environmental complexity, animals find it difficult to move freely, are unable to choose when and what to eat, with whom to associate with, where to sleep and where to shelter. Under these circumstances, behavioral problems arise and it seems unlikely that a normal range of species-specific behaviors will be expressed (IPS, 1993; Mateos Montero, 1994).

While several studies of capuchin monkeys have been conducted in research laboratories (i.e.: Visalberghi and Trinca, 1989; Visalberghi and Fragaszy, 1995; de Waal, 2000; Brosnan and de Waal, 2003), there is limited published information about capuchin behavior in zoos. Observations of species-specific behaviors recorded for Cebus in zoos are mostly anecdotal (C. olivaceus: Urbani, 1999; C. apella: Mendes et al., 2000; Urbani and Urquiza-Haas, 2002). The groups of C. paraguayanus studied here in the “old-style zoos” from Argentina were made up of monkeys that were: a) illegally captured and then confiscated, b) pets highly habituated to humans, c) obtained from institutional exchange or d) born in zoos. Our study aim was to examine to what extent captive C. paraguayanus express species-specific non-social behaviors. This study clarifies how C. paraguayanus behave in the captive conditions of old-style zoos, and it gives insight into the degree of behavioral plasticity found in this Neotropical primate in an environment with no human-directed enrichment protocol.

Materials and Methods

Study sites, subjects and husbandry

Data were collected in four zoos from Argentina: Jardín Zoológico de Buenos Aires (JZBA), Jardín Zoológico de La Plata (JZLP), Estación de Cría de Animales Silvestres de Berazategui, Buenos Aires province (ECAS), and Estación Zoológica Experimental de Santa Fe (EZE). The enclosures, constructed for public viewing, varied in size from 34 m3 to 127 m3. They contained wire-meshed walls and roofs, soil or cement floors, indoor and outdoor rooms, crossbars, ropes, platforms, dry tree trunks, and some refuges. We also present data from a 100 m2 island enclosure at JZBA. The composition of the groups of C. paraguayanus in each zoo is summarized in Table 1.

The monkeys were fed once a day around 09:00 with vegetables, fleshy fruit, bird eggs, and, except at EZE, meat and bread. Both food and water were placed on trays on the floor. None of the groups observed were presented with any routines of environmental enrichment.

Table 1.

Sex-age classes of the studied colonies of Cebus pamguayanus.

t01_65.gif

Behavioral observations

The observations were collected between 1994 and 2002 during the course of other studies by AMG (1994–1998, see Giudice, 2000), and RP (1994/2002, see Pavé, 2003). The total observation time was 1232 hours (Table 2). Research projects had similar study goals, and in all the observation periods, scan and ad libitum sampling were used (Altmann, 1974) in 30 minute blocks. Data were collected between 9:00 and 19:00.

In this study, nine species-specific behaviors (as defined in Table 3) are described and analyzed qualitatively. For hunting activity, we were able to obtain hourly occurrence rates across all the study periods, and we calculated these as the frequency of the hunting events divided by the total observation time per group.

Results

Hunting behavior

We estimated an hourly frequency of hunting behavior at the different zoos: 0.85 events per hour at JZBA (n =164 events), 0.56 events per hour at ECAS (n = 206 events), 0.52 events per hour at JZLP (n = 130 events), and 0.40 events per hour at EZE (n = 109 events). Note that these frequencies do not take group size into account. More than 98% of hunting activity was directed toward invertebrates, mainly flies. Adults and juveniles of each sex hunted flying insects and even a three-month old infant was observed in hunting attempts. In the JZBA cage enclosure, an adult female hunted and consumed a house sparrow (Passer domesticus). At ECAS, an adult female hunted a rufous hornero (Furnarius rufus). Also at ECAS, the final phase of hunting a duck (Fam. Anatidae) was observed. The duck was grabbed through the wire mesh, and while the duck body remained outside the cage, each capuchin approached the prey, took a piece of it and ate its share away from other individuals. At EZE, the capture of a hornero (Furnarius sp.) was also observed; the bird got into one of the enclosures and was immediately chased by the alpha male of the group until it was captured through the wire mesh. The monkey killed the bird by biting its head and then ate parts of the head and carcass, which he also shared with an adult and two juvenile males in the group. At the JZBA cages, an adult male scattered food outside of the enclosure, apparently in order to attract pigeons (Columba livia) for capture when they came close enough to reach through the wire mesh. In addition, a juvenile male unsuccessfully hunted a kitten. The small cat was handled and pressed against the mesh, as the juvenile tried to pulled it into the enclosure. At JZBA island, an adult female handled an approximately 20 cm long aquatic turtle (Trachemys sp.); she repeatedly hit the turtle against the floor, examined it, bit it, and finally lost interest and left it.

