Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
2 July 2014 Searching for consensus in molt terminology 11 years after Howell et al.'s “first basic problem”
Jared D. Wolfe, Erik I. Johnson, Ryan S. Terrill
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Howell et al. (2003) published an innovative augmentation to terminology proposed by Humphrey and Parkes (1959) that classified bird molt on the basis of perceived evolutionary relationships. Despite apparent universal applicability, Howell et al.'s (2003) proposed terminological changes were met with criticism that cited a failure to verify the evolutionary relationships of molt and an inability to recognize homologous molts even within closely related taxa. Eleven years after Howell et al. (2003), we revisit arguments against a terminological system of molt based on evolutionary relationships, suggest an analytical framework to satisfactorily respond to critics, clarify terminology, and consider how to study molt variation within an evolutionary framework.

Terminology for molt, the scheduled replacement of feathers, is unnecessarily redundant, as exemplified by the multitude of terms for the same molt (Figure 1). The abundance of jargon stems from classifying molt in relation to environmental or life-cycle seasonality (e.g., breeding; Wolfe and Pyle 2012). Unfortunately, categorizing molt in relation to breeding or seasonality does not provide a universal system of molt terminology because not all birds breed every year or reside in seasonal environments—a fact championed by ornithologists for more than half a century (Humphrey and Parkes 1959, 1963, Rohwer et al. 1992, Howell et al. 2003, 2004). For example, Humphrey and Parkes (1959:14) noted that naming molts on the basis of “environmental and endocrinal phenomena” is problematic because such phenomena “may be related in different ways in different groups of birds, and . . . these relationships can only be obscured by making the nomenclature of plumages and molts contingent on states of any other cycle or developmental processes.”

FIGURE 1.

Diagram showing the annual plumages of Indigo Bunting (Passerina cyanea), following Howell et al.'s (2003) terminology. In addition to plumages, we included examples of redundant molt terminology associated with each plumage.

i0004-8038-131-3-371-f01.tif

The problems cited by Humphrey and Parkes (1959) are more than theoretical. For example, Jackson (1915) first suggested that the summer body molt in most male ducks should be aligned with the later complete wing molt and, because it produced a cryptic plumage, Jackson considered this a “winter” plumage followed by a brightly colored “summer” plumage even though it occurs primarily in the winter (Jackson 1915, Pyle 2005). Since Jackson (1915), variations in the terminology for duck plumage and molt have proliferated in the literature, resulting in a diverse, confusing, and sometimes counterintuitive list of molt nomenclature based on season and breeding status (Schiøler 1921, Witherby et al. 1939, Dement'ev and Gladkov 1952, Oberholser 1974, Cramp and Simmons 1977, Marchant and Higgins 1990). The misleadingly descriptive and unstructured nature of such terminology has undoubtedly suppressed or obscured comparative studies in molt and plumage variation across taxa. Humphrey and Parkes (1959), recognizing that classification of molt on the basis of seasonality and life-cycle events unnecessarily differentiates identical molts and ignores the evolutionary history of molt diversification, proposed a unifying system of molt terminology, later augmented by Howell et al. (2003), in which molt terminologies were modified to reflect perceived homologies (Table 1). Importantly, the definition of “homology” used here and by Humphrey and Parkes (1959) refers not to similarity of structure or function, but to historical continuity through inheritance with modification (Wagner 2007). Note that we prefer the term “paralogy” to describe repeated molts within a species because it more accurately reflects their assumed evolutionary relationship, despite the previous use of “homologous” by Howell et al. (2003) in such circumstances, which is instead more appropriate for cross-species comparisons.

TABLE 1.

Important molt and plumage definitions following Pyle (2008) and Howell (2010).

