Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2005 Alternative Male Mating Behaviors Dependent on Relative Body Size in Captive Oval Squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana (Cephalopoda, Loliginidae)
Toshifumi Wada, Takeshi Takegaki, Tohru Mori, Yutaka Natsukari
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

We observed the reproductive behavior of the oval squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana in captivity. The male used three different mating behaviors: male-parallel (MP), male-upturned (MU) and sneaking. Male competition over females frequently occurred before and during the female egg-laying period, and the outcome of most fights depended on male body size. Larger males guarded their partners from other males and performed MP mating during the egg-laying period of the paired females. In contrast, there was no pairing and mate guarding in MU mating and sneaking, which were adopted by smaller subordinate males as alternative tactics outside female egg-laying period and during the period, respectively. MP matings were 95% successful, but more than half of MU matings were unsuccessful. Higher mating success in MP mating was achieved through pairing, whereas males in MU mating were less successful because mating attempts without pair formation were often foiled by escape of the female. Sneaking was successful in all cases but occurred less frequently. Spermatophores were attached at the opening of the oviduct in MP mating, whereas they were attached around the female buccal membrane in MU mating and sneaking. Considering the route of egg transportation, higher fertilization success can be expected in MP mating because of the advantageous location of the attached spermatophores. Our results suggest that MP mating is used by larger, paired males during the female egg-laying period, and that MU mating and sneaking are alternative tactics adopted by smaller, subordinate males. These alternative mating behaviors would be conditional strategy dependent on relative body size, because some individual males displayed both MP and MU mating behaviors.

INTRODUCTION

Alternative male mating behaviors can arise during competition for mates if individual males differ in their competitive abilities, which depend mainly on body size (Gross, 1996). Larger males gain access to more partners due to large size advantage in male-male competition with size-difference, whereas smaller subordinate males often adopt strikingly different and non-competitive behavioral tactics (Alcock, 1979; Dominey, 1984). In several animal taxa such as birds, mammals, reptiles, and insects (e.g., Andersson, 1994; Hanks et al., 1996; Shine et al., 2000), larger body size in males has been reported to increase their mating opportunities and enhance mating success. However, it is difficult to quantify the relationship between male body size and mating success, particularly in many kinds of aquatic organisms. Here we report our empirical and experimental results on how body size in male squid affects their mating success through alternative mating tactics.

In cephalopods both sexes appear to mate promiscuously (Baur, 1998), and, especially in some loliginid squids, complex reproductive behavior have been reported (Hanlon and Messenger, 1996). Larger males form pairs with females and mate with the partners (Sauer et al., 1997; Hanlon et al., 1997; Hanlon et al., 2002), whereas sneaking copulations by smaller males are also known to occur in several Loligo species (Hanlon and Messenger, 1996; Sauer et al., 1997). Moreover, such multiple mating behaviors involve different places of attachment of spermatophores on the female's body (Drew, 1911; Hanlon and Messenger, 1996). In particular, four mating types have recently been reported in Sepioteuthis australis: “male-upturned mating”, “sneaker mating”, “male-parallel mating”, and “head-to-head mating” (Jantzen and Havenhand, 2002, 2003). However, it is unclear how each mating type is chosen and what the advantages of each are. Moreover, the underlying factors leading to multiple male mating behaviors are still unknown.

We investigated the reproductive behavior of Sepioteuthis lessoniana in a large tank. The aim of this study is firstly to describe the female egg-laying behavior and the multiple mating sequence, and then to clarify male mating tactics by comparing their body size, mating success, and places of attachment of spermatophores on the female's body. These comparisons will allow us to discuss how S. lessoniana males adopt alternative mating tactics in regard to intra-sexual selection.

