Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 April 2008 Height, Guy Wires, and Steady-Burning Lights Increase Hazard of Communication Towers to Nocturnal Migrants: A Review and Meta-Analysis
Travis Longcore, Catherine Rich, Sidney A. Gauthreaux
Author Affiliations +

Communication towers in North America kill millions of birds annually, and most of these are Neotropical species that migrate at night (Banks 1979, Shire et al. 2000). Estimates of total annual mortality in the United States are about 4–5 million to an order of magnitude greater (U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service [USFWS] 2000, Erickson et al. 2005). In 2000, the USFWS proposed guidelines to minimize avian collisions with communication towers. In November 2006, the Federal Communications Commission (FCC) announced a "notice of proposed rulemaking" that sought input on a proposal to require changes to tower design to reduce avian mortality. Here, we review and analyze the literature on the features of towers that can be regulated, particularly tower design and placement, to provide a scientific basis for regulation of tower construction and operation. We prepared an earlier version of this review (Longcore et al. 2005) for the American Bird Conservancy and other conservation groups in response to a "notice of inquiry" issued by the FCC in 2003 to gather information on collisions between birds and communication towers.

The ornithological literature contains frequent reports of birds killed at lights (see references in Weir 1976, Avery et al. 1980, Kerlinger 2000, Gauthreaux and Belser 2006). Two long-term studies with periodic searches confirmed that large numbers of birds can be killed at communication towers: (1) a 38-year study of a single 305-m television tower in west central Wisconsin documented 121,560 birds of 123 species killed (Kemper 1996), and (2) a 29-year study at a Florida television tower documented the deaths of 44,007 birds of 186 species (Crawford and Engstrom 2001). Because the FCC does not require monitoring of avian mortality at towers that it registers or otherwise approves, and because tower operators do not monitor mortality, bird kills reported in the literature represent only a minimum measurement of total mortality. Most sites are never visited to find dead birds, and most of those that are surveyed are visited only sporadically. Despite a number of useful reviews of the topic (Weir 1976, Avery et al. 1980, Trapp 1998, Kerlinger 2000) and recent progress on key issues such as the influence of lighting type and tower height (e.g., Jones and Francis 2003, Gauthreaux and Belser 2006, Gehring et al. unpubl. data), an analytical synthesis of factors influencing avian mortality at towers would aid policy development and focus future research. Here, we ask how design and placement of towers affect mortality of birds. Many variables influence rates of bird mortality at communication towers; certain types of weather conditions (e.g., frontal systems) are implicated in most large kills (see review in Gauthreaux and Belser 2006). Inclement weather and other physical variables, such as the effects of the lunar cycle, are beyond the control of regulators. Therefore, we concentrate on the elements of tower design that can influence bird mortality and that can be regulated.

METHODS

For each of the design features that influence mortality rates of migratory birds at communication towers (height, lighting, guy wires, and topographic position), we reviewed the published scientific literature and unpublished reports and consulted extensive bibliographies (Weir 1976, Avery et al. 1980, Trapp 1998, Kerlinger 2000). We conducted a meta-analysis of studies of bird kills at towers to investigate the influence of tower height and guy wires on bird mortality. Meta-analysis pools the results of many studies to detect relations that may be equivocal or contradictory in individual studies (Gates 2002). We included studies that met the following criteria: (1) methodology was clearly explained, (2) surveys around a tower were completed consistently through more than one fall season with >10 visits (i.e., at least fall and spring, or two falls), (3) tower height was provided, and (4) total number of birds killed was provided. To calculate annual mortality, we assumed that fall surveys constituted 75% of a year and that spring surveys constituted 25% (Crawford and Engstrom 2001). For each study or set of studies at the same location, we recorded mean annual mortality (total kill divided by number of years), the number of years of the study, tower height (m), and the presence and number of sets of guy wires and the presence and type of lighting if provided. When a study was done at a single location with towers of different heights, we recorded them separately. The effects of observer bias and predator removal were incorporated in some, but not all, studies, so we used unadjusted numbers for all towers. We transformed mean annual mortality (ln[x + 1]) and tower height (ln[x]) to normalize distributions and performed linear regressions with ln tower height and number of sets of guy wires as the explanatory variables. We also entered these variables sequentially into a multiple regression to identify any unique influence of either variable.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Tower height.—Overall, avian mortality increased with tower height. One comparative study addressed the effect of tower height on bird mortality. (Karlsson 1977) sent a survey on bird mortality to operators at all 400 towers in Sweden and received 250 responses. All towers <150 m tall had continuously illuminated red lights, whereas taller towers, which ranged up to 325 m, had an additional flashing white light at the top. Tower personnel based their responses on incidental observations, without any systematic surveys. The proportion of towers at which personnel reported bird mortality increased from 4% at towers <100 m tall to 68% at towers 300–325 m tall (Karlsson 1977). A second comparative study, in Michigan, documented far greater avian mortality at towers >305 m tall than at shorter towers (116–146 m; J. Gehring et al. unpubl. data).

