Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 January 2020 Anthropogenic Disturbances Drive Domestic Dog Use of Atlantic Forest Protected Areas
Ana M. O. Paschoal, Rodrigo L. Massara, Larissa L. Bailey, Paul F. Doherty, Paloma M. Santos, Adriano P. Paglia, André Hirsch, Adriano G. Chiarello
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Domestic dog is the most successful invasive mammalian predator species, and reducing its ecological impacts on wildlife is a central conservation goal globally. Free-ranging dogs can negatively interact with wildlife at multiple levels, posing issues for biodiversity conservation in tropical forests, especially in fragmented Atlantic Forest. To optimize future control programs, it is necessary to identify the main factors influencing their habitat use, particularly in natural reserves. We combined camera trapping data and occupancy models to characterize habitat use of dogs in six Atlantic Forest protected areas (134–36,000 ha). Our results show that dogs were more likely to use sites (Ψ^ ≥ 0.90) having higher housing density (≥4.00 houses/km2) or higher proportion of croplands and pasture (≥75%) relative to sites with no houses (Ψ^ = 0.23 ± 0.10) or lower proportion of croplands and pasture (Ψ^ = 0.34 ± 0.08). In addition, dogs had higher detection probability at camera locations on unpaved roads (p^ = 0.33 ± 0.05) relative to off-road sites (p^ = 0.18 ± 0.04), and in small protected areas with high housing density, that is, more disturbed sites, dogs had higher detection probabilities. Our findings indicate that the probability of dogs using a site within protected area is mainly driven by type and intensity of human activity in the surroundings. Given the urgent need to control free-ranging dogs within protected areas, we strongly recommend that managers target sites/areas within and near protected areas that have a rural housing density ≥ 4.00 houses/km2 or higher proportion of croplands and pasture (≥75%).

Introduction

Invasive species are considered one of the greatest threats to global biodiversity, and mammalian predators have contributed disproportionately to the decline and extinction of native species in a variety of ecosystems (Vitousek, D’Antonio, Loope, Rejmanek, & Westbrooks, 1997). The domestic dog (Canis familiaris, Linnaeus 1758) stands out among invasive mammalian predators, inducing drastic changes in communities and ecosystems around the world (Ritchie, Dickman, Letnic, & Vanak, 2014; Vanak, Dickman, Silva-Rodríguez, Butler, & Ritchie, 2014). They interact with native fauna at multiple levels (Vanak & Gompper, 2009), often resulting in negative impacts, such as predation, competition, surplus killing, pathogen spillover, and genetic introgression (Vanak & Gompper, 2009; Young, Olson, Reading, Amgalanbaatar, & Berger, 2011), contributing to the decline of biodiversity (Doherty, Dickman, Nimmo, & Ritchie, 2015; Doherty et al., 2017).

The dog derived from Eurasian gray wolves (Canis lupus; Shannon et al., 2015), and since domestication, it has been subsidized and introduced worldwide as a ubiquitous commensal of humans (Vanak & Gompper, 2009, 2010). Nowadays, it is the most common and widespread carnivoran on the planet (Vanak & Gompper, 2010). Under a combination of natural and strong artificial selection during multiple and independent processes of domestication, the dog now has life history traits of r-selected species (Kitala et al., 2001). They also have high behavioral flexibility (Wright, Eberhard, Hobson, Avery, & Russello, 2010) and large niche breadth (Vázquez, 2005). Together these traits allow dogs to survive, reproduce, and persist in a wide range of habitats (Ritchie et al., 2014; Ryall & Fahrig, 2006), making them one of the most successful invasive species (Miklósi, 2007).

The dog is a concern in exurban/rural areas, where they are commonly owned and associated with human housing, but they are allowed to range freely through the landscape, increasing the probability of contact with native fauna (i.e., free-ranging; see Vanak & Gompper, 2009). The influence of free-ranging dogs (i.e., hereafter dogs) on native fauna is usually higher at the boundary of native and disturbed habitats; thus, the dog is considered a type of edge effect (Lacerda, Tomas, & Marinho-Filho, 2009; Srbek-Araujo & Chiarello, 2008; Vanak et al., 2014), although this effect is not restricted to habitat edges (Vanak et al., 2014). As a highly mobile species, the dog can easily cross edge boundaries and invade adjacent habitats, including protected areas (Cantrell, Cosner, & Fagan, 2001; Sepúlveda, Pelican, Cross, Eguren, & Singer, 2015) and thus be detected 10 to 30 km from the edge (Meek, 1999; Vanak et al., 2014). Furthermore, the dog’s impact within protected areas may be enhanced through complex pathways (e.g., habitat- and community-mediated pathways) resulting from synergistic interactions between dogs and other ecological disturbances, especially at the habitat edges (Didham, Tylianakis, Gemmell, Rand, & Ewers, 2007; Doherty et al., 2015).

Although protected areas are generally considered the cornerstone of biodiversity conservation and the primary strongholds of wilderness (Bruner, Gullison, Rice, & de Fonseca, 2001), they are ecologically linked to the surrounding habitats and vulnerable to many anthropogenic disturbances emanating from outside their borders (Laurance et al., 2012; Lovejoy, 2006). Indeed, if dogs are common within protected areas, these areas are no longer functionally protected (Cantrell et al., 2001; Hansen & DeFries, 2007). Although few ecosystems are free of the influence and disturbance of dogs (Hughes & Macdonald, 2013; Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving, 2012), some are more prone to invasion than others (Pyšek & Richardson, 2010). Among tropical forests, one of the worst scenarios may be found at Brazilian Atlantic Forest, which is one of the most threatened and fragmented ecosystems (Canale, Peres, Guidorizzi, Gatto, & Kierulff, 2012; Ribeiro, Metzger, Martensen, Ponzoni, & Hirota, 2009).