Study species

The oval squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana is widely distributed throughout the Indo-Pacific area (Okutani, 1995) and is one of the important neritic fisheries resources, especially in southern Japan. In Japanese coastal waters, this species has been divided into three types on the basis of their morphological and genetic differences: AKAIKA, SHIROIKA and KUAIKA type (Izuka et al., 1994, 1996a, 1996b). Moreover, some different characteristics in reproduction between each group have been reported, such as spawning substrate use and number of eggs contained a single egg capsule (Segawa et al., 1993a, b). Although three mating positions have also been known in this species: “male-parallel” (Segawa, 1987), “head-to-head” (Lee et al., 1994), and “male-upturned” (Boal and Gonzalez, 1998), it has not been demonstrated whether such a difference in mating position corresponds to the difference in species type.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study animals

Adult Sepioteuthis lessoniana (n=201) which is belonging to SHIROIKA type widely distributed around southern Japan (Izuka et al., 1996b) were captured alive by small set nets off the coast near Tsuyazaki (33°47′N, 130°26′E) and Nata (33 °41′N, 130°24′E) in Fukuoka Prefecture, Kyushu, Japan, between 22 April and 1 August 1999. The nets were placed about 20–300 m offshore at 10–30 m deep. The living animals were transported by truck to a columnar reserve tank (3.5 m diameter, 1.8 m deep) in the Marine World Umi-nonakamichi Aquarium at Fukuoka City. All animals were temporarily kept in the reserve tank to habituate to housing conditions.

Animals used for observations were transferred to an observation tank (3.2×6.6×1.8 m depth, about 38000 L) with an acrylic viewing window (1.5×5.2 m). Nine to 32 individuals (17 average) were kept in the observation tank at any one time, and a total of 94 individuals (male, mean mantle length±SD=31.4±4.3 cm, range=22.7–43.2 cm, n=46; female, 25.5±3.0 cm, 19.5–30.5 cm, n=48) were observed over the period from 29 April to 12 August. Males were significantly larger than females (two-sample t-test, t=6.68, P< 0.001). To avoid handling stress, mantle length was measured after death. When the number of squid reduced due to the mortality, new squid were added to the observation tank from the reserve tank (mean 10 individuals at a time). Animals in both the reserve and observation tanks were fed either living or thawed fish and shrimp daily (17:00–18:00 h) in sufficient quantities.

Animal husbandry conditions

The observation tank was supplied with natural seawater (1680 L/h), and an artificial rock (about 1 m high) was set on the sandy bottom. Sea algae, Sargassum ringgoldianum and Sargassum ful-vellum, were fixed on the sandy bottom as a spawning substrate, because egg masses of S. lessoniana are attached to substrates under natural conditions (T. Wada, unpublished data). Water temperature was kept between 17°C and 20°C. Water quality (i.e., pH, ammonia, nitrite, and nitrate) was measured once every 2 weeks from May to August. Their values (pH: mean±SD=7.79±0.09; NH4- N: 0.01±0.01 ppm; NO2-N: .005±0.002 ppm; NO3-N: 1.0±0.1 ppm) were within the ranges reported previously for culture conditions through multiple generations in this species (Lee et al., 1994; Walsh et al., 2002). Artificial light (3200lux at the surface of the observation tank) was normally provided from 08:30–21:00 h, but the lighting period was sometimes extended when needed for observation: e.g., until the end of continuous egg-laying behavior. Squid's reproductive behavior continued during the extended lighting condition as well as the low lighting condition (only aisle lights in the aquarium).

Observation and recording

Behavioral observation and recording were made primarily between 17:30 and 21:00 h through the window of the observation tank, but sometimes the period was extended to obtain the sequence data of squid's reproductive behavior. In addition, to examine the frequency and diurnal periodicity of female egg-laying, we also recorded the start time of the egg-laying behavior during the other period (21:00–17:30 h). Reproductive behavior was recorded with two digital video cameras, models DCR-VX1000 and CCD-TR1000-NTSC (Sony, Tokyo, Japan). The former was hand-held to record each phase of the mating sequence in detail using the zoom function. This recording was conducted using “ad libitum sampling” (cf., Martin and Bateson, 1993) for the duration of observation almost every day. The latter was mounted on a tripod to record continuous series of reproductive behaviors. In the latter recording, we used a wide-angle setting to focus on the spawning substrate and conducted “focal animal sampling” on spawning pairs to acquire the sequence of egg-laying behaviors and mating intervals. Overall, we obtained 9.5 h of videotape recordings by the former method and 45.6 h with the latter. All recordings and post-video analyses were carried out by the same observer.