At a single site, (Crawford and Engstrom 2001) reported decreased mortality following the reduction of a 308-m tower to 90 m. (Kemper 1996) surveyed a 152-m tower for several years without recording bird mortality but immediately observed large mortality events when the shorter tower was replaced with a 305-m tower. Furthermore, in instances where a taller tower had been erected next to a shorter one, more birds began to be killed at the shorter tower than before (Stoddard and Norris 1967, Hoskin 1975), presumably because of the effect of lights on the taller tower.

We found no reports of instances where avian mortality decreased when a taller tower replaced a shorter tower or where avian mortality increased when a shorter tower replaced a taller tower. This is logical: taller towers have more surface area and, usually, more guy wires with which birds may collide. Furthermore, most migrants fly at 200–750 m (Able 1970, Bellrose 1971, Mabee et al. 2006). (Mabee and Cooper 2004) found 26–46% of total migrants, depending on the season and location, in the strata up to ~396 m (although the strength of their radar may have underestimated the number of birds at higher altitude). They found that only 2–15% of migrants flew below 91 m during clear weather (Mabee and Cooper 2004). Therefore, all other variables being equal, substantially more birds will encounter taller towers and their guy wires than shorter towers, which may not require any or as many guy wires.

For our meta-analysis, 26 towers in 14 states in the eastern United States met our criteria for inclusion (Table 1). The linear regression of ln-transformed mean annual mortality by tower height was significant (F = 68.7, df = 1 and 24, r2 = 0.74, P < 0.0001; Fig. 1A). The effects of tower height are amplified by lighting, so the lower mortality at shorter towers that do not require lighting, such as the two <60-m towers in the analysis, is likely to be partly attributable to the absence of lighting. It is impossible, however, to investigate the effects of height completely independent of lighting, because all towers >61 m tall require some form of obstruction lighting approved by the Federal Aviation Administration (FAA). To investigate the influence of height for the remainder of the data set, we omitted the two shorter towers and still obtained a significant, but weaker, relationship with a similar slope (F = 17.8, df = 1 and 22, r2 = 0.44, P < 0.0004; Fig. 1B). This result is not surprising; we expected few fatalities at short towers, but at taller towers the influence of other variables is likely to confound the influence of height.

TABLE 1.

Studies of birds killed at towers that provided estimates of annual mortality. No counts were adjusted for observer bias or scavenger removal except the shorter (90-m) tower reported by (Crawford and Engstrom 2001), which had a predator-control program in place. See text for the method of calculating duration.

i0004-8038-125-2-485-t01.gif

FIG. 1.

Linear regression of ln-transformed mean annual avian mortality by ln-transformed tower height (m): (A) including all data points and (B) omitting two towers ≤60 m tall. Linear regression shown with 95% confidence intervals for individual values.

i0004-8038-125-2-485-f01.gif

Our meta-analysis has a possible bias because of the tendency for researchers to report only data that show a positive result (Rosenthal 1979). Studies that detected no avian mortality at tall towers that were searched many times may be tucked in file drawers and never published. This type of bias is well recognized as a potential failing of meta-analysis (Gates 2002). For those towers where mortality has been reported, however, it seems that a consistent relationship exists between height and avian mortality.

Guy wires.—Most towers from which large bird kills have been reported have guy wires (but see Gregory 1975). Observational studies of birds in the vicinity of towers revealed that birds are much more likely to collide with the guy wires than with the tower itself (Brewer and Ellis 1958, Fisher 1966, Avery et al. 1976). Greater mortality caused by guyed towers would be expected because of the circling behavior exhibited by migrants under the influence of lights on towers (Gauthreaux and Belser 2006). In a study of bird mortality at transmission towers in Wisconsin, (Kruse 1996) found that locations of dead birds and of guy wires were highly correlated, implicating collisions with guy wires as the cause of death. (Weise 1971) searched three towers near Milwaukee daily from 1965 to 1970. Although each tower was 305 m tall, the tower with no guy wires killed "very few" birds, whereas two nearby towers with guy wires killed more birds in frequent small kills and in occasional kills of 300–500 birds in a night. Finally, J. Gehring et al. (unpubl. data) found dramatically lower mortality at freestanding towers than at guyed towers of the same height (116–146 m).

Wind power producers also have investigated the hazard of guy wires to migrating birds. Research on unguyed wind turbines and nearby guyed structures has confirmed the increased risk of guyed structures. For example, the average number of birds killed at a guyed meteorological tower was ~3× greater than the average rate of mortality at nearby turbines of a similar height without guy wires (Young et al. 2003).