Table 2.

Study periods in four zoos of Argentina.

t02_65.gif

Tool use

Capuchins were able to attain food out of their reach by using sticks, and occasionally, leaves and petioles that they stuck through the mesh wire. At EZE, an adult female took a stick from the adjoining cage, broke it up in two pieces and used one of them to acquire a piece of meat from that cage. The same monkey also used dry leaves that she had removed from her cage roof to try to acquire meat from the adjoining cage as well as a peanut from outside the cage (note that at EZE the monkeys did not receive meat or peanuts in their diet). Similarly, at ECAS, an adult male and female used branches to reach a slice of bread and grapes that were outside the cage and out of their reach. At ECAS and EZE, sticks and leaves were used to explore the soil or the grass growing outside the enclosures. Monkeys used sticks to dig in the ground and at the bark of dry trunks, to explore the holes of trunks or the wall, and to explore the water depth in the pond at the JZBA island. In the JZBA cages, an adult female was observed to use a little piece of straw from a broom to remove splinters from her hand.

Capuchins also used objects to kill animals. At EZE, an adult male killed a 25 cm long non-venomous snake by using the brim of a metal container. This monkey struck the brim of the container several times over the snake's head; then he bit the head and continued eating the rest of the snake. Other types of tool use included striking two objects against one another. At EZE, an adult male used sticks and stones to crack open fruit of the Pindó palm (Syagrus sp.). At JZBA, this activity took place at preferential locations in two cages: the objects were placed in a deep-set square metallic grate, which was used as an anvil. At EZE on two occasions monkeys used sticks like a lever to separate the wire mesh wall from the cage floor.

Table 3.

Cebus pamguayanus behaviors recorded in the study.

t03_65.gif

Foraging

This activity includes the handling and consumption of plant resources accessible from the enclosure. At EZE, monkeys consumed bermuda grass (Cynodon dactylon), which grew outside the front of the enclosures and, in some cases, the capuchins consumed leaves and twigs either with or without buds of poplar (Populus deltoides), plane tree (Platanus acerifolia), and tipu tree (Tipuana tipu). A tala tree (Celtis tala) next to one of the enclosures at ECAS had branches overhanging the cage roof that were not accessible to the monkeys. In autumn, monkeys were seen eating fruit from this tree when it fell into the enclosure. In one cage at JZBA in the early spring, some foliage from an ombu tree (Phytolacca dioica) fell on the roof of the enclosure, and monkeys consumed the inflorescences. Once, keepers at EZE put some soil outside the cages and near the wire mesh, and adults and juveniles consumed grass leaves and roots present in the new soil.

Manipulative events

A frequent observation was that the capuchins hit apples, potatoes, walnuts, or almonds several times against some hard surface, like the floor or the mesh, in order to eat the bits stuck to the surface or to the food. At EZE, another common food processing strategy was that adult and juvenile males rubbed dry leaves, grass, small sticks, food, or other items between their hands or against the enclosure floor.

Tap scanning

This activity is used by the capuchins in natural conditions to detect the presence of vertebrates or invertebrates hidden inside tree trunks. At EZE three adults and one juvenile were observed to tap scan on a dry tree trunk several times. They were also observed to tap scan on some sticks. In some cases, after tap scanning, they bit the tree bark. Also, these animals explored the holes of dry trunks with their fingers and in some cases, they employed a technique observed in natural conditions; they ripped up the bark of the tree with their mouths and hands and then licked the surface.

Fur rubbing

At EZE, an adult male took some water with bleach from the recently washed drinking dish and used his hand to rub it over his back. On another occasion, an adult male smelled the enclosure floor, rubbed his cheek on the same place, and then was imitated by one of the juveniles. At JZLP, two adult males were observed to rub onion over their bodies.

Drinking behavior

At ECAS a keeper left a bucket with water outside the enclosure, next to the mesh. The monkeys could not reach the bucket with their mouths, but they introduced their hands into the water, and then drank the water dripping from their arms.