Atlantic Forest has experienced a long history of intense landscape modification for commodity exports, which has been accelerated through the past century via coffee and sugarcane monocultures (Gibbs et al., 2010; Tabarelli, Aguiar, Ribeiro, Metzger, & Peres, 2010). Currently, ∼120 million people (70% of the Brazilian population) live in regions previously dominated by Atlantic Forest, resulting in unprecedented levels of habitat loss and other human disturbances (Tabarelli et al., 2010) creating opportunities for dog invasion in the remaining protected areas (Hansen & DeFries, 2007; Macdonald et al., 2009). The dog has become a major issue and represents one of the greatest challenges for biodiversity conservation in Atlantic Forest (Lessa, Guimarães, Bergallo, Cunha, & Vieira, 2016; Paschoal, Massara, Santos, & Chiarello, 2012; Paschoal et al., 2016), giving urgency to reducing its ecological impacts. The species distribution and habitat use define the types and degree of interactions that may occur between dog and native fauna (Vanak et al., 2014), thus understanding dog–habitat relationships is crucial for future action plans aimed at mitigating the dog’s impact on native fauna (Simberloff, Parker, & Windle, 2005).

In the past decade, dogs have become a major research theme in conservation biology (Hughes & Macdonald, 2013), yielding some information related to its space use and distribution (Sepúlveda et al., 2015; Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving, 2012). However, there is still limited knowledge about its habitat use and factors that determine its distribution within protected areas, especially in Brazil. Here, we used a combination of camera trapping data and occupancy models, which accounts for imperfect detection probability (MacKenzie et al., 2002, 2006), to achieve a better understanding of dog–habitat relationships in Atlantic Forest. Our main goals were to explore environmental factors that may influence dog occupancy (i.e., hereafter probability of use) in a highly heterogeneous tropical forest. In addition, we explored for variables that may account for variation in dog detection probabilities (MacKenzie et al., 2006).

Methods

Study Areas

We conducted our study across three state parks (Rio Doce, Sete Salões, and Serra do Brigadeiro) and three private protected areas (Feliciano Miguel Abdala, Mata do Sossego, and Fazenda Macedônia), all within the Atlantic Forest Biome in the state of Minas Gerais, southeastern Brazil (hereafter referred to as protected areas; Figure 1). These areas are predominately native forest remnants, surrounded mostly by disturbed habitats including pasture, croplands, and eucalyptus plantations (Massara, Paschoal, Doherty, Hirsch, & Chiarello, 2015). Abandoned pasture, small subsistence crops, some eucalyptus stands, and a network of trails and roads also occur within our protected areas, but in relatively small proportions compared with the native forest. Rural houses are found in both the protected areas and surrounding habitats (Figure 2).

Figure 1.

Location of the six protected areas sampled for free-ranging domestic dog in the Atlantic Forest, southeastern Brazil. The inset shows the current distribution of the Atlantic Forest remnants in green. FMA = Feliciano Miguel Abdala; MS = Mata do Sossego; FM = Fazenda Macedônia; SS = Sete Salões; SB = Serra do Brigadeiro; RD = Rio Doce. Adapted from Massara et al. (2015).

10.1177_1940082918789833-fig1.tif

Figure 2.

Protected areas sampled for free-ranging domestic dog in the Atlantic Forest, southeastern Brazil (photo by Ana M. O. Paschoal/Rodrigo L. Massara).

10.1177_1940082918789833-fig2.tif

Sampling Design and Field Methods

We sampled 120 camera sites (i.e., our sampling units), consisting of 20 camera sites randomly selected within each of the 6 protected areas. The minimum and mean distance between cameras was 200.55 m and 571.98 m, respectively. At each site, we placed two cameras (Tigrinus© conventional model, passive infrared sensor; Tigrinus Research Equipment, Brazil), except when we encountered logistical constraints (e.g., no site access). In these cases, cameras were relocated 50 to 100 m from the original location, and we recorded the actual camera site using a GPS unit. At two small private protected areas (i.e., Mata do Sossego and Fazenda Macedônia), some camera sites were established on surrounding lands to maintain our sampling design. Cameras were operated for 24 hours with an interval of 5 minutes between photographs. No bait or attractants were used that could produce heterogeneity in detection probability (Espartosa, Pinotti, & Pardini, 2011). Sampling occurred between 2008 and 2012. Each protected area was sampled for 80 days in both dry (April–September) and wet (October–March) seasons. Because we had a restricted number of cameras (n = 10), we rotated them among our random sites within each protected area. We sampled 5 sites for 20 consecutive days and then replaced the photographic film and batteries before moving them to another 5 sites in the protected area. We repeated this process until all 20 camera sites were sampled, totaling 80 days per season.

Modeling Probability of Use (Ψ) and Detection Probability (p) as a Function of Predictor Covariates

We chose covariates that were biologically important or those that reflected potential management actions in the protected areas or the surrounding habitat. We developed preliminary list of potential covariates using past studies and the dog’s natural history. We assessed correlations among covariates using Pearson’s correlation coefficient to develop the candidate model set. Covariates with correlation coefficients (|r|) greater than .7 were considered collinear (Whittington, St Clair, & Mercer, 2005), and one of the covariates was eliminated from further analyses (see Results section). Using the retained covariates, we modeled factors that could influence dogs’ probability of use (Ψ) and detection probability (p).

Dogs’ Probability of Use (Ψ)

We expected that dogs’ probability of use (Ψ) could vary with (a) landscape composition (the proportion of unpaved road, agricultural lands, eucalyptus, and size of protected areas); (b) proximity and intensity of human activities (rural housing density, distance to edge, and distance to house); and (c) other coexisting species (top predators and native mammal species). Specifically, we developed nine a priori hypotheses regarding factors that may influence dogs’ use of a site within protected areas (see the following).

To investigate the influence of protected area size on probability that dogs use a site, we measured the size of each protected area and categorized them into two groups (i.e., small, <1,000 ha and large, >10,000 ha). We expected that dogs’ use may be higher at sites located within smaller protected areas because those small patches of natural habitats tend to be more vulnerable to edge effects, thus more susceptible to cross-edge incursions by dogs. We also measured the linear distance between each camera site and the nearest house or forest edge. Dogs often reach high densities—and consequently high propagule pressure—where human population is high and in close proximity (Ordeñana et al., 2010). Thus, dogs should show higher levels of use at sites with higher housing density or near forested edge (Sepúlveda et al., 2015).