In our meta-analysis, 18 studies reported the number of sets of guy wires. For other studies, the number was not stated, but no studies included towers without guy wires. Annual mortality was significantly predicted by the number of sets of guy wires (F = 5.4, df = 1 and 15, r2 = 0.25, P < 0.03). In a multiple regression for this subset of studies, neither tower height nor number of sets of guy wires explains remaining variation when the other variable is entered first because of the collinearity of tower height and number of sets of guy wires (Pearson's correlation coefficient; r = 0.69, P < 0.001). Some towers have many sets of guy wires for their height (e.g., nine sets on a 439-m tower) or few sets for their height (e.g., five sets on a 610-m tower), but more studies would be needed to further specify any independent contributions of tower height and number of sets of guy wires.

Tower lighting.—The lighting scheme of communication towers is probably the most important factor contributing to bird kills at towers that can be controlled by humans (Cochran and Graber 1958, Avery et al. 1976, Gauthreaux and Belser 2006). Current federal regulations dictate the use of lighting for nighttime conspicuity for aviation safety on all obstructions ≥61 m tall and for structures within 5.6 km of an airport (FAA 2007). The only purpose in placing lights on communication towers and other structures is to provide for aviation safety by ensuring that pilots can see human-made obstructions.

Nocturnal migrants aggregate at lights when they have become disoriented or "trapped" by the lights after entering their zone of influence. This zone increases when fog is present in the air to reflect the light and when inclement weather or topographic factors force migrating birds to fly at lower altitudes. These mechanisms have been observed not only near communication towers but also near lightships, lighthouses, fires, oil flares, ceilometers, and city lights and lighted buildings (see references in Gauthreaux and Belser 2006, Montevecchi 2006).

Historical accounts suggest that, at least for birds attracted to lighthouses, continuously illuminated white lights are more attractive to birds than colored or flashing lights. (Barrington 1900) analyzed birds that were killed at 58 lighthouses and concluded that continuously illuminated lights were more attractive to migrants than blinking lights and that white lights were more attractive than red lights. Others have concluded that "fixed white lights are also more deadly than the revolving or coloured lights" (Dixon 1892:175) and that "coloured lights do not attract the birds as white ones so fatally do" (Thomson 1926:333). These observations are relevant to communication towers because, despite differences in height and lighting type, similar species are killed at lighthouses (see Allen 1880, Brewster 1886, Munro 1924, Lewis 1927) and communication towers (see Shire et al. 2000). Furthermore, the many anecdotal accounts of bird aggregations at lighthouses share common features of species composition and bird behavior with descriptions of bird aggregations at towers.

Duration of lighting is critical to whether birds are attracted to lights. The Dungeness Lighthouse in Kent, England, was well known for chronic bird kills. In 1961, its revolving beam was replaced with a bluish-white lamp that produced a 1-s flash every 10 s. A revolving beam causes the area around a light to be continuously illuminated, especially in foggy weather, even though the spot of the beam sweeps the horizon. At Dungeness, this continuous illumination was eliminated with the change to a flashing light. Observations during the transition week between lights, under similar weather conditions, showed bird aggregation with the constant revolving light but none with the intermittent light (Baldwin 1965). Reducing the intensity and breadth of a revolving beam was shown by (Jones and Francis 2003) to dramatically reduce the number of avian mortalities at the Long Point Lighthouse on Lake Erie in Ontario.

Some U.S. television towers were equipped with white strobe lights (e.g., L-865) instead of steady-burning red (L-810) and flashing red (L-864) lights for the first time in 1973 (Avery et al. 1976). Only one of the large one-night kills reported in the literature since then occurred at a tower with strobe lights. A witness to the aftermath of this notorious incident, when >10,000 Lapland Longspurs (Calcarius lapponicus) died in one night, considers the cause to have been whiteout snow conditions and lighting at facilities at ground level, not the tower lighting (E. A. Young pers. comm.).

Bird mortality was reduced substantially when lighting of a tower in Orlando, Florida, was changed from steady-burning red and flashing red lights to white strobe lights (W. Taylor pers. comm.). The tower was the site of large bird kills, and Taylor and colleagues had collected >10,000 birds over the years (Taylor and Anderson 1973, 1974). In 1974, the ~305-m guyed tower blew down and was replaced with a taller guyed tower with white strobe lights. Following the replacement, bird mortality was reduced drastically and no mass kills (i.e., >100 birds) were ever again reported at the site (Taylor 1981), despite many return visits following weather conditions previously associated with mortality events (W. Taylor pers. comm.).