i0004-8038-131-3-371-t01.eps

The defining difference between Humphrey and Parkes (1959) and Howell et al. (2003) lies in the naming of molt sequences within a bird's first year of life. Humphrey and Parkes (1959) concluded that the prejuvenal molt is unique because it does not have an apparent paralogy later in life. Humphrey and Parkes (1959) also identified the first prebasic molt (i.e. the molt immediately following the prejuvenal molt) as paralogous, with subsequent prebasic molts occurring approximately annually. Because not all species of birds replace all or parts of their juvenal plumage soon after leaving the nest (e.g., many Accipitridae and waterbirds; Howell 2010) and because first prebasic molts can range from being limited to complete across species (Pyle 1997), Howell et al. (2003) modified the terminology of Humphrey and Parkes (1959) by considering the prejuvenal molt synonymous with the first prebasic molt and reclassified the first prebasic molt as a preformative molt that occurs only early in life and lacks paralogous counterparts. As a result, Howell et al.'s (2003) system (hereafter “H-P-H”) considers the prebasic molt to be a recurring (often annual) complete or nearly complete paralogous molt, shared by all bird species, that arose deep in evolutionary time; periods between prebasic molts are called “molt cycles.” Howell et al.'s (2003) simple changes succinctly aligned molts and plumages across a diversity of taxa, thereby dramatically improving upon the terminological system originally proposed by Humphrey and Parkes (1959; see Howell et al. 2003:figure 1). Howell et al. (2003) further suggested that selective pressures led to the subsequent evolution of other molts inserted between prebasic molts, like the preformative molt, and also prealternate and presupplemental molts, defined by the cycle in which they occur (Table 1). Recognizing the number or type of inserted molt(s) within each cycle places every bird into 1 of 4 “molt strategies” (Howell et al. 2003, Pyle 2008, Howell 2010).

Substantiating H-P-H assumptions is an important step toward more rigorously examining the diversification of feathers and their maintenance through molt that has allowed birds to colonize the Earth's surface. Additionally, understanding the nature of inserted molts would provide insights into why certain avian lineages were more prone to acquiring extra molts, which may have facilitated life-history evolution. For example, there appear to be correlations between migratory behavior and the presence and extent of inserted molts. These extra molts may have arisen to mitigate structural damage associated with a migratory lifestyle and subsequently commandeered to reflect honest signals associated with winter-ground quality through changes in plumage coloration (Svensson and Hedenström 1999, Pyle and Kayhart 2010, Wolfe and Pyle 2011).

Like other physiological processes and behaviors, molts have presumably resulted from an evolutionary history in which simple adaptations preceded increasingly complex ones. For example, birds molt their feathers at least once a year, so parsimony suggests that an annual molt is a shared characteristic that preceded later inserted molts (which are not shared across taxa) and likely descended from their most recent common ancestor, instead of having evolved multiple times. Although highly variable in timing, this presumably ancestral annual molt is not known to have been lost in any extant birds (although geese may rarely skip an annual molt; see Summers 1983, Summers and Martin 1985), whereas the ability to fly has been lost potentially hundreds of times (Steadman 1989). Given that variation in molt insertions and in the timing of the annual molt probably aided in the global radiation of birds, we believe that a universal terminology encompassing the diversity of molt will promote collaborative study and a deeper understanding of avian evolution.

Here, to move us toward a unified molt terminology, we provide a review of hindrances associated with H-P-H and suggest an analytical framework to address such problems. We also assess ambiguous terminology associated with H-P-H and offer clarifications. Hereafter, to be as consistent as possible, we follow Howell et al.'s (2003) convention for naming molts and plumages (see Table 1).

Problems with H-P-H and Classifying Molt by Evolutionary History

Despite H-P-H's potential to improve our understanding of molt and despite its universal applicability, critics noted the system's many evolutionary assumptions, including presumed and untested homologies across, and paralogous annual molts within, species (Willoughby 2004, Jenni and Winkler 2004). Other critiques of H-P-H suggested that Howell et al. (2003) lacked appreciation for the plasticity of molt, which can obscure homologies (Willoughby 2004, Jenni and Winkler 2004). For example, diversity in the number of molts, extent of each molt, and color of the subsequent plumage can be tremendously variable among closely related taxa such as cardueline finches, making the identification of molt homologies difficult, if not impossible (Willoughby 2004; but see Howell 2010). Additional problems included difficulties in verifying Howell et al.'s (2003) assumption of paralogy between prejuvenal and later prebasic molts (Willoughby 2004, Jenni and Winkler 2004); even Howell et al. (2003:639) suggested that prejuvenal and prebasic molts “may be analogous based on a widely shared function” rather than resulting from a shared evolutionary history and underlying physiological process. An additional problem associated with H-P-H is variation in the definition of “molt strategy,” reflecting an inability to differentiate inserted molts. Howell et al. (2003) first defined different molt strategies by determining which inserted molt occurs within a particular cycle (e.g., prealternate or preformative molt within the first cycle; see Table 1). Pyle (2008) modified the definition of molt strategies by identifying the number of insertions (e.g., 1 or 2 insertions within the first cycle), thereby recognizing an inherent difficulty in distinguishing between first-cycle prealternate and preformative molts in some species (e.g., Ancient Murrelet [Synthliboramphus antiquus]; Pyle 2009). Most recently, Howell (2010) seemingly hybridized the 2 approaches by defining molt strategy on the basis of number of insertions, which usually correspond to a particular inserted molt, within a given cycle.

