Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 November 2002 Female Preference for Nest Size in the Stream Goby Rhinogobius sp. DA
Daisuke Takahashi, Masanori Kohda
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Females of the stream goby Rhinogobius sp. DA with paternal care favor males courting in fast water currents, whereby they mate males of high parental ability. Here we examined female choice of male nest size of this goby in laboratory. The dichotomous choice experiment clearly indicated that females prefer large nests. Spawning at large nests seems to improve egg survival rates in natural habitats in this goby. We discuss the possibility of multiple criteria in mate choice of this goby.

INTRODUCTION

Female mate choice may evolve whenever access to males of a certain quality or resources provided by males limits the reproductive success of females (Andersson, 1994). Females choose mates based on various criteria such as male morph traits, physical conditions, courtship behaviors, or conditions of resources males possess (Andersson, 1994). Recently, the importance of consideration of multiple criteria in female choice has been addressed (Pomiankowski and Iwasa, 1993; Kraak et al., 1999; Widemo and Sæther, 1999). However, most empirical studies of female choice focused on a single criterion partly because of the difficulty of discrimination among the criteria (Thompson, 1986; Hastings, 1988).

Rhinogobius sp. DA (Dark type) (Kawanabe and Mizuno, 1989) is a small fish living in deep pools of short streams with relatively steep gradients in Japan (Mizuno et al., 1979). After making nest under stones on the shallow riffle, a male attracts a female by courtship displays in the deep pool and lead to his nest (Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999; Takahashi, 2000a). Thereafter, the female deposits her clutch of eggs in a single layer on the nest ceiling, and the male attends to the eggs until hatching. Females have no choosiness of male body size, and prefer males courting in fast currents in the pool (Takahashi and Kohda, 2001; Takahashi, 2000b), and such males have good parental ability based on sufficient energy stock (Takahashi, 2000b).

In Rhinogobius sp. DA the number of eggs in nests is positively related to the nest size, and males compete for large nests (Takahashi et al., 2001). Some nests contain a large amount of eggs, which are much more than the female fecundity (Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999), indicating that some males can fertilize eggs from more than one female in a single breeding cycle. Females spawn a whole clutch at a time (D. Takahashi, personal observation), and a brood of a nest contains eggs at the same developmental stage regardless of brood size (Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999). Hence, males with larger nests may mate with more females simultaneously. In preliminary observations, we noted that females seem to have choosiness not only of male courting ability in water currents but also his nest characters. If females preferentially spawn eggs in large nests, the preference partly explains the male-male competition for large nests, and provides an evidence of female choice based on multiple criteria. Field observations on the female preference for nest in this goby, however, are quite difficult because riffles in which the Rhinogobius males make their nests are inconspicuous and often in turbulent water (Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999). The aim of the present study is to experimentally examine female choice of large nests in this goby under the laboratory condition.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

A total of 190 adult fish of Rhinogobius were collected from the Kashiwa River (3.2 km long), southwestern Shikoku Islands, Japan (33°1′N, 132°30′E) in a few months before breeding season (early April - early July) in 1998. We stocked about 20 fish of the same sex in 9 tanks [60×28×30 cm (H)] supplied with filtering water. The tank contained PVC pipes and artificial waterweeds as refuge sites. They were adequately fed daily with defrosted bloodworm and flake food (TetraFin, Tetra Co) during the experiments. The stock tanks and experiment tanks (see below) were kept in 14L10D photoperiod by fluorescence lights and in water temperature ca. 18°C. The conditions of the experiment tanks are similar to the natural habitat during the breeding period.

Female preference for nest size was examined in experiment tanks [60×28×30 cm (H)] with gravel bottom of 3 cm thick. A transparent PVC board separated the tank into two male-compartments (30×14 cm) and a female-compartment (30×28 cm) where air-stone was set for aeration (Fig. 1). Two males separated by the opaque board could not see each other. Males are larger than females, and only females could pass through the slits of 0.75 cm width at the bottom of the transparent board. Small (10×10 cm) and large (20×10 cm) clay tiles were put in two male-compartments (Fig. 1). The undersurface area of small tile was twice as large as the mean area of egg clutch in male nests of this fish in the field (50.9 cm2), and that of large one was larger than the maximum size (152.9 cm2 of multiple clutches). Males made a nest hole under the tile as large as possible: small nests under small tiles and large ones under large tiles.