Gauthreaux and Belser (2006) investigated the influence of lighting type on behavior of nocturnal migrants through direct observation at towers with different lighting schemes in Georgia and South Carolina. They found that although towers lit by white strobe lights can affect the path of birds during migration, no greater number of birds accumulated around them than at control sites. Furthermore, significantly more nonlinear flights per minute were seen at towers with red flashing and steady-burning lights than at control areas or towers with white strobe lights. These results suggest that although white strobe lights can cause birds to take more nonlinear flight paths, they do not result in birds accumulating around the tower. Gauthreaux and Belser (2006) concluded that the significantly greater number of paths per minute around the tower with red lights resulted from the attraction of the lights, added to the influence of the lights on orientation, leading to accumulations of individuals near the towers with steady-burning red and flashing red lights (see also Graber and Cochran 1960, Avery et al. 1976).

The evidence indicates that use of strobe or flashing lights on towers results in less bird aggregation and, by extension, lower bird mortality, than use of steady-burning lights. Indeed, the use of strobe lights has been recommended by a series of researchers investigating this topic. Verheijen (1985:13) concluded that "success has been achieved in the protection of nocturnal migrant birds through interrupting the trapping stimulus situation by . . . replacing the stationary warning lights on tall obstacles by lights of strobe or flashing type." Jones and Francis (2003) similarly concluded that strobe lights with a complete break between flashes would reduce bird mortality at tall structures.

The report by (Evans et al. 2007) also supports the conclusion that flashing lights with a dark phase have less effect on birds than solid lights. In an experimental comparison, (Evans et al. 2007) reported more calls of migrating birds around white, blue, and green steady-burning lights installed at ground level than during control periods or around flashing lights or red steady-burning lights. Although (Evans et al. 2007) presented convincing evidence that some wavelengths of continuous light influence the rate of calling in birds, further inference is limited because control sites were distant (107 km) and the relationship between calls and abundance is not well established. Data from (Cochran and Graber 1958) showed a negative correlation between birds seen per minute and calls heard per minute (our analysis, Pearson's correlation coefficient, r = −0.71; n = 16 sampling periods ranging from 2 to 10 min). (Farnsworth et al. 2004) found that hour-to-hour variation in calling rate of migrating birds was only weakly explained by hour-to-hour density of migrating birds measured by weather surveillance radar only 60 km from the study sites in South Carolina and New York. The failure of red steady-burning lights to result in additional calls of migrants in the unique experimental situation presented by (Evans et al. 2007) does not weaken the repeated observation that such lights cause aggregations when installed on towers.

Researchers analyzing bird kills at wind turbines have observed that red strobe-type lights do not attract night-migrating birds (P. Kerlinger et al. unpubl. data). Furthermore, Gehring et al. (unpubl. data) compared mortality of birds at towers with red strobe, red flashing, and white strobe lights and found that all three configurations resulted in less mortality than towers with steady-burning lights. From these studies, and the repeated identification of the importance of a dark phase for minimizing avian mortality, we conclude that removal of steady-burning lights and use of only synchronously flashing lights would reduce avian mortality at communication towers.

To reduce avian mortality, it is also important that accessory structures at towers not have constant exterior lighting. Studies at wind turbines reveal greater bird kills at turbines near lighted structures (P. Kerlinger et al. unpubl. data). Avoidance of lights on accessory structures for towers in natural areas would also reduce adverse effects on other taxa (Longcore and Rich 2004, Rich and Longcore 2006).

Topography.—Topography is known to concentrate migrants in certain locations (i.e., coastlines, mountain ridges, rivers, and hills). Considerable evidence of this effect has been gathered in Europe (Eastwood 1967, Bruderer and Jenni 1988, Bruderer 1999), with fewer studies in North America (Williams et al. 2001). Results of Williams et al.'s (2001) study in New Hampshire revealed the effect of the topography of the Appalachian Mountains on migratory birds, including Neotropical migrants traversing southeast over the mountain chain. At two ridgeline sites, the researchers observed "exceptional numbers of migrants at 2 to 30 m AGL [Above Ground Level]" (Williams et al. 2001:394). They concluded, in agreement with the European studies, that it should not be assumed that birds migrate in a broad front across mountains. Indeed, they described situations that resulted in large numbers of birds concentrated near crests of ridges and in passes. Although studies with weather surveillance radar provide evidence for broad-front migration (Gauthreaux and Belser 2003), such studies usually detect migrants flying at relatively greater heights. Consequently, low-flying migrants are often missed by weather surveillance radar and, because of their proximity to the ground, are more likely influenced by local topography. However, (Mabee et al. 2006) found that very few birds changed their behavior in response to ridgelines in a study along the Allegheny Front in West Virginia. This is not inconsistent with the observations of (Williams et al. 2001) but suggests that large numbers of birds are not found at crests of all ridges.