In addition to human activities, dog use might be influenced by other species. Specifically, we expected that the probability of dog use could vary according to the number of terrestrial mammal species and the occurrence of top predators. We expected a positive relationship between terrestrial mammal richness and probability of dog use because high mammal richness may indicate habitats with more potential resources that could supplement dog’s diet (Hughes & Macdonald, 2013; Ritchie et al., 2014). We calculated the number of terrestrial mammal species detected at each site and used this measure as a site covariate in our analysis. We also estimated the conditional occupancy probability (Ψconditional) of top predators (jaguars—Panthera onca and pumas—Puma concolor) for each camera site using a single-season occupancy model (Table 1; MacKenzie et al., 2002) incorporated into Program PRESENCE (Hines, 2006). Due to limited detections, we could not estimate different occupancy probabilities for each species; however, we believed that both top predators represent the same ecological relationship with dogs (Butler et al., 2014). Specifically, we expected dogs to avoid sites where top predators occur because they can impose suppressive effects on dogs, via consumptive (direct predation; Foster, Harmsen, Valdes, Pomilla, & Doncaster, 2010; Mazzolli, 2009) and nonconsumptive (fear-mediated) effects (Estes et al., 2011; Ritchie & Johnson, 2009).

Table 1.

Covariates Used to Model the Variation in Probability of Use (Ψ) and Detection Probability (p) of Free-Ranging Dogs in Six Protected Areas for Brazilian Atlantic Forest.

10.1177_1940082918789833-table1.tif

Dog Detection Probability (p)

We developed eight a priori hypotheses regarding factors that may influence dog detection probability (p) at used sites. We model variation in detection probability as a function of many of the same covariates used for the probability of dogs’ use (Table 1). Among all protected areas, we expected that sites located closer to edge or human habitats would be used by more dogs and consequently would have higher dog detection probability (or p) compared with more remote sites (Ordeñana et al., 2010).

Specifically, we expected that sites within smaller protected areas, close to human housing (distance or density) or edge, and with higher terrestrial mammal richness, may have higher detection probability than used sites within larger protected areas, further from houses or with lower rural housing density. While we might expect small, private protected areas to suffer more from edge effects, the covariates related to rural housing density and distance to edge and nearest house were not highly correlated.

Three additional covariates were used only to model potential variation in detection probability among sites. First, we recorded the camera site as either on (1) or off (0) unpaved roads and the number of days that cameras were operable. We expect that sites located on unpaved roads or sites where cameras operated for longer periods may have higher detection probability. Dogs preferentially move via roads and trails, which may increase their detection probability (Moreira-Arce, Vergara, & Boutin, 2015; Sepúlveda et al., 2015; Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving, 2012). Finally, we constructed a covariate for each site indicating whether top predators were detected (1) or not (0; Table 1) during each occasion. We expected a negative relationship between detection probability and top predator detection because dog may temporally avoid a site during occasions when top predators were detected (Lewis et al., 2015).

Model Fitting and Data Analysis

We used single-species single-season occupancy models to estimate probability that dogs use a site and detection probability (MacKenzie et al., 2002, 2006). Dog detection records (i.e., photographs) were summarized into a matrix of detection histories, reflecting if a dog was detected (1) or not (0) during each occasion at our 120 study sites. We defined an occasion as a 5-day period, so each site had 4 occasions per season.

A critical assumption of this model is that dogs’ use at each site is static and does not change during the study period (i.e., a site is either used or not during the study period; MacKenzie et al., 2006). We assessed the potential violation of this model assumption by exploring possible changes in the occupancy state between dry and wet seasons, using a dynamic occupancy model (MacKenzie, Nichols, Hines, Knutson, & Franklin, 2003). We fitted four models, where colonization and extinction parameters were either estimated (nonzero) or fixed to 0 (i.e., occupancy state is static between seasons), and we allowed p to vary or not between seasons. Models were fit using Program MARK (White & Burnham, 1999), and we used the relative difference in Akaike’s Information Criterion adjusted for small sample size (AICc) among models (ΔAICc) to evaluate which model was better supported by our data (Burnham & Anderson, 2002). The static occupancy model, suggesting that dog use did not change between seasons, was better supported (ΔAICc for the dynamic model = 2.85), and p was constant between seasons (ΔAICc for detection varying between seasons = 4.72). Therefore, we used the single-season occupancy approach with eight occasions (both seasons) and did not test for seasonality effect in detection probability in our subsequent analysis.

Initially, we considered models with an interaction between distance to housing and housing density for probability that dogs use a site; however, based on the ΔAICc, none of interactive models were supported by our data. Consequently, we developed a set of models consisting of all possible additive covariate combinations (Doherty, White, & Burnham, 2012) for our two parameters: Ψ, the probability that dogs use a site, and p, detection probability. This approach resulted in a balanced model set essential to interpret the cumulative AICc weights (w+) for each covariate (Burnham & Anderson, 2002).

We assessed goodness of fit (GOF) and evaluated overdispersion (i.e., lack of independence among camera sites) using our most parameterized model and the Pearson’s GOF test incorporated in Program PRESENCE (MacKenzie & Bailey, 2004).

Results

We found strong negative correlation between proportion of forest cover and proportion of agricultural lands (e.g., r = − .70). Because dog occurrence is often inversely correlated with vegetation (McKinney, 2006; Ryall & Fahrig, 2006) and closely related to human activities (Pita, Mira, Moreira, Morgado, & Beja, 2009; Soto & Palomares, 2015), we retained the proportion of agricultural lands and removed proportion of forest cover in our analyses. Our GOF test revealed no evidence of lack of fit and little overdispersion in the data (χ2 = 350.73; p value = .09; = 1.20), so we used AICc values and associated metrics for biological inference.

Our analyses showed uncertainty among Ψ and p model structures, but our most parsimonious model (w+ = 0.13), was ∼3 times more likely than any other model in our candidate set (Table 2). Consistent with a priori hypotheses, the probability of dog use showed a strong positive relationship with rural housing density (w+ = 0.82) and proportion of agricultural lands (w+ = 0.65; Table 3).

Table 2.