Analysis of the recordings and anatomy

Video recordings were analyzed on a high-resolution monitor (PVM-14M4J, Sony). Time data were referenced to the internal clocks on the video cameras; the clocks were set before every recording. Mating behavior was analyzed at 30 frames per second. To compare the relative body sizes of mated male and female (significance tested by a chi-squared test) we examined suitable stationary frames. In these comparisons, we used only data from easily discernable cases. Stationary frames were also used to compare the body sizes of fighting males. Sexes were discriminated by examining the stripe patterns of the dorsal mantle (Ikeda, 1933) and by the squid's behavior. To analyze the mating position performed by each male and the number of egg capsules produced by each female, some males and females were individually identified by their external characteristics (e.g., unique scars on the posterior end of the mantle or defects of the arm or fin). We defined mating success as successful spermatophore transfer to the female during the mating. To confirm the places of attachment of spermatophores, we dissected 62 females that had died in the reserve and observation tanks. The sperm masses found on the dead females were regard as those transferred by the males in this captivity, because the attached sperm masses fall out of female buccal membrane after several weeks (Wada and Kobayashi, 1995).

RESULTS

Reproductive behavior of Sepioteuthis lessoniana

Female egg-laying behavior occurred from 16 May to 4 August, whereas mating behavior was observed on every day of the study period. The female laid an egg capsule on the spawning substrate at average intervals of 4.2 min (SD=1.3 min, n=1427 by 14 females) and produced 24–159 egg capsules (mean±SD=75.1±42.7) in succession during a single egg-laying bout (mean observation time / individual±SD =5.1±3.1 h, n=19 egg-laying bouts by 14 females; the overall duration was defined as a female egg-laying period). The egg-laying bout was repeated several times during the study period (maximum of four times per female). The female usually started laying eggs in the morning (07:00–09:00 h, 21% of 19 egg-laying bouts) or evening (18:00–22:00 h, 52%).

Both males and females exhibited promiscuous mating behavior by mating with more than one partner. Three different matings were observed (Table 1): male-parallel mating (MP mating; Fig. 1a), male-upturned mating (MU mating; Fig. 1b) and sneaking. At least three individual males showed both MP and MU mating positions. The behavioral process of each mating is described below.

Table 1.

Differences between the three mating behaviors

i0289-0003-22-6-645-t01.gif

Fig. 1.

Behavioral sequences of two matings in Sepioteuthis lessoniana: a; male-parallel mating, b; male-upturned mating. For details see text. *R=Right, L=Left, Sp.=Spermatophores, I–IV=Arm number

i0289-0003-22-6-645-f01.gif

Male-parallel mating (MP): MP mating was observed 174 times and was performed by distinct pairs. The pair swam side by side and the male guarded its partner against other males (which was defined as a pairing behavior). This mating occurred only during egg-laying period by the paired female. Paired males repeated the MP mating at 29.5 min intervals on average (SD=39.8 min, range=0.8–324.5 min, n=155). Males succeeded in attaching spermatophores to females in 166 MP matings (95%). The successful MP mating sequence was as follows.