These studies provide evidence that placement of communication towers along ridgelines may result in higher bird mortality than at other locations. Birds can be killed at a tower whenever large numbers are flying near it at the same elevation as the tower. This can occur because the tower is tall or because it is placed topographically where birds are concentrated close to the ground. At ridgeline locations, inclement weather is not required for concentrations of birds to be found at low elevation (Williams et al. 2001). Radar studies can be conducted before siting a tower in an area that may concentrate night migrants so that the tower can be located to avoid such sites (e.g., Mabee and Cooper 2004, Mabee et al. 2006).

Policy implications.—Enough reliable information is available to implement communication tower guidelines that would reduce existing and future significant adverse effects on birds. Although additional research would be useful, avian mortality would be reduced by restricting the height of towers, avoiding guy wires, using only red or white strobe-type lights as obstruction lighting, and avoiding ridgelines for tower sites. These recommendations are included in current guidelines established by the USFWS (2000), and implementing them within an adaptive management approach is advisable (Holling 1978, Walters 1986, Haney and Power 1996). Adaptive management allows for a management action to be taken—such as requiring only strobe-type lights on all towers or requiring that towers be constructed without, or with fewer, guy wires—while continuing to increase scientific knowledge by studying the effects of such actions. Future recommendations may be modified to incorporate the findings of such studies. Many alternative mitigation strategies could be investigated and eventually adopted under an adaptive management approach, but immediate action based on current knowledge is needed to reduce adverse effects of communication towers on birds.

Acknowledgments

The research reported here was supported, in part, by American Bird Conservancy, The Humane Society of the United States, and Defenders of Wildlife. We thank L. M. Sullivan for research assistance, W. B. Kristan III for statistical advice, and two reviewers and associate editor J. Jones for extremely constructive critiques. Previous versions of this manuscript were improved by comments from J. R. Longcore, C. Zonneveld, G. Winegrad, J. Gehring, and M. Avery.

LITERATURE CITED

1.

K. P. Able 1970. A radar study of the altitude of nocturnal passerine migration. Bird-Banding 41:282–290. Google Scholar

2.

J. A. Allen 1880. Destruction of birds by light-houses. Bulletin of the Nuttall Ornithological Club 5:131–138. Google Scholar

3.

M. Avery and T. Clement . 1972. Bird mortality at four towers in eastern North Dakota—Fall 1972. Prairie Naturalist 4:87–95. Google Scholar

4.

M. Avery, P. F. Springer, and J. F. Cassel . 1976. The effects of a tall tower on nocturnal bird migration—A portable ceilometer study. Auk 93:281–291. Google Scholar

5.

M. Avery, P. F. Springer, and J. F. Cassel . 1977. Weather influences on nocturnal bird mortality at a North Dakota tower. Wilson Bulletin 89:291–299. Google Scholar

6.

M. L. Avery, P. F. Springer, and N. S. Dailey . 1980. Avian mortality at man-made structures: An annotated bibliogra-phy (revised). U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Biological Services Program, FWS/OBS–80/54.  Google Scholar

7.

J. Baird 1970. Mortality of fall migrants at the Boylston television tower in 1970. Chickadee 40:17–21. Google Scholar

8.

J. Baird 1971. Mortality of birds at the Boylston television tower in September of 1971. Chickadee 41:20–23. Google Scholar

9.

D. H. Baldwin 1965. Enquiry into the mass mortality of nocturnal migrants in Ontario: Final report. Ontario Naturalist 3:3–11. Google Scholar

10.

R. C. Banks 1979. Human related mortality of birds in the United States. U.S. Department of the Interior, Fish and Wildlife Service Special Scientific Report—Wildlife No. 215.  Google Scholar

11.

R. M. Barrington 1900. The Migration of Birds as Observed at Irish Lighthouses and Lightships. R.H. Porter. London Edward Ponsonby. Dublin. Google Scholar

12.

F. C. Bellrose 1971. The distribution of nocturnal migrants in the air space. Auk 88:397–424. Google Scholar

13.

M. L. Bierly 1973. 1971 fall television tower casualties in Nashville. Migrant 44:5–6. Google Scholar

14.

B. Boso 1965. Bird casualties at a southeastern Kansas TV tower. Transactions of the Kansas Academy of Science 68:131–136. Google Scholar

15.

R. Brewer and J. A. Ellis . 1958. An analysis of migrating birds killed at a television tower in east-central Illinois, Sep-tember 1955–May 1957. Auk 75:400–414. Google Scholar

16.

W. Brewster 1886. Bird migration. Part 1. Observations on nocturnal bird flights at the light-house at Point Lepreaux, Bay of Fundy, New Brunswick. Memoirs of the Nuttall Ornithological Club 1:5–10. Google Scholar

17.