In response to critics, Howell et al. (2004) reiterated that feather color should not be used to identify homologous molts and that molt homology should be considered “comparable,” awaiting verification of evolutionary origins when “a genetic basis will eventually be discovered that can reveal homologies of molts” (Howell et al. 2004:207). Although H-P-H may be correct in assuming that an annual molt is highly conserved, note that timing of development is one of the most variable elements of evolution and that the vast majority of morphological evolution occurs through changes in developmental timing (West-Eberhard 2003). We propose that the evolution of molt should be studied under the framework of heterochrony, or changes in timing of developmental events. Developmental events and associated regulatory genes are evolutionarily malleable and easily duplicated, and molt is a physiologically complicated process. Repeated annual molts in birds are likely paralogous, and inserted molts may be the result of duplication and modification of an ancestral molt. On the basis of these realizations, we believe that 2 questions must be addressed to verify H-P-H assumptions and move toward a universally accepted system of molt terminology: (1) What was the ancestral molt? (2) How many times and why have inserted molts arisen?

Testing H-P-H Assumptions

One major obstacle in testing H-P-H assumptions is the determination of whether the prejuvenal and later prebasic molts are paralogous within, and homologous across, taxa. Within species, the relationship between prejuvenal and prebasic molts may represent a synapomorphic character or, conversely, 2 independently derived traits with respect to other molts. Distinguishing between these possibilities is difficult because every known bird species has a molt resembling the prejuvenal and prebasic molt, which limits our ability to analyze variation within a phylogenetic framework. If the prejuvenal and prebasic molts evolved independently deep in evolutionary time, then regulation of prejuvenal and prebasic molts may operate independently and might be assessed through transcriptomic analysis. Such an analysis could sample a diversity of species and molts to assess how dissimilar molts are activated within and across species. Results of a transcriptomic analysis experiment would cluster molts by their expression profile, allowing analysts to determine expression and genetic similarities of molts within an individual. Despite apparent potential for transcriptomic analysis, we realize that molt is likely determined by many genes, gene duplication, or manipulation in the timing of events (Carroll 2008), thereby complicating molecular analysis focused on determining the evolutionary relationship of paralogous molts within a species.

Despite inherent difficulties in examining evolutionary relationships of paralogous molts within species, variation in patterns of the prebasic molt may shed some light on this issue. All birds molt their feathers simultaneously during the prejuvenal molt, and many groups of birds molt remiges simultaneously as adults (e.g., Gaviidae, Rallidae, Anatidae, Alcidae, and Gruidae; Pyle 2008). This retention of a juvenile trait into adulthood may be viewed as an example of neoteny. The fact that simultaneous molt, when exhibited in adults, is expressed during prebasic molts and never during the preformative or prealternate molts provides evidence for ancestral relationship and physiological connection between the prejuvenal and prebasic molts (Pyle 2008). Because parsimony and similarities in feather replacement strongly suggest a shared evolutionary origin of annual molts (prebasic and prejuvenal) within species, the number and extent of inserted molts (preformative, prealternate, and presupplemetal molts; see Table 1) between prebasic molts can be examined as characters throughout a phylogeny to estimate relationships between similar molts in different species, in much the same way that behavior is examined by ethologists (Greene and Burghardt 1978; Figure 2). Testing this assumption would determine whether multiple insertions within molt cycles (e.g., prealternate and preformative molts) in gulls (Laridae) and warblers (Parulidae) are the result of 1 or multiple evolutionary events. Realistically, finding single origins for inserted molts across all bird taxa is not likely and does not render the fundamental concept of H-P-H nomenclature invalid; it would only necessitate new definitions of inserted preformative, prealternate, and presupplemental molts reflecting their homoplasy.

FIGURE 2.

Schematic illustrating our proposed analysis whereby molt insertions are associated with individual species, across a phylogeny, to ascertain whether insertions denote synapomorphies across distantly related taxa. Our schematic is based on the phylogeny proposed by Sibley and Ahlquist (1990).