First, the paired male usually raised up either his right arm I or both arms I vertically (91% of 174 matings), and swung the arm(s) back and forth beside the female (Fig. 1a-1). Second, the male moved toward the ventral side of the female and touched her neck or the anterior part of the mantle with his right arm IV (Fig. 1a-2). Third, his right arm IV (non-hectocotylized) was inserted into the female's mantle cavity through the left part of the opening (Fig. 1a-3). Then, several spermatophores ejected through the funnel were held with the modified part of the hectocotylized left arm IV (Fig. 1a-4). The left arm IV was also inserted into the female's mantle cavity alongside the right arm IV (Fig. 1a-5). After a short time (mean insertion time±SD=4.8±1.2 s; uninterrupted sequence data were available at 116 MP matings; Fig. 2), the male released the spermatophores and withdrew both arms IV. The time spent for a MP mating, which starts from male's approach to a female and ends at the release of the female, was 15.0 s on average (SD=4.4 s, range=6.5–29.1 s, n=102; from only uninterrupted sequence data). Immediately after mating, the female often ejected only the outer sheaths of the spermatophores through her funnel. Males transferred spermatophores only once during each MP mating, except in one case in which they were transferred twice (0.6% of 166 successful matings). Sperm masses were confirmed on only the opening of the oviduct within the mantle cavity of female, and those that still had the outer sheaths of the spermatophores were found in two individuals among all dissected females (Fig. 3).

Fig. 2.

Larger male inserting left arm IV (hectocotylus) into female's mantle cavity through the left part of the opening (MP mating).

i0289-0003-22-6-645-f02.jpg

Fig. 3.

Sperm masses with the outer sheaths of spermatophores attached on opening of the oviduct. *a=Sperm masses, b=Opening of oviduct, c=Outer sheaths of spermatophores

i0289-0003-22-6-645-f03.jpg

Male-upturned mating (MU): MU mating was observed 85 times. Neither pairing nor male mate guarding was observed in this pattern. MU mating occurred outside the egg-laying period of the female. Males succeeded in attaching spermatophores to females in 37 MU matings (44%). The successful MU mating sequence was as follows.

First, the male abruptly approached the female from any directions and stayed behind her (Fig. 1b-1). Second, the male flipped upside-down (Fig. 1b-2). Before flipping, the male often shook his body minutely. Third, the male touched the dorsal mantle or head of the female while maintaining an upside-down position (Fig. 1b-3). Simultaneously, the male ejected several spermatophores through his funnel and held them with his left arm IV, while the right arm IV was inserted into the female's buccal cavity. Then, the male inserted the spermatophores with his hectocotylized left arm IV into the female's buccal cavity alongside the right arm IV, while the right and left arms I and II held the female's body (Fig. 1b-4). Finally, the right arm IV was withdrawn from the female's buccal cavity, leaving only the left arm IV inside (mean insertion time±SD=3.1±1.3 s; uninterrupted sequence data were available at 28 MU matings) (Fig. 1b-5). The hectocotylized left arm IV was bent at the end holding the spermatophores (Fig. 4). The mated male and female were released each other or the female swam backwards at the end of mating. No spermatophore was observed in the withdrawn left arm IV. The time spent for a MU mating (from male's approach to the release of the female), was 8.4 s on average (SD=2.1 s, range=5.7–12.3 s, n=13; from only uninterrupted sequence data). Males transferred spermatophores only once during each MU mating. Many sperm masses were confirmed around the buccal membrane in all dead females.

Fig. 4.

Smaller male inserting left arm IV (hectocotylus) into female's buccal cavity (MU mating).

i0289-0003-22-6-645-f04.jpg

Sneaking: Sneaking was observed 7 times. Although the mating position and attachment place of spermatophores in sneaking were the same as those observed in MU mating (see above and Fig. 1b), the sneaking males mated with the females pairing with larger males during the female egg-laying period. Sneaking was successful in attaching spermatophores to females in all 7 cases (100%).

Male–male competition and mating success

Male competition over females was observed particularly during and immediately before the female egg-laying period. Fighting males often extended their arms (especially arms III) and changed their body color from white to dark brown. The displays determined the outcome of fights in the most contests: Relatively larger males were more likely to win the competition. Even when sometimes grasping each other, larger males usually chased away and defeated smaller ones (93% of 27 fights). The winner paired with the female and maintained the partner, resulting in that male body size was significantly larger in MP than in MU mating and sneaking (χ2=51.8, P<0.001). Mated males were larger than their partners in 74% of 149 MP matings, but in only 21% of 66 MU mating and sneaking (Table 2).