Behavioral thermoregulation

In each of the zoos, energy-conserving or energy-dissipating postures were observed. In winter, animals rested using the curled posture in the sunny places of the enclosure. In summer, monkeys adopted the stretched posture leaning their ventral region on the floor or on thick crossbars, especially in shady places. These behaviors were observed in all the sex-age classes studied.

Vertical flight response

At EZE, in response to calls emitted by a group member or by monkeys from adjoining cages, the capuchin group would suddenly climb to the upper part of the enclosure, or run into the indoor room. They also reacted daily in this way in response to the presence of a zookeeper near the enclosures. This vertical flight response was observed upon detection of southern crested caracaras (Polyborus plancus) flying overhead, after vocalizations emitted by dogs and captive flamingos (Phoenicopterus sp.), and on two occasions in response to spider monkeys (Ateles paniscus) emitting loud vocalizations in adjoining cages. At ECAS vertical flight behavior was observed at least once a day. A call made by a capuchin group member was the most frequent stimulus that triggered this response. The individual that made the call would climb rapidly up the mesh wire to the cage roof, and this behavior was immediately imitated by other group members. In all cases, after a few seconds on the roof or in the indoor room, the monkeys returned to the floor.

Discussion

In the four study populations of C. paraguayanus kept in old-style zoo enclosures, we observed the normal expression of nine species-specific behaviors, and all the sex-age classes analyzed expressed at least some of these behaviors. Monkeys hunted flying insects and different vertebrates, mainly birds. The hunting of invertebrates and vertebrates has been recorded often in capuchin monkeys in natural and semi-free ranging conditions (Izawa, 1979; Freese and Oppenheimer, 1981; Janson, 1985; Brown et al., 1986; Robinson and Janson, 1987; Rose, 1997; Ferreira et al., 2002; Sampaio and Ferrari, 2005). In one case, in our study an adult male scattered food outside the cage, apparently to attract pigeons as potential prey. This may suggest a certain cognitive ability, and is in line with similar observations in other captive capuchins, documented to attract ducklings and fishes (McGrew and Marchant, 1997; Mendes et al., 2000). This observation also highlights the ability of capuchins to take great advantage of the opportunities that are presented in the zoos and to develop alternative foraging strategies to meet them.

Tool use was observed in a variety of contexts. Branches and leaves were spontaneously used like arm extensions to obtain food that was out of reach, or to explore distant or hidden surfaces. As observed in wild Cebus (Moura and Lee, 2004), individuals in this study used objects to dig in the ground. An adult female used a small piece of straw from a broom to remove splinters from her hand. Monkeys also cracked fruits with sticks and stones using the same actions as described by Izawa and Mizuno (1977). An adult male used the brim of a metal container to kill a non-venomous snake. Although Boinski (1988) reported the use of a branch by wild Cebus capucinus to attack a venomous snake, the case observed here with a potential prey item can be considered a prey processing technique equivalent to the use of objects as hammers to crack open oysters (see Fernandez, 1991), and to the cases described above with fruits and reported in wild Cebus (i.e.: Izawa and Mizuno, 1977; Anderson, 1990; Moura and Lee, 2004). Our monkeys often hit different foods against hard surfaces and then ate the bits; this food processing technique has been observed in captive (Urbani and Urquiza-Hass, 2002) and wild Cebus (Moura and Lee, 2004). In one zoo (JZBA), individuals used a square metallic grate as an anvil to facilitate the cracking open of fruits. This anvil technique has been reported in several Cebus studies (i.e.: Ottoni and Mannu, 2001; Fragaszy et al., 2004a). Perhaps the most interesting tool use was by an adult female, who took a stick from the adjoining cage, broke it up in two pieces, and used one of them to obtain meat from that cage. The sequence is interesting for various reasons. First, it shows the ability of capuchins to solve problems. It also shows the capacity of the female to modify the stick before using it like a tool. Similar actions have been documented in Cebus apella libidinosus (Moura and Lee, 2004) and Pan troglodytes (Boesch and Boesch, 1990; Pruetz and Bertolani, 2007).