Fig. 1

A diagram of the tanks used in the experiments of female choice of male nests. In fact, male compartments are divided by an opaque board.

i0289-0003-19-11-1241-f01.gif

Asymmetries in eye use in relation to brain functions have been reported in some fishes (Bisazza et al., 1997). In a dichotomous choice experiment, female choosiness for right- or left-side males would be affected by such asymmetries. To avoid the effect of the asymmetries, large nests were put in right male-compartments from the female compartment in 8 of 17 nest choice experiments.

Female Rhinogobius choose males with good physical condition over a certain threshold level (Takahashi and Kohda, 2001), and thus, such good condition males were used in the experiment. After weighing and measuring size, we introduced two sexually active males of different total length (TL) (mean difference was 14.7 mm±4.8 SD, n=17) into two male compartments. All males made nests under the tiles by the next day. Females ready to spawn exhibit a nuptial color of deep yellow on the enlarged abdomen. A nuptial female was released into the female-compartment 2 days after the male introduction. All males eagerly courted females entering his compartment. In all trials, females responded to courtships of two males, and entered both male nests before spawning. The loose of the nuptial coloration on the slender belly in the females was the sign of spawning. Females spawn a whole clutch at one nest (D. Takahashi, personal observation). Soon after checking the sigh, we turned over both tiles and checked the spawning nests. Mean TL of males and females were 80.8 mm (±8.4 SD, n=34) and 62.0 mm (±3.9, n=17), respectively. The condition factors [K=body weight (mg)×100/standard length (mm)3.04; Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999] of males before the experiments were not so varied (mean=1.94, CV=0.11, n=30; no data in 4 males). Because the mean K of which males can court in fast water currents is 1.80 (±0.12 SD, range=1.61-1.95; Takahashi, 2000b; Takahashi and Kohda, 2001), the experiment males had so good physical conditions that they may be able to court females in the fast currents. Although courtship intensity can be a criterion of mate choice in some fish (e.g., Karino, 1995), Rhinogobius females do not choose males depending on the courtship intensity (Takahashi, 2000b). Therefore, in the present study, we did not consider the courtship intensity as criteria for female choice.

In statistical analysis, we used binomial test and multiple logistic regression analysis by SPSS 6.1 (SPSS Inc.). The logistic regression analysis was used in order to clear independent effects of male body size, nest position and nest size to the female mating preference. Backward step-wise analysis was used to remove variables from the model. Following Forslund (2000), one of the males in each trial was randomly chosen as the focal male, and his spawning success in the trial (i.e., whether the trial female mated with him or not) was used as the binomial dependent variable in the logistic regression analysis. As independent variables we entered relative TL, nest position in right or left male compartment and nest size. We calculated the relative TL by first estimating the mean TL within a trial pair of males, then subtracting the mean TL from the focal male's TL, and finally dividing this deviance by the mean TL.

RESULTS

The mean period from the female introduction to spawning was 2.7 days (±1.3 SD, n=17). Before spawning, females repeatedly visited and entered each of the two nests, and males vigorously courted the females.

In the 17 nest choice experiments, females spawned the whole clutch in either one of the two nests. Most females spawned at large nests (14 spawnings in the large nest, 3 in the small one; binomial test, P<0.05). Nine females spawned in right nests, and 8 in left (P>0.3). Although females seemed to mate smaller males more, the female selectivity for male body size was not significant (6 matings with the large male, 11 with the small one; P>0.1). In the multiple logistic regression analysis, a model composed by the nest size produced the best fit (Table 1; χ32 =10.06, P<0.05; −2 log likelihood=13.44, goodness of fit=13.32).