B. Bruderer 1999. Three decades of tracking radar studies on bird migration in Europe and the Middle East. 107–141. Migrating Birds Know No Boundaries. ( Y. Leshem, Y. Mandelik, and J. Shamoun-Baranes . ). In-ternational Center for the Study of Bird Migration. Latrun, Israel. Google Scholar

18.

B. Bruderer and L. Jenni . 1988. Strategies of bird migration in the area of the Alps. 2150–2161. Acta XIX Congressus Internationalis Ornitologici. ( H. Ouellet ). National Museum of Natural Sciences. Ot-tawa, Ontario. Google Scholar

19.

L. D. Caldwell and G. J. Wallace . 1966. Collections of migrating birds at Michigan television towers. Jack-Pine Warbler 44:117–123. Google Scholar

20.

J. H. Carter III and J. F. Parnell . 1976. TV tower kills in eastern North Carolina. Chat 40:1–9. Google Scholar

21.

J. H. Carter III and J. F. Parnell . 1978. TV tower kills in eastern North Carolina: 1973 through 1977. Chat 42:67–70. Google Scholar

22.

W. W. Cochran and R. R. Graber . 1958. Attraction of nocturnal migrants by lights on a television tower. Wilson Bulletin 70:378–380. Google Scholar

23.

R. L. Crawford and R. T. Engstrom . 2001. Characteristics of avian mortality at a north Florida television tower: A 29-year study. Journal of Field Ornithology 72:380–388. Google Scholar

24.

C. Dixon 1892. The Migration of Birds: An Attempt to Reduce Avian Season-flight to Law. Chapman and Hall. London. Google Scholar

25.

E. Eastwood 1967. Radar Ornithology. Methuen. London. Google Scholar

26.

W. P. Erickson, G. D. Johnson, and D. P. Young Jr . 2005. A summary and comparison of bird mortality from an-thropogenic causes with an emphasis on collisions. 1029–1042. Bird Conservation Implementation and Integration in the Americas: Proceedings of the Third International Partners in Flight Conference 2002. ( C. J. Ralph and T. D. Rich . ). U.S. Department of Agriculture, Forest Service. Pacific Southwest Research Station. General Technical Report PSW-GTR-191. Google Scholar

27.

W. R. Evans, Y. Akashi, N. S. Altman, and A. M. Manville II . 2007. Response of night-migrating songbirds in cloud to colored and flashing light. North American Birds 60:476–488. Google Scholar

28.

A. Farnsworth, S. A. Gauthreaux Jr., and D. van Blaricom . 2004. A comparison of nocturnal call counts of migrat-ing birds and reflectivity measurements on Doppler radar. Journal of Avian Biology 35:365–369. Google Scholar

29.

Federal Aviation Administration. 2007. Obstruction marking and lighting. Advisory Circular AC 70/7460-1K. U.S. Department of Transportation, Federal Aviation Administration. Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

30.

H. I. Fisher 1966. Midway's deadly antennas. Audubon Magazine 68:220–223. Google Scholar

31.

S. Gates 2002. Review of methodology of quantitative reviews using meta-analysis in ecology. Journal of Animal Ecology 71:547–557. Google Scholar

32.

S. A. Gauthreaux Jr. and C. G. Belser . 2003. Radar ornithology and biological conservation. Auk 120:266–277. Google Scholar

33.

S. A. Gauthreaux Jr. and C. G. Belser . 2006. Effects of artificial night lighting on migrating birds. 67–93. Ecological Consequences of Artificial Night Lighting. ( C. Rich and T. Longcore . ). Island Press. Washing-ton, D.C. Google Scholar

34.

K. A. Goodpasture 1974a. Fall 1972 television tower casualties in Nashville. Migrant 45:29–31. Google Scholar

35.

K. A. Goodpasture 1974b. Fall 1973 television tower casualties in Nashville. Migrant 45:57–59. Google Scholar

36.

K. A. Goodpasture 1975. Nashville television tower casualties, 1974. Migrant 46:49–51. Google Scholar

37.

K. A. Goodpasture 1976. Nashville television tower casualties, 1975. Migrant 47:8–10. Google Scholar

38.

K. A. Goodpasture 1984. Television tower casualties, Nashville, Tennessee 1976–1983. Migrant 55:53–57. Google Scholar

39.

K. A. Goodpasture 1986. Nashville, Tennessee television tower casualties, 1984. Migrant 57:70–72. Google Scholar

40.

R. R. Graber and W. W. Cochran . 1960. Evaluation of an aural record of nocturnal migration. Wilson Bulletin 72:253–273. Google Scholar

41.

H. Gregory 1975. Unusual fall tower kill. Bluebird 42 4:9–10. Google Scholar

42.

A. Haney and R. L. Power . 1996. Adaptive management for sound ecosystem management. Environmental Man-agement 20:879–886. Google Scholar

43.