Table 2.

Relative size-differences between males and females at MP (n=149), MU and sneaker matings (n=66)

i0289-0003-22-6-645-t02.gif

Mating success rate was higher in MP (95%) than in MU matings (44%) (χ2=90.6, P<0.001). MP matings were 100% successful (157 cases) when the male started just when the female transferred the egg from the mantle cavity to her arms through her funnel, but were less successful in other cases (53% of 17 cases) because the females escaped from the mating attempts of the males. Unsuccessful MU matings (n=40, excluding eight indiscernible cases) could be attributed to the escape of females during these trials (60%) or interruption by the males themselves (40%).

DISCUSSION

Male Sepioteuthis lessoniana belonging to SHIROIKA type used three different matings dependent on the relative body size: male-parallel (MP) mating by larger, paired males, and male-upturned (MU) mating and sneaking by smaller, unpaired males. MP mating and sneaking occurred during the female egg-laying period, whereas MU mating outside the period.

MP mating is common in loliginid squids (Table 6.2 in Hanlon and Messenger, 1996), whereas MU mating seems to be quite unusual but is the same pattern described by Boal and Gonzalez (1998) and Ueta et al. (2002) in this species and by Jantzen and Havenhand (2002) in the related species Sepioteuthis australis. Sneaking of S. lessoniana occurred during the female egg-laying period with a male-upturned (MU) position, as has been reported for S. australis (Jantzen and Havenhand, 2003). Moreover, the final phase of MU mating (32% of 37) was similar to the head-to-head mating position reported in Loligo spp. (Drew, 1911; Hanlon et al., 2002; T. Wada, unpublished data), which occurred when the female swam backwards at the end of mating and pulled the male. The head-to-head mating position should be regard as a variation of MU mating in this species.

The mating success rate in male S. lessoniana was significantly higher in MP than in MU mating. Male mating success depended on the timing of occurrence, and so higher success in MP mating seemed to be achieved through pairing. Males in MP mating that paired before mating started the mating mostly during egg transportation of paired female through her funnel, and fully succeeded in attaching spermatophores. That is, mating timing determines mating success because the female cannot supply jet propulsion from her funnel for the escape. In contrast, males in MU mating were less successful, because mating attempts without pair formation were often avoided owing to escape of the female. Thus, pairing before mating is indispensable for high mating success of males. MP mating was adopted by larger males because they won against smaller males in the competition to pair with females. The outcome of most fights was correlated with relative body size. Although relative size advantage in aggressive contests over females has been reported in other loliginid squids, L. pealei and L. plei (Arnold, 1962; Moynihan and Rodaniche, 1982; DiMarco and Hanlon, 1997), the effect of body size on male mating success was not mentioned in these papers. Our data are therefore the first to confirm that large body size in male loliginid squid is an important factor leading to high mating success. The sexual size dimorphism in some loliginid species might have evolved partly through intra-sexual selection (i.e., male–male competition). Moreover, small unpaired males of S. lessoniana sometimes adopted sneaking tactics and thereby fully succeeded in attaching spermatophores to females. Although it was not clear why sneaking occurred less frequently (n=7), sneaking males were often chased away by larger paired males after the spermatophore transfer. There may be higher costs and risks for sneaking than for MP and MU matings.

In addition to high mating success, higher fertilization success is expected in MP mating because of the advantageous location of the attached spermatophores and repeated matings during the egg-laying period of the paired female. With MP mating, sperm attached on the opening of the oviduct would come into contact with unfertilized eggs earlier than those attached around the buccal membrane by MU mating and sneaking, because all eggs first pass through the oviduct after ovulation (illustrated in Fig. 5). Analyses of dead females confirmed the presence of sperm masses around the buccal membrane in all of these females, whereas sperm masses remained at the opening of the oviduct in only two dead females. Because males inserted the hectocotylized left arm IV holding the spermatophores into the female's mantle cavity during each MP mating (Fig. 2), spermatophores should have been deposited on the opening of the oviduct in only MP mating. It is unclear at present why sperm masses were not found on the opening of the oviduct in the other dead females. Moreover, repeated MP matings (mean=29.5 min interval) by the paired male during the female egg-laying period would increase the fertilization opportunities. Thus, the sperm in MP mating may have a higher probability of fertilizing more eggs than those in MU mating and sneaking.