i0004-8038-131-3-371-f02.tif

Clarification of Terms

In addition to testing H-P-H assumptions, ambiguous molt terminology associated with H-P-H must be clarified. In particular, the term “definitive” is used in 4 different contexts: (1) plumage maturation (definitive plumage), (2) molts that produce a fully maturated plumage (definitive molt), (3) molts that have paralogous counterparts in subsequent cycles (definitive molt), and (4) cycles that include only molts with paralogous counterparts in subsequent cycles (definitive molt cycle) (Humphrey and Parkes 1959, Rimmer 1988, Heise and Rimmer 2000, Howell et al. 2003, Pyle 2008). Originally, Humphrey and Parkes (1959) suggested the term “definitive” to describe maturated plumages; unfortunately, using “definitive” in its original context is confusing, as exemplified by White-crowned Manakins (Dixiphia pipra). In the first cycle, these manakins undergo a partial preformative molt, resulting in a green bird with some retained juvenal feathers for the remainder of the first cycle. The second prebasic molt is complete in extent, resulting in green males and females without retained juvenal plumage. After the third prebasic molt, females remain green (and are identical to second basic females), but males transition to a striking black and white plumage that is either fully maturated or becomes fully maturated after the fourth prebasic molt (Ryder and Durães 2005). Therefore, despite being the same age, female White-crowned Manakins enter a “definitive plumage” (sensu Humphrey and Parkes 1959) 1 or 2 yr before males, even though a “definitive molt” or “definitive cycle” commences with the second prebasic molt in both sexes. Some other species, such as Wrentit (Chamaea fasciata), undergo a complete preformative molt that results in a fully maturated plumage aspect, similar to second and later basic plumages (Pyle 1997). These 2 examples demonstrate that a definitive plumage could result from a definitive preformative, second prebasic, third prebasic, or fourth prebasic molt; thus, the term “definitive” as currently used is context dependent and confuses a discussion of evolutionary relationships across and even within species.

We believe that using “definitive” to describe plumage maturation violates the spirit of H-P-H, in which 1 molt results in 1 plumage and is not used to classify plumage aspect (Howell et al. 2003). Presumably on the basis of this realization, Pyle (2008:13) used molts with paralogous counterparts in subsequent cycles to define definitive molts as attaining “stasis in terms of extent and timing, irrespective of plumage aspect.” It may seem natural to define definitive molts as having paralogous counterparts in subsequent cycles (sensu Pyle 2008); however, this definition presents inconsistencies where the prejuvenal molt, presumed to be paralogous with later prebasic molts, might be considered definitive but is followed by a preformative molt that lacks counterparts in subsequent cycles. We suggest that molt and plumage terminology should be consistently applied across taxa to better reflect presumed evolutionary history and, thus, recommend that “definitive” be used only in the context of describing molt cycles and the molts that occur within them, irrespective of plumage aspect. In this context, the second molt cycle would represent the definitive molt cycle in most birds and, therefore, individual molts occurring within the second and later cycles should be considered definitive.

In conclusion, H-P-H is a unique system of nomenclature because, like systematics and taxonomy, it strives to base molt terminology on the evolutionary history of the subject. However, no major attempt has been made to validate the evolutionary framework in which H-P-H classifies molt. To move toward molt nomenclature consensus, the evolutionary assumptions of H-P-H must be tested and the terminology solidified. Through phylogenetic analyses and a stronger understanding of underlying physiological and genetic controls of assumed prebasic molts, researchers will begin to subject the H-P-H system to continual refinement, thereby providing novel insights into avian evolution and natural history.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Thanks to P. Pyle, G. Seeholzer, and two anonymous reviewers who greatly improved the quality of the manuscript, and thanks to S. Taylor's graduate seminar class at Louisiana State University for vetting the manuscript. This article was approved for publication by the Director of the Louisiana Agricultural Experimental Station as manuscript no. 2014-241-15327.

LITERATURE CITED

1.

S. B Carroll (2008). Evo-devo and an expanding evolutionary synthesis: A genetic theory of morphological evolution. Cell 134:25–36. Google Scholar

2.

S Crampand K. E. L Simmons (Editors) (1977). The Birds of the Western Palearctic, vol. 1. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Google Scholar

3.

G. P Dement'evand N. A Gladkov (1952). Birds of the Soviet Union, vol. 4. Gosudarstvennoe Izdatel'stvo “Sovetskaya Nauka,” Moscow, Russia. Google Scholar

4.

H. W Greeneand G. M Burghardt (1978). Behavior and phylogeny: Constriction in ancient and modern snakes. Science 200:74–77. Google Scholar

5.

C. D Heiseand C. C Rimmer (2000). Definitive prebasic molt of Gray Catbirds at two sites in New England. The Condor 102:894–904. Google Scholar

6.

S. N. G Howell (2010). Molt in North American Birds. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, New York, NY, USA. Google Scholar

7.

S. N. G Howell C Corben P Pyleand D. I Rogers (2003). The first basic problem: A review of molt and plumage homologies. The Condor 105:635–653. Google Scholar

8.

S. N. G Howell C Corben P Pyleand D. I Rogers (2004). The first basic problem revisited: Reply to commentaries on Howell et al. (2003). The Condor 106:206–210. Google Scholar

9.