J. Herron 1997. Television transmission tower kills in Lewis County, West Virginia. Redstart 64:114–117. Google Scholar

44.

C. S. Holling 1978. Adaptive Environmental Assessment and Management. Wiley. New York. Google Scholar

45.

J. Hoskin 1975. Casualties at the CKVR TV tower, Barrie. Nature Canada 42:39–40. Google Scholar

46.

J. Jones and C. M. Francis . 2003. The effects of light characteristics on avian mortality at lighthouses. Journal of Avian Biology 34:328–333. Google Scholar

47.

J. Karlsson 1977. Fågelkollisioner med master och andra byggnadsverk [Bird collisions with towers and other man-made constructions]. Anser 16:203–216. Google Scholar

48.

C. Kemper 1996. A study of bird mortality at a west central Wisconsin TV tower from 1957–1995. Passenger Pigeon 58:219–235. Google Scholar

49.

P. Kerlinger 2000. Avian mortality at communication towers: A review of recent literature, research, and methodology. U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

50.

K. Kruse 1996. A study of the effects of transmission towers on migrating birds. M.S. thesis. University of Wisconsin, Green Bay. Google Scholar

51.

A. R. Laskey 1960. Bird migration casualties and weather conditions autumns 1958–1959–1960. Migrant 31:61–65. Google Scholar

52.

A. R. Laskey 1962. Migration data from television tower casualties at Nashville. Migrant 33:7–8. Google Scholar

53.

A. R. Laskey 1963a. Casualties at WSIX TV tower in autumn, 1962. Migrant 34:15. Google Scholar

54.

A. R. Laskey 1963b. Mortality of night migrants at Nashville TV towers, 1963. Migrant 34:65–66. Google Scholar

55.

A. R. Laskey 1964. Data from the Nashville T. V. tower casualties autumn 1964. Migrant 35:95–96. Google Scholar

56.

A. R. Laskey 1967. Spring mortality of Black-poll Warblers at a Nashville T. V. tower. Migrant 38:43. Google Scholar

57.

A. R. Laskey 1968. Television tower casualties at Nashville, autumn 1967. Migrant 39:25–26. Google Scholar

58.

A. R. Laskey 1969a. T.V. tower casualties at Nashville in autumn 1968. Migrant 40:25–27. Google Scholar

59.

A. R. Laskey 1969b. Autumn 1969 T. V. tower casualties at Nashville. Migrant 40:79–80. Google Scholar

60.

A. R. Laskey 1971. TV tower casualties at Nashville: Spring and autumn, 1970. Migrant 42:15–16. Google Scholar

61.

H. F. Lewis 1927. Destruction of birds by lighthouses in the provinces of Ontario and Quebec. Canadian Field-Naturalist 41:55–58. 75–77. Google Scholar

62.

T. Longcore and C. Rich . 2004. Ecological light pollution. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 2:191–198. Google Scholar

63.

T. Longcore, C. Rich, and S. A. Gauthreaux Jr . 2005. Scientific basis to establish policy regulating communica-tions towers to protect migratory birds: Response to Avatar Environmental, LLC, report regarding migratory bird collisions with communications towers, WT Docket No. 03-187. Federal Communications Commission Notice of Inquiry. Land Protection Part-ners. Los Angeles. Google Scholar

64.

T. J. Mabee and B. A. Cooper . 2004. Nocturnal bird migration in northeastern Oregon and southeastern Washington. Northwestern Naturalist 85:39–47. Google Scholar

65.

T. J. Mabee, B. A. Cooper, J. H. Plissner, and D. P. Young . 2006. Nocturnal bird migration over an Appalachian ridge at a proposed wind power project. Wildlife Society Bulletin 34:682–690. Google Scholar

66.

W. A. Montevecchi 2006. Influences of artificial light on marine birds. 94–113. Ecological Consequences of Artificial Night Lighting. ( C. Rich and T. Longcore . Island Press. Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

67.

S. R. Morris, A. R. Clark, L. H. Bhatti, and J. L. Glasgow . 2003. Television tower mortality of migrant birds in west-ern New York and Youngstown, Ohio. Northeastern Naturalist 10:67–76. Google Scholar

68.

D. Mosman 1975. Bird casualties at Alleman, Ia. TV tower. Iowa Bird Life 45:88–90. Google Scholar

69.

J. A. Munro 1924. A preliminary report on the destruction of birds at lighthouses on the coast of British Columbia. Ca-nadian Field-Naturalist 38:141–145. 171–175. Google Scholar

70.

J. Nehring and S. Bivens . 1999. A study of bird mortality at Nashville's WSMV television tower. Migrant 70:1–8. Google Scholar

71.