Captive capuchins employed tap-scanning to detect the presence of hidden animals inside trunks and sticks. They used their fingers to explore inside holes of different substrates and in some occasions they ripped up bark to obtain something or just to lick the surface. All these species-specific behaviors show that capuchins continue to use sensory cues to guide their search for prey and display the manual dexterity and extractive foraging characteristic of capuchin monkeys (Izawa, 1978, 1979; Phillips et al., 2003), even when in captive conditions with minimal enrichment. The captive monkeys displayed fur rubbing and dipping and licking behaviors. Monkeys in this study fur rubbed with a water-bleach solution and with onion over their bodies. Fur rubbing using several plant and animal materials has been observed in wild and captive Cebus (Baker, 1996; Gilbert et al., 1998), and it has also been reported in owl monkeys, Aotus (Zito et al., 2003). Dipping and licking behavior allows for access to drinking water even when it cannot be directly obtained by the mouth, and it has been observed in Old “World Primates (Cercopithecus aethiops: Wrangham, 1981; Procolobus badius temminckii: Starin, 2002) as well as in wild and captive Neotropical primates (Cebus capucinus: Freese, 1978; C. olivaceus: Urbani, 1999; Alouatta caraya: Bicca-Marques, 1992; Giudice and Mudry, 2000; Saimiri sp.: Baldwin and Baldwin, 1981). In primates behavioral thermoregulation is important to conserve heat during cold periods and to dissipate heat in hot periods (Bicca-Marques and Calegaro-Marques, 1998). In winter, animals rested huddling in sunny places using the curled posture. In summer, C. paraguayanus rested using the stretched posture especially in shady places (Paterson, 1986). These strategies have been observed in wild capuchins (Fragaszy et al., 2004b) and other Neotropical primates (Alouatta caraya: Zunino, 1989; Bicca-Marques and Calegaro-Marques, 1998; A. palliata: Muñoz et al., 2002).

We also observed the vertical flight response in the captive groups. In wild conditions, capuchins produce different alarm calls toward aerial and terrestrial predators and they react differently to each type of call (Fedigan, 1993; Di Bitetti, 2001; Rose et al., 2003). The same antipredator responses were expressed daily by our C. paraguayanus groups. They moved upwards or downwards in the cages in response to the detection of caregivers or flying raptors or in response to vocalizations of dogs and flamingos. The only species of raptor that the individuals detected and gave alarm calls for was a southern crested caracara (Polyborus plancus). Rose et al. (2003) reported that Cebus capucinus in Costa Rica often alarm-called for Polyborus plancus but they did not observe predation attempts by this raptor.

Interestingly, vertical flight response was observed in capuchins after loud vocalizations emitted by spider monkeys (Ateles paniscus) kept in adjoining cages. Other interactions observed between Cebus and Ateles were initiated by the capuchins and included both affiliation, in the form of gentle touching (n = 2 events), and aggression, including tail and limb pulling, chases over the wire mesh, and pinches (n = 9 events). Although interactions have been observed between Cebus capucinus and Ateles geoffroyi in Costa Rica (Rose et al., 2003), this is the first report of of Cebus responding to Ateles alarm calls. Inter-specific responses to alarm calls do exist in other primates (Saguinus labiatus / S. fasticollis: Buchanan-Smith, 1990; Saguinus mystax / S. fuscicollis: Heymann, 1990; Cercopithecus mitis / C. ascanius: Cords, 1990).

While the present study is about non-social behaviors, two social behaviors observed in the study deserve mention. First, food sharing occurred subsequent to the hunting of a bird (Furnarius sp.). This cooperative behavior has been reported in free-ranging conditions for C. capucinus (Perry and Rose, 1994; Rose, 1997) and in semi-free-ranging conditions for C. apella (Ferreira et al., 2002). In the observed case the hunter, the alpha male, shared part of the prey by allowing other group members to take scraps that fell on the floor. This sort of food transfer is the most common type observed by Rose (1997) and Ferreira et al. (2002). Second, the attempt at hunting by a three month old infant may have been a result of local or object enhancement, as the infant had the opportunity to watch adults hunting for the same prey items. As previously noted in other studies of capuchins (Perry and Rose, 1994; Ottoni and Manu, 2001), through the observations of older animals, the infants learn by trial and error to be more proficient at hunting.

Our results confirm the behavioral plasticity of the capuchin monkeys, their flexible foraging strategies, and their ability to explore and manipulate the environment, including through the use of tools. They displayed a high capacity to take advantage of the opportunities that were presented often inadvertently in the highly predictable conditions studied. Taking into consideration that modern zoos should be conservation agents not only of genetic diversity but also of the behavioral diversity (Rabin, 2003), zoos should be encouraged to increase the opportunities to stimulate the expression of species-specific behaviors in capuchin monkeys and other species.