Model Selection Results for the Top 10 Models in the Candidate Set.

10.1177_1940082918789833-table2.tif

Table 3.

Cumulative AICc Weights (w+) for Covariates Used to Model the Variation in Probabilities of Use (Ψ) and Detection (p) of Free-Ranging Dog in Six Protected Areas of Brazilian Atlantic Forest.

10.1177_1940082918789833-table3.tif

Sites with high rural housing density (≥4.00 houses/km2; Figure 3(a)) or higher proportion of croplands and pasture (≥75%; Figure 3(b)) were likely to be used by dogs (Ψ^ ≥ 0.90) compared with sites with no houses (Ψ^ = 0.23 ± 0.10; Figure 3(a)) or agricultural activities (Ψ^ = 0.34 ± 0.08; Figure 3(b)). There was little evidence that any other covariate influenced dogs’ probability of use (w+ < 0.20; Table 3; Figure 4).

Figure 3.

Probability of free-ranging dog use as a function of site-specific proportion of rural housing density (a) and agricultural lands (b) in Brazilian Atlantic Forest. Estimates are from the most parsimonious model that included the covariates, psi(AG + HD) and p(CL + HD).

10.1177_1940082918789833-fig3.tif

Figure 4.

Some detected free-ranging domestic dog in the Atlantic Forest, southeastern Brazil.

10.1177_1940082918789833-fig4.tif

In addition, we found moderate evidence for effects of camera location (w+ = 0.57) and rural housing density (w+ = 0.46) on dog detection probability (Table 3). Dogs had higher detection probability at used sites located on unpaved roads (p^ = 0.33 ± 0.05) than off-road sites (p^ = 0.18 ± 0.04; Figure 5). As expected, sites with higher house density had higher detection probabilities (Figure 5). All other covariates had little support (w+ < 0.35) and thus did not strongly influence the detection probability of dogs at used sites (Table 3).

Figure 5.

Detection probability of free-ranging dog as a function of rural housing density in Brazilian Atlantic Forest using the most parsimonious model that included this covariate, psi(AG + HD) and p(CL + HD). Estimates are based on either camera sites installed on (black line) or off (gray line) unpaved roads. Dashed lines represent the 95% confidence intervals.

10.1177_1940082918789833-fig5.tif

Discussion

Accordingly, our predictor variables associated with human activities were the most important in shaping dogs’ habitat use within protected areas. Dogs are highly adapted to anthropogenic habitats (Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving, 2012; Vanak & Gompper, 2010), and consequently, their spatial distribution is strongly influenced by human activities, such as rural housing and agricultural lands (Paschoal et al., 2016; Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving, 2012; Vanak & Gompper, 2010). These activities reflect two factors that are thought to lead to thriving dog populations, namely, increased food supply resulting in augmented propagule pressure and favorable habitat for dogs derived from modifications in the landscape (Hansen et al., 2005; McKinney, 2006; Vanak & Gompper, 2009).

Further, the dogs’ probability of use is a function not only of activity type (e.g., agriculture) but also of its magnitude (intensity) in the landscape (e.g., housing density), even in low-density rural areas (≤ 7 houses/km2; Hansen et al., 2005). Our sites composed by ≥50% agriculture and with ≥4 houses/km2 were respectively ∼ 2 to 4 times more likely to be used by a dog when compared with sites with no agriculture or rural housing. In addition to increased dog abundance, land-use intensification leads to a landscape mosaic with increased habitat edges that juxtapose disturbed and natural habitats. This facilitates dog occurrence within protected areas through cross-edge incursions (Cantrell et al., 2001; Sepúlveda et al., 2015), where dogs may cross habitat edges and move from a human modified habitat to an adjacent protected area, exacerbating potential impacts on native fauna (Cantrell et al., 2001). This cross-edge incursion events might be further increased at the borders of protected areas (Hansen et al., 2005; Wittemyer, Elsen, Bean, Burton, & Brashares, 2008) because protected areas provide ecosystem services such as water and soil quality, lower temperature, among others, that beneficiates human activities.

We found no evidence that other variables (e.g., eucalyptus, mammal richness, roads) influence the probability that a dog uses a site within protected area. Our inability to show a direct relationship between top predators and dogs’ probability of use may be explained by the alarmingly low population status of these top predators. Due to fragmentation, habitat loss, overexploitation, and persecution, top predators’ populations are declining worldwide (Hansen et al., 2005; Ritchie & Johnson, 2009), even in areas with low human densities, due to their intolerance to anthropogenic landscapes (Hansen et al., 2005; Ritchie & Johnson, 2009). Population of these two large cats in Atlantic Forest are critically low, even in protected areas, leading to unprecedented rates of local extinctions (Canale et al., 2012; Hansen et al., 2005). This current scenario may enhance the impact of dogs on native fauna (Doherty et al., 2015) because the collapse of top predators can result in outbreaks of mesopredators (mesopredator release), leading to detrimental impacts on the underlying prey community (Crooks & Soulé, 1999; Estes et al., 2011).

Our detection results also show a preponderant influence of anthropogenic features, particularly roads and house density. Used camera sites located on unpaved roads were ∼2 times more likely to detect dogs than off-road cameras, even though a small proportion of our camera sites (25%) were placed on unpaved roads. Bare ground and linear features associated with disturbed habitats (e.g., unpaved roads, trails) can act as movement corridors and hunting grounds for carnivorans (Soisalo & Cavalcanti, 2006). Elsewhere, dogs also seem to preferentially move through habitats/areas via roads (paved or not) and trails (Sepúlveda et al., 2015; Silva-Rodríguez & Sieving, 2012; Srbek-Araujo & Chiarello, 2008). Therefore, our results are consistent with similar patterns observed in other studies where roads and trails facilitate dog movement within protected areas (Vanak et al., 2014). In addition, dog detection probability was ∼3 times higher at sites with housing density ≥ 4 houses/km2 than at sites with no houses. This pattern could be related to two factors: First, dog density is directly linked to human density (Ordeñana et al., 2010), and an increase in local dog abundance raises the species detection probability in a predictable way (Royle, Nichols, & Kéry, 2005). Typically, small protected areas have higher housing density (Table 1) and higher abundance/density of dogs (Paschoal et al., 2016). Second, dog usually center their activity around their homesite (i.e., rural housing; Dürr & Ward, 2014; Meek, 1999), where they spend most of their time (Ruiz-Izaguirre et al., 2014; Sepúlveda et al., 2015). This spatially aggregated activity pattern likely leads to a higher frequency of use and thus a higher detection probability.