Fig. 5.

Places of attachment of sperm attached by three different matings, and estimated route of egg transportation. In our hypothesis on the fertilization mechanism, sperm at the opening of the oviduct from MP mating would have a positional advantage for fertilization.

i0289-0003-22-6-645-f05.gif

When are sperm that were attached around the female buccal membrane by MU mating and sneaking used for the fertilization? Wada and Kobayashi (1995) have reported for S. lessoniana that the females continuously produced fertilized eggs at least during 20 days after isolation from males. In such cases, the eggs were most likely fertilized from sperm stored in the seminal receptacle at the ventral portion of female buccal membrane because there is no sperm storage organ around the opening of oviduct (i.e., for MP mating). Although the mechanism underlying fertilization including cryptic female choice (i.e., sperm selection; Eberhard, 1996) in S. lessoniana is still unknown, the female can store up and release active sperm for nearly one month without mating (Wada and Kobayashi, 1995). Therefore, the female may make effective use of sperm stored from MU mating and sneaking for the fertilization when sperm at the opening of oviduct are not present.

The present study found that MP mating is used by larger, paired males during the female egg-laying period, whereas MU mating and sneaking are adopted by smaller, subordinate males as alternative mating tactics. These alternative mating behaviors would be conditional strategy dependent on relative body size, because some individual males showed both MP and MU matings. The larger males have a mating advantage leading to higher mating success (i.e., pairing), and sperm attached in MP mating may have a greater chance of fertilizing more eggs than those in MU mating and sneaking. Therefore, though there is no direct evidence, we believe that males in MP mating gain more paternity than those in MU mating and sneaking. However, a high percentage of fertilization by small sneaker or satellite males has been reported in the Australian giant cuttlefish Sepia apama (Naud et al., 2004) and in other animal groups (e.g., Brockmann et al., 2000; Fu et al., 2001). In the future studies it will be necessary to confirm the paternity of each of the offspring from alternative male mating tactics by molecular biological analysis using microsatellite DNA markers (e.g., Shaw and Boyle, 1997; Buresch et al., 2001; Shaw and Sauer, 2004).

Acknowledgments

We thank S. Wakisaka, K. Takada, H. Hiruda, and the staff of Marine World Uminonakamichi, who gave us the chance to perform this study and assisted us greatly in the capture and maintenance of live squid. We are also grateful to C. G. Satuito for reviewing our initial manuscript, and R. T. Hanlon, Y. Ueta, and two anonymous reviewers for critical comments and helpful suggestions.

REFERENCES

1.

J. Alcock 1979. The evolution of intraspecific diversity in male reproductive strategies in some bees and wasps. In “Sexual selection and reproductive competition in insects”. Ed by M. S. Blum and N. A. Blum , editors. Academic Press. New York. pp. 381–402. Google Scholar

2.

M. Andersson 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton University Press. Princeton. Google Scholar

3.

J. M. Arnold 1962. Mating behavior and social structure in Loligo pealeii. Biol Bull 123:53–57. Google Scholar

4.

B. Baur 1998. Sperm competition in molluscs. In “Sperm competition and sexual selection”. Ed by T. R. Birkhead and A. P. Møller , editors. Academic Press. London. pp. 255–305. Google Scholar

5.

J. G. Boal and S. A. Gonzalez . 1998. Social behaviour of individual oval squids (Cephalopoda, Teuthoidea, Loliginidae, Sepioteuthis lessoniana) within a captive school. Ethology 104:161–178. Google Scholar

6.

H. J. Brockmann, C. Nguyen, and W. Potts . 2000. Paternity in horseshoe crabs when spawning in multiple-male groups. Anim Behav 60:837–849. Google Scholar

7.