C. P. Nicholson, R. D. Tankersley Jr., J. K. Fiedler, and N. S. Nicholas . 2005. Assessment and prediction of bird and bat mortality at wind energy facilities in the southeastern United States: Final Report, 2005. Tennessee Valley Authority. Knoxville. Google Scholar

72.

C. Rich and T. Longcore . 2006. Ecological Consequences of Artificial Night Lighting. Island Press. Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

73.

R. Rosenthal 1979. The "file drawer problem" and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin 86:638–641. Google Scholar

74.

P. J. Sawyer 1961. Bird mortality at the WENH-TV tower in Deerfield, New Hampshire. New Hampshire Audubon Quarterly 14:46–49. Google Scholar

75.

G. G. Shire, K. Brown, and G. Winegrad . 2000. Communication towers: A deadly hazard to birds. American Bird Conservancy. Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

76.

H. L. Stoddard Sr. and R. A. Norris . 1967. Bird casualties at a Leon County, Florida TV tower: An eleven-year study. Bulletin of Tall Timbers Research Station 8:1–104. Google Scholar

77.

F. Strnad 1962. Birds killed at the KROC-TV tower, Ostrander, Minnesota. Flicker 34:7–9. Google Scholar

78.

F. V. Strnad 1975. More bird kills at KROC-TV tower, Ostrander, Minnesota. Loon 47:16–21. Google Scholar

79.

W. K. Taylor 1981. No longer a big killer. Florida Naturalist 54 2:4–5–10. Google Scholar

80.

W. K. Taylor and B. H. Anderson . 1973. Nocturnal migrants killed at a central Florida TV tower; autumns 1969–1971. Wilson Bulletin 85:42–51. Google Scholar

81.

W. K. Taylor and B. H. Anderson . 1974. Nocturnal migrants killed at a central Florida TV tower, autumn 1972. Flor-ida Field Naturalist 2:40–43. Google Scholar

82.

A. L. Thomson 1926. Problems of Bird-Migration. H.F. & G. Witherby. London. Google Scholar

83.

J. L. Trapp 1998. Bird kills at towers and other human-made structures: An annotated partial bibliography (1960–1998). U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Office of Migratory Bird Management. Arlington, Virginia. Google Scholar

84.

U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. 2000. Service guidance on the siting, construction, operation and decommission-ing of communications towers. U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. Washington, D.C. Google Scholar

85.

F. J. Verheijen 1985. Photopollution: Artificial light optic spatial control systems fail to cope with. Incidents, causations, remedies. Experimental Biology 44:1–18. Google Scholar

86.

C. J. Walters 1986. Adaptive Management of Renewable Resources. MacMillan. New York. Google Scholar

87.

R. D. Weir 1976. Annotated bibliography of bird kills at man-made obstacles: A review of the state of the art and solutions. Department of Fisheries and the Environment, Environmental Management Service, Canadian Wildlife Service. Ontario Region, Ottawa. Google Scholar

88.

C. M. Weise 1971. Relative abundance of small landbirds in southeastern Wisconsin. Passenger Pigeon 33:173–188. Google Scholar

89.

T. C. Williams, J. M. Williams, P. G. Williams, and P. Stokstad . 2001. Bird migration through a mountain pass studied with high resolution radar, ceilometers, and census. Auk 118:389–403. Google Scholar

90.

D. P. Young Jr., W. P. Erickson, R. E. Good, M. D. Strickland, and G. D. Johnson . 2003. Final report: Avian and bat mortality associated with the initial phase of the Foote Creek Rim Wind Power Project, Carbon County, Wyoming, November 1998–June 2002. Western EcoSystems Technology. Cheyenne, Wyoming. Google Scholar

91.

E. A. Young and M. B. Robbins . 2001. Bird mortality at the KTKA-TV tower, near Topeka, Kansas, 1998–2000 (Grant # 60181-8-P269). Report to the U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, Region 6 Nongame Migratory Bird Program. Cowley County Community College. Arkansas City, Kansas. Google Scholar

92.

E. A. Young, G. Wiens, and M. Harding . 2000. Avian surveys for the wind turbine site and the Jeffrey Energy Center, Western Resources, Pottawatomie County, Kansas, October 1998–October 1999: Project #KRD-9814. Final report to Western Resources, Inc., and Kansas Electric Utilities Research Program. Southwestern College. Winfield, Kansas. Google Scholar
Travis Longcore, Catherine Rich, and Sidney A. Gauthreaux "Height, Guy Wires, and Steady-Burning Lights Increase Hazard of Communication Towers to Nocturnal Migrants: A Review and Meta-Analysis," The Auk 125(2), 485-492, (1 April 2008). https://doi.org/10.1525/auk.2008.06253
Received: 30 November 2006; Accepted: 1 December 2007; Published: 1 April 2008
Back to Top