Our study also sheds light on links or insights between dog and wildlife distribution in these areas. In other words, it is known that free-ranging dog occurrence negatively affects the distribution of native species, such as maned wolf (Chrysocyon brachyurus) and giant anteater (Myrmecophaga tridactyla) in the Brazilian Cerrado (Lacerda et al., 2009) and margay (Leopardus wiedii), oncilla (Leopardus tigrinus), naked-tailed armadillo (Cabassous sp.), and golden-headed lion tamarin (Leontopithecus chrysomelas) in the Brazilian Atlantic Forest (Cassano, Barlow, & Pardini, 2014).

Specifically for our studied protected areas, a companion study conducted at the same time and with the same sampling design, found that dog densities were approximately 3 to 85 times higher than ocelot (Leopardus pardalis) densities (Massara et al., 2015; Paschoal et al., 2016), and ocelots were rare where free-ranging dogs were abundant and vice versa (Massara et al., 2015; Paschoal et al., 2016), indicating an inverse pattern in abundance (i.e., abundance-asymmetry hypothesis; Vázquez et al., 2007).

In addition, ocelot occupancy at sites with a high number of dogs was almost 4 times lower than at sites with no dogs (Massara et al., 2018a), and the species tended to be even more nocturnal at sites where the landscape was intensively altered by human activities (i.e., rural housing and agriculture; Massara et al., 2018b). Therefore, ocelots may restrict their activity in these areas or use them only at night hours, when encounters with humans or with free-ranging dogs associated with human residences are diminished.

However, it is unlikely that these changes in ocelot behavior per se might be enough to guarantee long-term persistence of the species, especially in the current scenario of the Atlantic Forest biome, and, therefore, the outlook for ocelots in these forest remnants may be extremely pessimistic if management actions are not implemented. For example, the degree of human modification within and surrounding protected areas is a crucial factor to understand and anticipate dog occurrence and its impact, therefore, can promote support for effective management strategies aiming native fauna conservation.

This finding is a valuable and concerning information, given that people live within the boundaries of 70% of protected areas in the tropics (Bruner et al., 2001) and that human populations continue to grow near the edges of biodiversity hotspots, like Atlantic Forest protected areas (Laurance, 2015; Wittemyer et al., 2008). Human population growth creates greater demand for food, leading to conversion of natural lands to agricultural use (Foley et al., 2005; Grau, Gasparri, & Aide, 2008), mostly via high-intensity crop production (Foley et al., 2005). This dynamic increases the susceptibility of natural habitats, especially protected areas, to subsequent dog invasions. Our findings reinforce that domestic dog is one of the most species within protected areas in different biomes, including Atlantic Forest (Lacerda et al., 2009; Lessa et al., 2016; Paschoal et al., 2016; Srbek-Araujo & Chiarello, 2008).

The Atlantic Forest is heavily influenced by edge effects because about 80% of its fragments consist of small patches, and nearly half of the remaining area is less than 100 m from the nearest edge (Ribeiro et al., 2009). Thus, dog occurrence and their corresponding impact will likely be enhanced, not only because of this exposure to edge but also due to increasing anthropogenic pressure in the coming decades (Hansen & DeFries, 2007), reducing the conservation effectiveness of protected areas (DeFries, Hansen, Newton, Hansen, & Townshend, 2005; Hansen & DeFries, 2007).

Implications for Conservation

Due to the current scenario of the Atlantic Forest protected areas, our findings highlight the importance of buffer zones for deterring and minimizing the use of protected areas by free-ranging dogs. However, protocols associated with the implementation of buffer zones, suggested by the Brazilian system of protected areas (Federal Law# 9985; July 18, 2000; Federal Resolution# 428; December 17, 2010), are not effective for protecting species from this external threat (Massara et al., 2018a).

Furthermore, dogs in our study areas were not feral, suggesting that human residents play an important role in curbing the impact of dogs on native fauna (Ritchie et al., 2014) and are an essential component for long-term dog management (Schwartzman, Moreira, & Nepstad, 2000). The future ability of protected areas to maintain current biodiversity requires the understanding that protected areas are components of larger ecosystems and are often ecologically linked to their surrounding habitats (Hansen & DeFries, 2007).

Given the urgent need to control free-ranging dogs within natural areas, we strongly recommend that managers prioritize zones, within a buffer of ∼10 km surrounding protected areas, that have ≥4.00 houses/km2 or a higher proportion of croplands and pasture (≥75%). In these zones, managers should work with local residents aiming at free-ranging dog eradication or containment (McGeoch et al., 2016), applying multiple and complementary strategies (e.g., educational programs, neutering campaigns, application and enforcement of laws; Miller, Ritchie, & Weston, 2014).

Acknowledgments

We thank Colorado State University for technical support and Dr. Mauro G. Rodrigues (UNESP), Dr. Paglia’s laboratory (LEC – Ecology and Conservation Laboratory), and three anonymous reviewers for providing insightful comments that helped to improve the article. We also thank our field assistant, Julianna Letícia Santos, and others local volunteers.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This study was funded by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq 472802/2010-0) and Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de Minas Gerais. Support for this study was provided by Fundação Biodiversitas, Associação Preserve Muriqui, Mineiração Curimbaba, Instituto Estadual de Florestas, Cenibra, Pontifícia Universidade Católica de Minas Gerais, and Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais. The Comissão de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal do Nível Superior provided grants to A. M. O. P. and R. L. M., and A. G. C. has a research grant from CNPq.

References

1.

Bruner, A. G., Gullison, R. E., Rice, R. E., de Fonseca, G. A. B., (2001) Effectiveness of parks in protecting tropical biodiversity. Science 291: 125–128. Google Scholar

2.

Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R., (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: A practical information-theoretic approach, New York, NY: Springer Verlag. Google Scholar

3.

Butler, J. R. A., Linnell, J. D. C., Morrant, D., Athreya, V., Lescureux, N., McKeown, A., (2014) Dog eat dogs, cat eat dog: Social-ecological dimensions of dog predation by wild carnivores. In: Gompper, M. E., (ed.) Free-ranging dogs and wildlife conservation, Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 117–143. Google Scholar

4.

Canale, G. R., Peres, C. A., Guidorizzi, C. E., Gatto, C. A. F., Kierulff, M. C. M., (2012) Pervasive defaunation of forest remnants in a tropical biodiversity hotspot. PloS One 7(8): e41671. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041671Google Scholar

5.

Cantrell, R. S., Cosner, C., Fagan, W. F., (2001) How predator incursions affect critical patch size: The role of the functional response. American Naturalist 158: 368–375. Google Scholar

6.

Cassano, C. R., Barlow, J., Pardini, R., (2014) Forest loss or management intensification? Identifying causes of mammal decline in cacao agroforests. Biological Conservation 169: 14–22. Google Scholar

7.

Crooks, K. R., Soulé, M. E., (1999) Mesopredator release and avifaunal extinctions in a fragmented system. Nature 400: 563–566. Google Scholar

8.

DeFries, R., Hansen, A. J., Newton, A. C., Hansen, M., Townshend, J., (2005) Isolation of protected areas in tropical forests over the last twenty years. Ecological Applications 15: 19–26. Google Scholar

9.

Didham, R. K., Tylianakis, J. M., Gemmell, N. J., Rand, T. A., Ewers, R. M., (2007) Interactive effects of habitat modification and species invasion on native species decline. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 22: 489–496. Google Scholar

10.

Doherty, P. F., White, G. C., Burnham, K. P., (2012) Comparison of model building and selection strategies. Journal of Ornithology 152: 317–323. Google Scholar

11.

Doherty, T. S., Dickman, C. R., Glen, A. S., Newsome, T. M., Nimmo, D. G., Ritchie, E. G., Wirsing, A. J., (2017) The global impacts of domestic dogs on threatened vertebrates. Biological Conservation 210: 56–59. Google Scholar

12.

Doherty, T. S., Dickman, C. R., Nimmo, D. G., Ritchie, E. G., (2015) Multiple threats, or multiplying the threats? Interactions between invasive predators and other ecological disturbances. Biological Conservation 190: 60–68. Google Scholar

13.

Dürr, S., Ward, M. P., (2014) Roaming behavior and home range estimation of domestic dogs in Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander communities in northern Australia using four different methods. Preventive Veterinary Medicine 117: 340–357. Google Scholar

14.

Espartosa, K. D., Pinotti, B. T., Pardini, R., (2011) Performance of camera trapping and track counts for surveying large mammals in rainforest remnants. Biodiversity Conservation 20: 2815–2829. Google Scholar

15.

Estes, J. A., Terborgh, J., Brashares, J. S., Power, M. E., Berger, J., Bond, W. J., Wardle, D. A., (2011) Trophic downgrading of planet earth. Science 333: 301–306. Google Scholar

16.

Foley, J. A., Defries, R., Asner, G. P., Barford, C., Bonan, G., Carpenter, S. R., Snyder, P. K., (2005) Global consequences of land use. Science 309: 570–574. Google Scholar

17.

Foster, R. J., Harmsen, B. J., Valdes, B., Pomilla, C., Doncaster, C. P., (2010) Food habits of sympatric jaguars and pumas across a gradient of human disturbance. Journal of Zoology 280: 309–318. Google Scholar

18.

Gibbs, H. K., Ruesch, A. S., Achard, F., Clayton, M. K., Holmgren, P., Ramankutty, N., & Foley, J. A. (2010). Tropical forests were the primary sources of new agricultural land in the 1980s and 1990s. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 107, 16732–16737. Google Scholar

19.

Grau, H. R., Gasparri, N. I., Aide, T. M., (2008) Balancing food production and nature conservation in the Neotropical dry forests of northern Argentina. Global Changing Biology 14: 985–997. Google Scholar

20.

Hansen, A. J., DeFries, R., (2007) Ecological mechanisms linking protect-ted areas to surrounding lands. Ecological Applications 17: 974–988. Google Scholar

21.

Hansen, A. J., Knight, R. L., Marzluff, J. M., Powell, S., Brown, K., Gude, P. H., Jones, K., (2005) Effects of exurban development on biodiversity: Patterns, mechanisms, and research needs. Ecological Applications 15: 1893–1905. Google Scholar

22.

Hines, J. E. (2006). PRESENCE2 – Software to estimate patch occupancy and related parameters. USGS-PWRC. Retrieved from  http://www.mbr-pwrc.gov/software/presence.htmlGoogle Scholar

23.

Hughes, J., Macdonald, D. W., (2013) A review of the interactions between free-roaming domestic dogs and wildlife. Biological Conservation 157: 341–351. Google Scholar

24.

Kitala, P., McDermott, J., Kyule, M., Gathuma, J., Perry, B., Wandeler, A., (2001) Dog ecology and demography information to support the planning of rabies control in Machakos District, Kenya. Acta Tropica 78: 217–230. Google Scholar

25.

Lacerda, A. C. R., Tomas, W. M., Marinho-Filho, J., (2009) Domestic dogs as an edge effect in the Brasília National Park: Interactions with native mammals. Animal Conservation 12: 477–487. Google Scholar

26.

Laurance, W. F., (2015) Emerging threats to tropical forests. Annals of the Missouri Botanical Garden 100: 159–169. Google Scholar

27.

Laurance, W. F., Useche, D. C., Rendeiro, J., Margareta, K., Bradshaw, C. J. A., Sloan, S. P., Zamzani, F., (2012) Averting biodiversity collapse in tropical forest protected areas. Nature 489: 290–294. Google Scholar

28.

Lessa, I., Guimarães, T. C. S., Bergallo, H. G., Cunha, A., Vieira, E., (2016) Domestic dogs in protected areas: A threat to Brazilian mammals? Natureza & Conservação 14: 46–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ncon.2016.05.00Google Scholar

29.