K. M. Buresch, R. T. Hanlon, M. R. Maxwell, and S. Ring . 2001. Microsatellite DNA markers indicate a high frequency of multiple paternity within individual field-collected egg capsules of the squid Loligo pealeii. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 210:161–165. Google Scholar

8.

F. P. DiMarco and R. T. Hanlon . 1997. Agonistic behavior in the squid Loligo plei (Loliginidae, Teuthoidea): fighting tactics and the effects of size and resource value. Ethology 103:89–108. Google Scholar

9.

W. J. Dominey 1984. Alternative mating tactics and evolutionarily stable strategies. Am Zool 24:385–396. Google Scholar

10.

G. A. Drew 1911. Sexual activities of the squid, Loligo pealii. J Morphol 22:327–359. Google Scholar

11.

W. G. Eberhard 1996. Female control: sexual selection by cryptic female choice. Princeton University Press. Princeton. Google Scholar

12.

P. Fu, B. D. Neff, and M. R. Gross . 2001. Tactic-specific success in sperm competition. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:1105–1112. Google Scholar

13.

M. R. Gross 1996. Alternative reproductive strategies and tactics: diversity within sexes. Trends Ecol Evol 11:92–98. Google Scholar

14.

L. M. Hanks, J. G. Millar, and T. D. Paine . 1996. Body size influences mating success of the eucalyptus longhorned borer (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae). J Insect Behav 9:369–382. Google Scholar

15.

R. T. Hanlon, M. R. Maxwell, and N. Shashar . 1997. Behavioural dynamics that would lead to multiple paternity within egg capsules of the squid Loligo pealei. Biol Bull 193:212–214. Google Scholar

16.

R. T. Hanlon and J. B. Messenger . 1996. Cephalopod behaviour. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge. Google Scholar

17.

R. T. Hanlon, M. J. Smale, and W. H. H. Sauer . 2002. The mating system of the squid Loligo vulgaris reynaudii (Cephalopoda: Mollusca) off South Africa: fighting, guarding, sneaking, mating and egg laying behavior. Bull Mar Sci 71:331–345. Google Scholar

18.

H. Ikeda 1933. Sex-correlated markings in Sepioteuthis lessoniana Ferussac. Venus Jpn J Malacol 3:324–329. (in Japanese with English resume). Google Scholar

19.

T. Izuka, S. Segawa, T. Okutani, and K. Numachi . 1994. Evidence on the existence of three species in the oval squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana complex in Ishigaki Island, Okinawa, Southwestern Japan, by isozyme analyses. Venus Jpn J Malacol 53:217–228. Google Scholar

20.

T. Izuka, S. Segawa, and T. Okutani . 1996a. Identification of three species in oval squid, Sepioteuthis lessoniana complex by chromatophore arrangements on the funnel. Venus Jpn J Malacol 55:139–142. Google Scholar

21.

T. Izuka, S. Segawa, and T. Okutani . 1996b. Biochemical study of the population heterogeneity and distribution of the oval squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana complex in southwestern Japan. Am Malacol Bull 12:129–135. Google Scholar

22.

T. M. Jantzen and J. N. Havenhand . 2002. Preliminary field observations of mating and spawning in the squid Sepioteuthis australis. Bull Mar Sci 71:1073–1080. Google Scholar

23.

T. M. Jantzen and J. N. Havenhand . 2003. Reproductive behavior in the squid Sepioteuthis australis from South Australia: Interactions on the spawning grounds. Biol Bull 204:305–317. Google Scholar

24.

P. G. Lee, P. E. Turk, W. T. Yang, and R. T. Hanlon . 1994. Biological characteristics and biomedical applications of the squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana cultured through multiple generations. Biol Bull 186:328–341. Google Scholar

25.

P. Martin and P. Bateson . 1993. Measuring behaviour: An introductory guide. edn 2Cambridge University Press. London. Google Scholar

26.