Table 1

Logistic regression analysis with female preference as dependent variable and male relative otal length (TL), nest position and nest size as independent variables. The table shows statics after the first step, when all variables and the constant have been entered into the model. Sample size is 17.

i0289-0003-19-11-1241-t01.gif

DISCUSSION

In some fishes with paternal care, females prefer large males because of their high defending ability against nest intruders (e.g., Bisazza and Marconato, 1988) or vigorous parental care due to sufficient energy reserves (Downhower and Brown, 1980). In this study, however, there was no female selectivity for male size, as was in the previous studies (Takahashi, 2000b; Takahashi and Kohda, 2001). Males of this goby tightly close the nest entrance with gravel after spawning (Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999). The non-preference for large males may correspond with low risks of egg predations by the tight closing. In the Kashiwa River from which the individuals for the present experiment were collected, candidates for the egg predators are only Rhinogobius sp. DA, and there are few non-guarding males that eat conspecific eggs in the breeding period (Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999). Thus, the occurrence of hetero-cannibalism to eggs reported in some fish (e.g., Bisazza and Marconato, 1988) seems to be rare in this fish. Furthermore, in Rhinogobius sp. DA male body size is unlikely to be related to parental ability (Takahashi, 2000b).

Why does female Rhinogobius prefer large nests? In many fish species where males care for broods in nests, large brood sizes may induce high intensity of paternal care (e.g., Coleman et al., 1985; Sargent and Gross, 1993). It is also reported that a large brood with multiple clutches may have dilution effect of filial cannibalism on each female's own eggs (Rohwer, 1978; Unger and Sargent, 1988; Kraak and Groothuis, 1994). These reports suggest that survival of eggs increases with the number of females spawning in a nest, and that females should prefer males having nests where other females have already spawned or will spawn. The two aspects of the intensive male care and dilution effect might be of the case in a freshwater goby of which females prefer large nests which tend to have large brood size (Bisazza et al., 1989). Similarly, in our goby larger nests have larger broods (Takahashi et al., 2001). Female Rhinogobius should receive those benefits in relation to their egg survival by the choice of large nests. The female preference for large nests will make males compete for such nests, and may indirectly cause the sexual size dimorphism of this fish (Takahashi et al., 2001).

In natural habitats, Rhinogobius females first encounter males in deep stream pools, and then are led to the male nests in riffles (Takahashi and Yanagisawa, 1999; Takahashi, 2000a). If the male size is strictly related to the nest size, the former can be an accurate information of the latter. In fact, male sizes are roughly related to nest sizes (Takahashi et al., 2001), but females do not choose large males in the pool (Takahashi, 2000b: Takahashi and Kohda, 2001). These observations and the present study strongly suggest that females directly check the nest size. Thus, Rhinogobius females may have two criteria in the procedure of the mate choice. At first females may choose males of sufficient energy stock (i.e., good parental care) through the dancing ability in fast current in deep pools (Takahashi and Kohda, 2001), and after arriving at nest sites in riffle females may examine the size of the nest the male possesses.

Both of the male quality and nest quality may more or less affect brood hatching rates in many fish species of which males care for offspring in nests. Many studies of female choice in such fishes have focused on either male traits [e.g., body size (Bisazza and Marconato, 1988) and intensity of courtship (Karino, 1995)] or nest quality [e.g., nest size (Bisazza et al., 1989) and presence of eggs (Kraak and Groothuis, 1994)]. The finding of the present study implies that multiple criteria in female choice should be taken into consideration in fishes with paternal males of nest care for deeper elucidation of their sexual selection patterns.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Y. Yanagisawa and members of UWA Research Station of Ehime University, and members of Laboratory of Animal Sociology of Osaka City University for their support in this study. Y. Sakai and N. Okuda kindly read an early draft of the manuscript. Two anonymous reviewers offered constructive criticism for the revise of the manuscript. This work was partly supported in finance by Osaka City Grant for Graduate Course Students and Kansai Organization for Nature Conservation Grant.

REFERENCES

1.

M. Andersson 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton University Press. New Jersey. Google Scholar

2.

A. Bisazza and A. Marconato . 1988. Female mate choice, male-male competition and parental care in the river bullhead, Cottus gobio L. (Pisces, Cottidae). Anim Behav 36:1352–1360. Google Scholar

3.

A. Bisazza, A. Marconato, and G. Marin . 1989. Male competition and female choice in Padogobius martensi (Pisces, Gobiidae). Anim Behav 38:406–413. Google Scholar

4.

A. Bisazza, R. Pignatti, and G. Vallortigara . 1997. Laterality in detour behaviour: interspecific variation in poeciliid fish. Anim Behav 54:1273–1281. Google Scholar

5.

R. M. Coleman, M. R. Gross, and R. C. Sargent . 1985. Parental investment decision rules: a test in the bluegill sunfish. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:59–66. Google Scholar

6.