P. S Humphreyand K. C Parkes (1959). An approach to the study of molts and plumages. The Auk 76:1–31. Google Scholar

10.

P. S Humphreyand K. C Parkes (1963). Comments on the study of plumage succession. The Auk 80:496–503. Google Scholar

11.

A. C Jackson (1915). Notes on the moults and sequence of plumages in some British ducks. British Birds 9:34–42. Google Scholar

12.

L Jenniand R Winkler (2004). The problem of molt and plumage homologies and the first plumage cycle. The Condor 106:187–190. Google Scholar

13.

S Marchantand P. J Higgins (Editors) (1990). Handbook of Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic Birds, vol. 1, parts A and B. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Google Scholar

14.

H. C Oberholser (1974). The Bird Life of Texas, vol. 1. University of Texas Press, Austin, TX, USA. Google Scholar

15.

P Pyle (1997). Identification Guide to North American Passerines, part 1. Slate Creek Press, Bolinas, CA, USA. Google Scholar

16.

P Pyle (2005). Molts and plumages of ducks (Anatinae). Waterbirds 28:208–219. Google Scholar

17.

P Pyle (2008). Identification Guide to North American Birds, part 2. Slate Creek Press, Point Reyes Station, CA, USA. Google Scholar

18.

P Pyle (2009). Age determination and molt strategies in North American alcids. Marine Ornithology 37:219–226. Google Scholar

19.

P Pyleand R Kayhart (2010). Replacement of primaries during the prealternate molt of a Yellow Warbler. North American Bird Bander 35:178–181. Google Scholar

20.

C. C Rimmer (1988). Timing of the definitive prebasic molt in Yellow Warblers at James Bay, Ontario. The Condor 90:141–156. Google Scholar

21.

S Rohwer C. W Thompsonand B. E Young (1992). Clarifying the Humphrey-Parkes molt and plumage terminology. Condor 94:297–300. Google Scholar

22.

T. B Ryderand R Durães (2005). It's not easy being green: Using molt and morphological criteria to age and sex green-plumage manakins (Aves: Pipridae). Ornitología Neotropical 16:481–491. Google Scholar

23.

E. L Schiøler (1921). A short description of the sequence of plumages in some Palearctic surface-feeding ducks. British Birds 15:130–138. Google Scholar

24.

C. G Sibleyand J. E Ahlquist (1990). Phylogeny and Classification of Birds: A Study in Molecular Evolution. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT, USA. Google Scholar

25.

D. W Steadman (1989). Extinction of birds in Eastern Polynesia: A review of the record, and comparisons with other Pacific island groups. Journal of Archaeological Science 16:177–205. Google Scholar

26.

R. W Summers (1983). Moult-skipping by Upland Geese Chloëphaga picta in the Falkland Islands. Ibis 125:262–266. Google Scholar

27.

R. W Summersand S. I Martin (1985). Moult-skipping by the Lesser Magellan Goose in Argentina. Wildfowl 36:42–44. Google Scholar

28.

E Svenssonand A Hedenström (1999). A phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of moult strategies in Western Palearctic warblers (Aves: Sylviidae). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 67:263–276. Google Scholar

29.

G. P Wagner (2007). The developmental genetics of homology. Nature Reviews Genetics 8:473–479. Google Scholar

30.

M. J West-Eberhard (2003). Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Google Scholar

31.

E. J Willoughby (2004). Molt and plumage terminology of Howell et al. (2003) still may not reflect homologies. The Condor 106:191–196. Google Scholar

32.

H. F Witherby F. C. R Jourdain N. F Ticehurstand B. W Tucker (1939). The Handbook of British Birds, vol. 3. H. F. and G. Witherby, London, UK. Google Scholar

33.

J. D Wolfeand P Pyle (2011). First evidence for eccentric prealternate molt in the Indigo Bunting (Passerina cyanea): Possible implications for adaptive molt strategies. Western Birds 42:257–262. Google Scholar

34.

J. D Wolfeand P Pyle (2012). Progress in our understanding of molt patterns in Central American and Caribbean landbirds. Ornitología Neotropical 23 (Supplement):169–176. Google Scholar
Jared D. Wolfe, Erik I. Johnson, and Ryan S. Terrill "Searching for consensus in molt terminology 11 years after Howell et al.'s “first basic problem”," The Auk 131(3), 371-377, (2 July 2014). https://doi.org/10.1642/AUK-13-087.1
Received: 7 April 2014; Accepted: 1 April 2014; Published: 2 July 2014
KEYWORDS
first basic problem
Howell et al. (2003)
molt cycle
molt strategies
molt terminology
phylogenetic analysis
Back to Top