M. Moynihan and A. F. Rodaniche . 1982. The behavior and natural history of the Caribbean reef squid Sepioteuthis sepioidea. With a consideration of social, signal, and defensive patterns for difficult and dangerous environments. Adv Ethol 25:1–151. Google Scholar

27.

M. J. Naud, R. T. Hanlon, K. C. Hall, P. W. Shaw, and J. N. Havenhand . 2004. Behavioural and genetic assessment of reproductive success in a spawning aggregation of the Australian giant cuttlefish, Sepia apama. Anim Behav 67:1043–1050. Google Scholar

28.

T. Okutani 1995. Cuttlefish and squids of the world in color. National Cooperative Association of Squid Processors. Tokyo. Google Scholar

29.

W. H. H. Sauer, M. J. Roberts, M. R. Lipinski, M. J. Smale, R. T. Hanlon, D. M. Webber, and R. K. O'Dor . 1997. Choreography of the squid's “Nuptial Dance”. Biol Bull 192:203–207. Google Scholar

30.

S. Segawa 1987. Life history of the oval squid, Sepioteuthis lessoniana in Kominato and adjacent waters central Honshu, Japan. J Tokyo Univ Fish 74:67–105. Google Scholar

31.

S. Segawa, S. Hirayama, and T. Okutani . 1993a. Is Sepioteuthis lessoniana in Okinawa a single species? In “Recent Advances in Cephalopod Fisheries Biology”. Ed by T. Okutani, R. K. O'Dor, and T. Kubodera , editors. Tokai University Press. Tokyo. pp. 513–521. Google Scholar

32.

S. Segawa, T. Izuka, T. Tamashiro, and T. Okutani . 1993b. A note on mating and egg deposition by Sepioteuthis lessoniana in Ishigaki Island, Okinawa, Southwestern Japan. Venus Jpn J Malacol 52:101–108. Google Scholar

33.

P. W. Shaw and P. R. Boyle . 1997. Multiple paternity within the brood of single females of Loligo forbesi (Cephalopoda: Loliginidae), demonstrated with microsatellite DNA markers. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 160:279–282. Google Scholar

34.

P. W. Shaw and W. H. H. Sauer . 2004. Multiple paternity and complex fertilisation dynamics in the squid Loligo vulgaris reynaudii. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 270:173–179. Google Scholar

35.

R. Shine, M. M. Olsson, I. T. Moore, M. P. Lemaster, M. Greene, and R. T. Mason . 2000. Body size enhances mating success in male garter snakes. Anim Behav 59:F4–F11. Google Scholar

36.

Y. Ueta, T. Takagi, A. Hirosawa, A. Namimatsu, S. Goto, and T. Toshikawa . 2002. Preliminary observations on the mating and spawning behavior of oval squid (Sepioteuthis lessoniana) at a man-made spawning bed settled in the coastal waters of Tokushima Prefecture. Bull Tokushima Pref Fish Res Ins 1:33–36. (in Japanese with English abstract). Google Scholar

37.

Y. Wada and T. Kobayashi . 1995. On an iteroparity of the oval squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana. Nippon Suisan Gakkaishi 61:151–158. (in Japanese with English abstract). Google Scholar

38.

L. S. Walsh, P. E. Turk, J. W. Forsythe, and P. G. Lee . 2002. Mariculture of the loliginid squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana through seven successive generations. Aquaculture 212:245–262. Google Scholar
Toshifumi Wada, Takeshi Takegaki, Tohru Mori, and Yutaka Natsukari "Alternative Male Mating Behaviors Dependent on Relative Body Size in Captive Oval Squid Sepioteuthis lessoniana (Cephalopoda, Loliginidae)," Zoological Science 22(6), 645-651, (1 June 2005). https://doi.org/10.2108/zsj.22.645
Received: 15 November 2004; Accepted: 1 April 2005; Published: 1 June 2005
KEYWORDS
Alternative mating tactics
mating success
pairing
Sepioteuthis lessoniana
sneaking
sperm competition
Back to Top