J. F. Downhower and L. Brown . 1980. Mate preference of female mottled sculpins, Cottus bairdi. Anim Behav 28:728–734. Google Scholar

7.

P. Forslund 2000. Male-male competition and large size mating advantage in European earwigs, Forficula auricularia. Anim Behav 59:753–762. Google Scholar

8.

P. A. Hastings 1988. Correlates of male reproductive success in the browncheek blenny, Acanthemblemaria crockeri (Blennioidea: Chaenopsidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22:95–102. Google Scholar

9.

H. Kawanabe and N. Mizuno . ed. 1989. Freshwater fishes of Japan. Yama-Kei. Tokyo. in Japanese. Google Scholar

10.

K. Karino 1995. Male-male competition and female mate choice through courtship display in the territorial damselfish Stegastes nigricans. Ethology 100:126–138. Google Scholar

11.

S. B. M. Kraak, T. C. M. Bakker, and B. Mundwiler . 1999. Sexual selection in sticklebacks in the field: correlates of reproductive, mating, and paternal success. Behav Ecol 10:696–706. Google Scholar

12.

S. B. M. Kraak and T. G. G. Groothuis . 1994. Female preference for nests with eggs based on the presence of the eggs themselves. Behaviour 131:189–206. Google Scholar

13.

N. Mizuno, S. Uehara, and M. Maki . 1979. Study on a freshwater fish, Rhinogobius brunneus (Pisces: Gobiidae) IV. Habitat segregation among sympatric populations of 4 color types. Jpn J Ecol 29:137–147. in Japanese with English summary. Google Scholar

14.

A. Pomiankowski and Y. Iwasa . 1993. Evolution of multiple sexual preferences by Fisher's runaway process of sexual selection. P Roy Soc Lond B Bio 253:173–181. Google Scholar

15.

S. Rohwer 1978. Parent cannibalism of offspring and egg raiding as a courtship strategy. Am Nat 112:429–440. Google Scholar

16.

R. C. Sargent and M. R. Gross . 1993. Williams' principle: an explanation of parental care in teleost fishes. In “The Behaviour of Teleost Fishes 2nd ed”. Ed by T. J. Picher Chapman & Hall. London. pp. 333–361. Google Scholar

17.

D. Takahashi 2000a. Conventional sex role in an amphidromous Rhinogobius goby in which females exhibit nuptial coloration. Ichthyol Res 47:303–306. Google Scholar

18.

D. Takahashi 2000b. Studies on the reproductive ecology of a freshwater goby in the genus Rhinogobius. Ph.D thesis. Osaka City University. Osaka. Google Scholar

19.

D. Takahashi and M. Kohda . 2001. Females of a stream goby choose mates that court in fast water currents. Behaviour 138:937–946. Google Scholar

20.

D. Takahashi and Y. Yanagisawa . 1999. Breeding ecology of an amphidromous goby of the genus Rhinogobius. Ichthyol Res 46:185–191. Google Scholar

21.

D. Takahashi, M. Kohda, and Y. Yanagisawa . 2001. Male-male competition for large nests as a determinant of male mating success in a Japanese stream goby Rhinogobius sp. DA. Ichthyol Res 48:91–95. Google Scholar

22.

S. Thompson 1986. Male spawning success and female mate choice in the mottled sculpin, Forsterygion varium (Pisces: Trip-terygiidae). Anim Behav 34:580–589. Google Scholar

23.

L. M. Unger and R. C. Sargent . 1988. Allopaternal care in the fathead minnow, Pimephales promelas: females prefer males with eggs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 23:27–32. Google Scholar

24.

F. Widemo and S. A. Sæther . 1999. Beauty is in the eye of the beholder: causes and consequences of variation in mating preferences. Trends Ecol Evol 14:26–31. Google Scholar
Daisuke Takahashi and Masanori Kohda "Female Preference for Nest Size in the Stream Goby Rhinogobius sp. DA," Zoological Science 19(11), 1241-1244, (1 November 2002). https://doi.org/10.2108/zsj.19.1241
Received: 15 May 2002; Accepted: 1 September 2002; Published: 1 November 2002
KEYWORDS
female choice
multiple criteria
nest size
paternal care
Rhinogobius
Back to Top