Acknowledgments

Part of this study was supported by a Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET) fellowship to Aldo M. Giudice. We are grateful to the veterinarian and caretakers from Jardín Zoológico de Buenos Aires, Jardín Zoológico de La Plata, Estación de Cría de Animales Silvestres, and Estación Zoológica Experimental de Santa Fe, for their help and technical support.

References

1.

J. R. Anderson 1990. Use of objects as hammers to open nuts by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Folia Primatol. 54: 138–145. Google Scholar

2.

J. Altmann 1974. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227–267. Google Scholar

3.

M. Baker 1996. Fur rubbing: Use of medicinal plants by capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Am. J. Primatol. 38: 263–270. Google Scholar

4.

J. Baldwin and J. Baldwin 1981. The squirrel monkeys, genus Saimiri. In: Ecology and Behavior of Neotropical Primates , A. F. Coimbra-Filho and R. A. Mittermeier (eds.), pp. 277–330. Rio de Janeiro, Academia Brasilera de Ciencias. Google Scholar

5.

B.B. Beck 1980. Animal Tool Behavior. Garland STPM Press, New York. Google Scholar

6.

J. C. Bicca-Marques 1992. Drinking behavior in the black howler monkey (Alouatta caraya). Folia Primatol. 58(2): 107–111. Google Scholar

7.

J. C. Bicca-Marques and C. Calegaro-Marques 1998. Behavioral thermoregulation in a sexually and developmentally dichromatic Neotropical primate, the black-and-gold howling monkey (Alouatta caraya). Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 106: 533–546. Google Scholar

8.

C. Boesch and H. Boesch 1990. Tool use and tool making in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatol. 54: 86–90. Google Scholar

9.

S. Boinski 1988. Use of a club by wild white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) to attack a venomous snake (Bothrops asper). Am. J. Primatol. 14: 177–179. Google Scholar

10.

S. F. Brosnan and F. B. M. de Waal 2003. Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425: 297–299. Google Scholar

11.

A. Brown , S. Chalukian , L. Malmierca and O. Colillas 1986. Habitat structure and feeding behavior of Cebus apella (Cebidae) in “El Rey”National Park, Argentina. In: Current Perspectives in Primate Social Dynamics , D. M. Taub and F. A. King (eds.), pp. 137–151. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York. Google Scholar

12.

H. M. Buchanan-Smith 1990. Polyspecific association of two tamarin species, Saguinus labiatus and Saguinus fuscicollis, in Bolivia. Am. J. Primatol. 22: 205–214. Google Scholar

13.

M. Cords 1990. Mixed-species association of East African guenons: General patterns or specific examples? Am. J. Primatol. 21: 101–114. Google Scholar

14.

C. M. Crockett 1998. Psychological well-being of captive nonhuman primates: Lessons from laboratory studies. In: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals , D. Shepherdson , J. Mellen and M. Hutchins (eds.), pp. 129–152. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, DC. Google Scholar

15.

G. Díaz and R. Ojeda 2000. Libro Rojo de los Mamíferos Amenazados de Argentina. SAREM, Buenos Aires. Google Scholar

16.

M. S. Di Bitetti 2001. Food associated calls in the tufted capuchin monkey (Cebus apella). Docoral thesis, State University of New York, Stony Brook, New York. Google Scholar

17.

L. Fedigan 1993. Sex differences and intersexual relations in adult white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus). Int. J. Primatol. 14(6): 853–877. Google Scholar

18.

M. E. B. Fernandez 1991. Tool use and predation of oysters (Crassostrea rhizophorae) by the tufted capuchin, Cebus apella apella, in brackish-water mangrove swamp. Primates 32(4): 529–531. Google Scholar

19.

R. Ferreira , B. Resende , M. Mannu , E. Ottoni and P. Izar 2002. Bird predation and prey-transfer in brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Neotrop. Primates 10 (2): 84–89. Google Scholar

20.

D. M. Fragaszy 1990. Sex and age differences in the organization of behavior in wedge-capped capuchins. Cebus olivaceus. Behav. Ecol 1(1): 81–94. Google Scholar

21.

D. M. Fragaszy , P. Izar , E. Visalberghi , E. B. Ottoni and M. G. De Oliveira 2004a. Wild capuchin monkeys use anvils and stone pounding tools. 27th Meeting of the American Society of Primatologists, Madison, WI. Google Scholar

22.