Lewis, J. S., Logan, K. A., Alldredge, M. W., Bailey, L. L., VandeWoude, S., Crooks, K. R., (2015) The effects of urbanization on population density, occupancy, and detection probability of wild felids. Ecological Applications 25: 1880–1895. Google Scholar

30.

Lovejoy, T. E., (2006) Protected areas: A prism for a changing world. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21: 329–333. Google Scholar

31.

Macdonald, R. I., Forman, R. T., Kareiva, P., Neugarten, R., Salzer, D., Fisher, J., (2009) Urban effects, distance, and protected areas in an urbanizing world. Landscape and Urban Planning 93: 63–75. Google Scholar

32.

MacKenzie, D. I., Bailey, L. L., (2004) Assessing the fit of site-occupancy models. Journal of Agricultural, Biological, and Environmental Statistics 9: 300–318. Google Scholar

33.

MacKenzie, D. I., Nichols, J. D., Hines, J. E., Knutson, M. G., Franklin, A. B., (2003) Estimating site occupancy, colonization, and local extinction when a species is detected imperfectly. Ecology 84: 2200–2207. Google Scholar

34.

MacKenzie, D. I., Nichols, J. D., Lachman, G. B., Droege, S., Royle, A. J., Langtimm, C. A., (2002) Estimating site occupancy rates when detection probabilities are less than one. Ecology 83: 2248–2255. Google Scholar

35.

MacKenzie, D. I., Nichols, J. D., Royle, J. A., Pollock, K. H., Bailey, L. L., Hines, J. E., (2006) Occupancy estimation and modelling: Inferring patterns and dynamics of species occurrence, Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier Publishing. Google Scholar

36.

Massara, R. L., Paschoal, A. M. O., Bailey, L. L., Doherty, P. F., Barreto, M. F., Chiarello, A. G., (2018a) Effect of humans and pumas on the temporal activity of ocelots in protected areas of Atlantic Forest. Mammalian Biology 92: 86–93. Google Scholar

37.

Massara, R. L., Paschoal, A. M. O., Bailey, L. L., Doherty, P. F., Hirsch, A., Chiarello, A. G., (2018b) Factors influencing ocelot occupancy in Brazilian Atlantic Forest reserves. Biotropica 50: 125–134. Google Scholar

38.

Massara, R. L., Paschoal, A. M. O., Doherty, P. F., Hirsch, A., Chiarello, A. G., (2015) Ocelot population status in protected Brazilian Atlantic Forest. PLoS One 10(11): e0141333. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141333Google Scholar

39.

Mazzolli, M., (2009) Mountain lion Puma concolor attacks on a maned wolf Chrysocyon brachyurus and a domestic dog in a forestry system. Mastozoología Neotropical 16: 465–470. Google Scholar

40.

McGeoch, M. A., Genovesi, P., Bellingham, P. J., Costello, M. J., McGrannachan, C., Sheppard, A., (2016) Prioritizing species, pathways, and sites to achieve conservation targets for biological invasion. Biological Invasions 18: 299–314. Google Scholar

41.

McKinney, M. L., (2006) Correlated non-native species richness of birds, mammals, reptiles and plants: Scale effects of area, human population and native plants. Biological Invasions 8: 415–425. Google Scholar

42.

Meek, P. D., (1999) The movement, roaming behavior and home range of free-roaming domestic dogs, Canis lupus familiaris, in coastal New South Wales. Wildlife Research 26: 847–855. Google Scholar

43.

Miklósi, A., (2007) Dog behaviour, evolution, and cognition, London, England: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar

44.

Miller, K. K., Ritchie, E. G., Weston, M. A., (2014) The human dimensions of dog-wildlife interactions. In: Gompper, M. E., (ed.) Free ranging dogs and wildlife conservation, Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 286–301. Google Scholar

45.

Moreira-Arce, D., Vergara, P. M., Boutin, S., (2015) Diurnal human activity and introduced species affect occurrence of carnivores in a human-dominated landscape. PloS One 10(9): e0137854. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137854Google Scholar

46.

Ordeñana, M. A., Crooks, K. R., Boydston, E. E., Fisher, R. N., Lyren, L. M., Siudyla, S., Turschak, G. M., (2010) Effects of urbanization on carnivore species distribution and richness. Journal of Mammalogy 91: 1322–1331. Google Scholar

47.

Paschoal, A. M. O., Massara, R. L., Bailey, L. L., Kendall, W. L., Doherty, P. F., Hirsch, A., Paglia, A. P., (2016) Use of Atlantic Forest protected areas by free-ranging dogs: Estimating abundance and persistence of use. Ecosphere 76: 67–76. Google Scholar

48.

Paschoal, A. M. O., Massara, R. L., Santos, J. L., Chiarello, A. G., (2012) Is the domestic dog becoming an abundant species in the Atlantic Forest? A study case in southeastern Brazil. Mammalia 76: 67–76. Google Scholar

49.

Pita, R., Mira, A., Moreira, F., Morgado, R., Beja, P., (2009) Influence of landscape characteristics on carnivore diversity and abundance in Mediterranean farmland. Agriculture, Ecosystems & Environment 132: 57–65. Google Scholar

50.

Pyšek, P., Richardson, D. M., (2010) Invasive species, environmental change and management, and health. Annual Review of Environment and Resources 35: 25–55. Google Scholar

51.

Ribeiro, M. C., Metzger, J. P., Martensen, A. C., Ponzoni, F. J., Hirota, M. M., (2009) The Brazilian Atlantic Forest: How much is left, and how is the remaining forest distributed? Implications for conservation. Biological Conservation 142: 1141–115. Google Scholar

52.

Ritchie, E. G., Dickman, C. R., Letnic, M., Vanak, A. T., (2014) Dogs as predators and trophic regulators. In: Gompper, M. E., (ed.) Free-ranging dogs and wildlife conservation, Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 55–65. Google Scholar

53.

Ritchie, E. G., Johnson, C. N., (2009) Predator interactions, mesopredator release and biodiversity conservation. Ecological Letters 12: 982–998. Google Scholar

54.

Royle, J. A., Nichols, J. D., Kéry, M., (2005) Modelling occurrence and abundance of species when detection is imperfect. Oikos 110: 353–359. Google Scholar

55.

Ruiz-Izaguirre, E., Woersem, A., Eilers, K., Wieren, S., Bosch, G., Zijpp, A., Boer, I., (2014) Roaming characteristics and feeding practices of village dogs scavenging sea-turtle nests. Animal Conservation 18: 146–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/acv.12143Google Scholar

56.

Ryall, K. L., Fahrig, L., (2006) Response of predators to loss and fragmentation of prey habitat: A review of theory. Ecology 87: 1086–1093. Google Scholar

57.

Schwartzman, S., Moreira, A., Nepstad, D., (2000) Rethinking tropical forest conservation: Perils in parks. Conservation Biology 14: 1351–1357. Google Scholar

58.

Sepúlveda, M. A., Pelican, K., Cross, P., Eguren, A., Singer, R. S., (2015) Fine-scale movements of rural free-ranging dogs in conservation areas in the temperate rainforest of the coastal range of southern Chile. Mammalian Biology 80: 290–297. Google Scholar

59.

Shannon, L. M., Boyko, R. H., Castelhano, M., Corey, E., Hayward, J. J., McLean, C., . . . Boyko, A. R. (2015). Genetic structure in village dogs reveals a Central Asian domestication origin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112, 13639–13644. Google Scholar

60.

Silva-Rodríguez, E. A., Sieving, K. E., (2012) Domestic dogs shape the landscape-scale distribution of a threatened forest ungulate. Biological Conservation 150: 103–110. Google Scholar

61.

Simberloff, D., Parker, I., Windle, P. N., (2005) Introduced species policy, management, and future research needs. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 3: 12–20. Google Scholar

62.

Soisalo, M. K., Cavalcanti, S. M. C., (2006) Estimating the density of a jaguar population in the Brazilian Pantanal using camera-traps and capture-recapture sampling in combination with GPS radio-telemetry. Biological Conservation 129: 487–496. Google Scholar

63.

Soto, C. A., Palomares, F., (2015) Human-related factors regulate the presence of domestic dogs in protected areas. Oryx 49: 254–260. Google Scholar

64.

Srbek-Araujo, A. C., Chiarello, A. G., (2008) Domestic dogs in Atlantic Forest protected areas of south-eastern Brazil: A camera-trapping study on patterns of entrance and site occupancy rates. Brazilian Journal of Biology 68: 771–779. Google Scholar

65.

Tabarelli, M., Aguiar, A. V., Ribeiro, M. C., Metzger, J. P., Peres, C. A., (2010) Prospects for biodiversity conservation in the Atlantic Forest: Lessons from aging human modified landscapes. Biological Conservation 143: 2328–2340. Google Scholar

66.

Vanak, A. T., Gompper, M. E., (2009) Dogs Canis familiaris as carnivores: Their role and function in intraguild competition. Mammalian Review 39: 265–283. Google Scholar

67.

Vanak, A. T., Gompper, M. E., (2010) Interference competition at the landscape level: The effect of free-ranging dogs on a native mesocarnivore. Journal of Applied Ecology 47: 1225–1232. Google Scholar

68.

Vanak, A. T., Dickman, C. R., Silva-Rodríguez, E. A., Butler, J. R. A., Ritchie, E., (2014) Top-dogs and underdogs: Competition between dogs and sympatric carnivores. In: Gompper, M. E., (ed.) Free ranging dogs and wildlife conservation, Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 69–87. Google Scholar

69.

Vázquez, D. P., (2005) Exploring the relationship between niche breadth and invasion success. In: Cadotte, M. W., McMahon, S. M., Fukami, T., (eds) Conceptual ecology and invasion biology, New York, NY: Springer, pp. 317–332. Google Scholar

70.

Vázquez, D. P., Melián, C. J., Williams, N. M., Blüthgen, N., Krasnov, B. R., Poulin, R., (2007) Species abundance and asymmetric interaction strength in ecological networks. Oikos 116: 1120–1127. Google Scholar

71.

Vitousek, P. M., D’Antonio, C. M., Loope, L. L., Rejmanek, M., Westbrooks, R., (1997) Introduced species: A significant component of human-caused global change. New Zealand Journal of Ecology 21: 1–16. Google Scholar

72.

White, G. C., Burnham, K. P., (1999) Program MARK: Survival estimation from populations of marked animals. Bird Study 46: 120–139. Google Scholar

73.

Whittington, J., St Clair, C. C., Mercer, G., (2005) Spatial responses of wolves to roads and trails in mountain valleys. Ecological Applications 15: 543–553. Google Scholar

74.

Wittemyer, G., Elsen, P., Bean, W. T., Burton, C. O., Brashares, J. S., (2008) Accelerated human population growth at protected area edges. Science 321: 123–126. Google Scholar

75.

Wright, T. F., Eberhard, J. R., Hobson, E. A., Avery, M. L., Russello, M. A., (2010) Behavioral flexibility and species invasions: The adaptive flexibility hypothesis. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 22: 393–404. Google Scholar

76.

Young, J. K., Olson, K. A., Reading, R. R., Amgalanbaatar, S., Berger, J., (2011) Is wildlife going to the dogs? Impacts of feral and free-roaming dogs on wildlife populations. Bioscience 61: 125–132. Google Scholar
© The Author(s) 2018 Creative Commons Non Commercial CC BY-NC: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
Ana M. O. Paschoal, Rodrigo L. Massara, Larissa L. Bailey, Paul F. Doherty, Paloma M. Santos, Adriano P. Paglia, André Hirsch, and Adriano G. Chiarello "Anthropogenic Disturbances Drive Domestic Dog Use of Atlantic Forest Protected Areas," Tropical Conservation Science 11(1), (1 January 2020). https://doi.org/10.1177/1940082918789833
Received: 29 May 2018; Accepted: 21 June 2018; Published: 1 January 2020
KEYWORDS
BIOLOGICAL INVASIONS
Brazil
Canis familiaris
domestic species
occupancy models
Back to Top