D. M. Fragaszy , E. Visalberghi and L. M. Fedigan 2004b. The Complete Capuchin: The Biology of the Genus Cebus. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

23.

C. H. Freese 1978. The behavior of white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus) at a dry season waterhole. Primates 19: 275–286. Google Scholar

24.

C. H. Freese and J. R. Oppenheimer 1981. The capuchin monkeys, genus Cebus. In: Ecology and Behavior of Neotropical Primates , A. F. Coimbra-Filho and R. A. Mittermeier (eds.), pp. 301–385. Academia Brasileira de Ciências, Rio de Janeiro. Google Scholar

25.

T. M. Gilbert , D. A. Brown and S. T. Boysen 1998. Social effects on anointing behavior in capuchins (Cebus apella). Am. J. Primatol. 45: 182. Google Scholar

26.

A. M. Giudice 2000. Análisis del comportamiento de Cebus apella en jardines zoológicos. Doctoral thesis. University of Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina. Google Scholar

27.

A. M. Giudice and M. D. Mudry 1995. Mono caí (Cebus apella) en cautiverio: Composición de grupos. Bol. Primatol. Latinoam. 5(1): 29–33. Google Scholar

28.

A. M. Giudice and M. D. Mudry 2000. Drinking behavior in the black howler monkey (Alouatta caraya). Zoocriaderos 3(1): 11–19. Google Scholar

29.

E. W. Heymann 1990. Reactions of wild tamarins Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis to avian predators. Int. J. Primatol. 11: 327–337. Google Scholar

30.

International Primatological Society, 1993. International Guidelines: IPS Code of Practice: 1. Housing and environmental enrichment. Prim. Rep. 35(1): 8–16. Google Scholar

31.

K. Izawa 1978. Frog-eating behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella). Primates 19(4): 633–642. Google Scholar

32.

K. Izawa 1979. Foods and feeding behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella). Primates 20(1): 57–76. Google Scholar

33.

K. Izawa and A. Mizuno 1977. Palm-fruit cracking behavior of wild black-capped capuchin (Cebus apella). Primates 18(4): 773–792. Google Scholar

34.

E. Jalles-Filho 1995. Manipulative propensity and tool use in capuchin monkeys. Current Anthropol. 36(4): 664–667. Google Scholar

35.

C. H. Janson 1985. Aggressive competition and individual food consumption in wild brown capuchin monkeys. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 18: 125–138. Google Scholar

36.

R. A. Martinez , A. M. Giudice , V. B. Szapkievich , M. Ascunce , M. Nieves , G. Zunino and M. D. Mudry 2002. Parameters modeling speciogenic processes in Cebus apella (Primate: Platyrrhini) from Argentina. Mastozool. Neotrop. 9(2): 171–186. Google Scholar

37.

C. Mateos Montero 1994. El bienestar animal, una evaluación científica del sufrimiento animal. In Etología: Introductión a la Cientia del Comportamiento , J. Carranza (ed.), pp.493–527. Publicaciones de la Universidad de Extremadura. Cáceres, España. Google Scholar

38.

F. D. Mendes , L. B. R. Martins , J. A. Pereira and R. F. Marquezan 2000. Fishing with a bait: A note on behavioural flexibility in Cebus apella. Folia Primatol. 71: 350–352. Google Scholar

39.

W. C. McGrew and L. F. Marchant 1997. Using the tools at hand: Manual laterality and elementary technology in Cebus spp. and Pan. Int. J. Primatol. 18: 787–810. Google Scholar

40.

A. C. de A. Moura and P. C. Lee 2004. Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga Dry Forest. Science 306: 1909. Google Scholar

41.

M. D. Mudry 1990. Cytogenetic variability within and across populations of Cebus apella in Argentina. Folia Primatol. 54: 206–216 Google Scholar

42.

D. Muñoz , Y. Garcia del Valle , B. Franco , A. Estrada and A. M. Magaña 2002. Estudio del patron de actividad general de monos aulladores (Alouatta palliata) en el Parque Yumká, Tabasco, México. Neotrop. Primates 10 (1): 11–17. Google Scholar

43.

E. B. Ottoni and M. Mannu 2001. Semifree-ranging tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) spontaneously use tools to crack open nuts. Int. J. Primatol. 22 (3): 347–358. Google Scholar

44.

J. D. Paterson 1986. Shape as a factor in primate thermoregulation. In: Current Perspective in Primate Social Dymanics , D. M. Taub and F. A. King (eds.), pp.228–242. Van Nostrand Reinhold, New York. Google Scholar

45.

R. Pavé 2003. Estudio del comportamiento de Cebus apella en condiciones de cautiverio. Bachelor's thesis, University of Litoral, Santa Fe, Argentina. Google Scholar

46.

S. Perry and L. M. Rose 1994. Begging and transfer of coati meat by white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus. Primates 35: 409–415. Google Scholar

47.

K. A. Phillips , B. W. Grafton and M. E. Haas 2003. Tapscanning for invertebrates by capuchins (Cebus apella). Folia Primatol. 74: 162–164. Google Scholar

48.

J. D. Pruetz and P. Bertolani 2007. Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biol. 17: 1–6. Google Scholar

49.

L. A. Rabin 2003. Maintaining behavioural diversity in captivity for conservation: Natural behaviour management. Anim. Welfare 12: 85–94. Google Scholar

50.

L. Rose 1997. Vertebrate predation and food-sharing in Cebus and Pan. Int. J. Primatol. 18: 727–765. Google Scholar

51.

J. Robinson and C. H. Janson 1987. Capuchins, squirrel monkeys and atelines: Socioecological convergence with Old World primates. In: Primate Societies , B. B. Smuts , D. L. Cheney , R. M. Seyfarth , R. W. Wrangham and T. T. Struhsaker (eds.), pp.69–82. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Google Scholar

52.

D. T. Sampaio and S. F. Ferrari 2005. Predation of an infant titi monkey (Callicebus moloch) by a tufted capuchin (Cebus apella). Folia Primatol. 76: 113–115. Google Scholar

53.

E. D. Starin 2002. Drinking observations in Temminck's red colobus. Folia Primatol. 73: 137–139. Google Scholar

54.

L. M. Rose 1997. Vertebrate predation and food-sharing in Cebus and Pan. Int. J. Primatol. 18: 727–765. Google Scholar

55.

L. M. Rose , S. Perry , M. A. Panger , K. Jack , J. H. Manson , J. Gros-Louis , K. C. Mackinnon and E. Vogel 2003. Interspecific interactions between Cebus capucinus and other species: Data from three Costa Rican sites. Int. J. Primatol. 24(4): 759–796. Google Scholar

56.

J. Terborgh 1983. Five New World Primates: A Study in Comparative Ecology. Princeton University Press, Princeton. Google Scholar

57.

B. Urbani 1999. Spontaneous use of tools by wedge-capped capuchin monkeys (Cebus olivaceus). Folia Primatol. 70: 172–174. Google Scholar

58.

B. Urbani and T. Urquiza-Haas 2002. Corte innovador de frutas por el mono capuchino pardo (Cebus apella) en cautiverio. Lab. Prim. Newsl. 41(4). Google Scholar

59.

E. Visalberghi 1990. Tool use in Cebus. Folia Primatol. 54: 146–154. Google Scholar

60.

E. Visalberghi and L. Trinca 1989. Tool use in capuchin monkeys: Distinguishing between performing and understanding. Primates 30: 511–521. Google Scholar

61.

E. Visalberghi and D. Fragaszy 1995. The behaviour of capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, with novel food: The role of social context. Anim. Behav. 49: 1089–1095. Google Scholar

62.

F. B. M. de Waal 2000. Attitudinal reciprocity in food sharing among brown capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 60: 253–261. Google Scholar

63.

R. W. Wrangham 1981. Drinking competition in vervet monkeys. Anim. Behav. 29: 904–910. Google Scholar

64.

M. Zito , S. Evans and P. J. Weldon 2003. Owl monkeys (Aotus spp.) self-anoint with plants and millipedes. Folia Primatol. 74: 159–161. Google Scholar

65.

G. E. Zunino 1989. Hábitat, dieta y actividad del mono aullador negro (Alouatta caraya) en el noreste de Argentina. Bol. Primatol. Latinoam. 1(1): 74–97. Google Scholar
Aldo M. Giudice and Romina Pavé "Cebus paraguayanus in Zoos: The Spontaneous Expression of Species-Specific Behaviors," Neotropical Primates 14(2), 65-71, (1 August 2007). https://doi.org/10.1896/044.014.0203
Published: 1 August 2007
KEYWORDS
captivity
cautiverio
Cebus paraguayanus
conductas naturales
natural behavior
zoológicos
zoos
Back to Top