Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 March 2010 Spring Migration of Ruffs Philomachus pugnax in Fryslân: Estimates of Staging Duration Using Resighting Data
Yvonne I. Verkuil, Jan J. Wijmenga, Jos C.E.W. Hooijmeijer, Theunis Piersma
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Seasonal bird migration involves long flights, but most time is actually spent at intermediate staging areas. The duration of stay at these sites can be evaluated with mark—recapture methods that employ day-to-day local encounters of individually marked birds. Estimates of staging duration are based on two probabilities: the immigration probability, the complement of a bird's seniority to an area, and the emigration probability, the complement of the staying probability. Estimating total staging duration from seniority and staying probabilities requires validation for resighting data and here we compare three data categories of Ruffs Philomachus pugnax passing through The Netherlands during northward migration: (1) newly colour-ringed, (2) previously colour-ringed and (3) radio-tagged Ruffs (recorded by automated receiving stations). Between 2004 and 2008, 4363 resighting histories and 95 telemetry recording histories were collected. As sample sizes for females were low, only data for males were analysed. Possible catching effects affecting estimates of staging duration were explored. Staying probability was estimated for all data. Seniority however, could not be estimated for newly marked Ruffs; the assumption of equal ‘capture’ probability for reverse-time models applied to estimate seniority is violated for seasonal resighting histories starting with a catching event. Therefore, estimates of total staging duration were based on resightings of previously colour-marked birds only. For radio-tagged birds a minimal staging duration (time between tagging and last recording) was calculated. Modelling indicated that newly colour-ringed birds had a higher staying probability than previously colour-ringed birds, but the difference translated to a prolonged staging duration in newly ringed birds of only 0.4–0.5 d, suggesting a very small catching effect. The minimal staging duration of radio-tagged birds validated estimates of staging duration for colour-ringed birds in 2007 but not in 2005. In 2005 a low resighting probability resulted in underestimates of staging duration. We conclude that (1) estimates of staying probability can be affected by catching although effects on staging duration might be small, and that (2) low resighting probabilities can lead to underestimates in staging duration. In our study previously ringed Ruffs resighted in 2006–08 yielded reliable estimates of staging duration as data had sufficiently high resighting probabilities. Average staging durations varied between 19 d in 2008 and 23 d in 2006.

During bird migration, periods of rest and refuelling are punctuated by flight episodes (Piersma 1987). Therefore, the total duration of migration is not so much determined by the flight time as by the time in preparation of these flights, the staging duration (Hedenström & Alerstam 1997, Hedenström 2008). The use of colour-rings and radio telemetry, and the development of statistical tools, have enabled better estimations of staging duration and turnover at (re-)fuelling sites (Kersten & Smit 1984, Nebel et al. 2000, Schaub & Jenni 2001, Schaub et al. 2001, Battley et al. 2004, Ydenberg et al. 2004, Rice et al. 2007, Salewski et al. 2007, Bächler & Schaub 2007). At a staging site there is a constant flux of birds entering the site and birds that leave (Schaub et al. 2001). This means that counts cannot be used directly to infer population size and turnover. When individual staging duration is taken into account, however, numbers using a site can be estimated (Bishop et al. 2000, Frederiksen et al. 2001, Ydenberg et al. 2004, Lee et al. 2007).

From 2004–08 we studied the staging ecology of Ruffs Philomachus pugnax in Fryslân, The Netherlands (Jukema & Piersma 2000, Jukema et al. 2001b, Verkuil & de Goeij 2003). With 1000s of birds observed, this area ranks amongst the larger known staging areas of Ruffs in Europe (Wymenga 1999). The Ruff is a gregarious, strongly sexually dimorphic shorebird whose reproductive behaviour has been well-studied (Hogan-Warburg 1966, van Rhijn 1991, Lank et al. 1995, Widemo 1997, Lank & Dale 2001, Jukema & Piersma 2006), but relatively little is known about migration behaviour and demography (but see OAG Münster 1989, OAG Münster 1992, Melter & Bergmann 1996, Jukema & Piersma 2000). Ruffs breed in temperate to arctic areas ranging from Western Europe to Eastern Siberia (Zöckler 2002). Although most birds winter in the Sahel and the savannahs of eastern Africa and in India (Zwarts et al. 2009), a small proportion of males remain in NW Europe during winter (OAG Münster 1996).

Table 1.

Overview of possible estimations (+) of staging duration in Yeari for the three data categories collected at staging location for Ruffs in The Netherlands. Notation is according Schaub et al. (2001): ‘α’ is staying probability as used to estimate duration of stay after encounter ; ‘γ’ is seniority probability as used to estimate duration of stay before encounter’; staging duration, the total time present at given time interval, is the sum of duration of stay before encounter and duration of stay after encounter.

t01_21.gif

In this paper we use three separate data categories to estimate staging duration: (1) newly colour-ringed Ruffs, (2) previously colour-ringed Ruffs returning to the area, and (3) telemetry data of newly radio-tagged Ruffs (Table 1). Based on our exploration of these categories, we present a design for using capture—resighting statistics to estimate staging duration in colour-ringed birds with long stopovers. Following Warnock (2009) we choose to use the term staging duration rather than stopover duration to emphasize that the period is much larger than the few days stopovers in songbirds. Estimates for staging duration were obtained by adding up the staying time, the time that a bird would remain in the study area after Timei;, and the seniority, the estimated time the bird was already present before Timei (Schaub et al. 2001, Schaub et al. 2004). Staying and seniority probabilities (their complements are the emigration and immigration probabilities) are estimated using survival analysis (Schaub et al. 2001). Birds are caught and colour-ringed, and estimates of staging duration are made based on local resightings within the same season (Kersten & Smit 1984, Ydenberg et al. 2004). Yet, seniority, which is part of staging duration, can only be calculated using individuals marked in previous years, because the assumption of equal ‘recapture’ probability for reverse-time modelling is violated when the encounter history starts with a catching event (Table 1). In our study system, we individually colour-marked more than 4500 individual Ruffs over five successive spring seasons, many of which returned the following years. The first encounter of returning marked birds in the new migration season is not a catch but a resighting. We were also able to compare data of returning colour-ringed Ruffs (marked in Yeari-1) with newly colour-ringed Ruffs observed in Yeari of colour-ringing, which allowed us to investigate the extent and strength of a possible catching effect on the staying probability.

The capture—resighting models estimate the resighting probability that should be independent of marking technique (Salewski et al. 2007, Bächler & Schaub 2007). However, the resighting probability in colour-ring studies can be very low. This problem can be overcome with radio-tags, which give much higher reporting probabilities. Radio-telemetry, however, is costly and potentially more invasive to the birds. It certainly does not allow for large sample sizes to be accumulated over years. We applied radio-tags to a small subset of male Ruffs to verify the accuracy of estimates from colour-ring resighting data.

METHODS

Study area and data collection

This study was carried out at the main western European spring staging area (Wymenga 1999). During spring and autumn migration, Ruffs use the south-western part of the province of Fryslân, The Netherlands, to moult and refuel (Jukema et al. 1995, Verkuil & de Goeij 2003). The study area consists of grasslands intensely managed for dairy farming, and the borders of Lake IJsselmeer. The area covers c. 400 km2 (Fig. 1). During 2004–08 staging Ruffs were studied over the entire passage periods, from the first week of March until late May. Catches were made on all days except Sundays with traditional ‘wilsternets’, a c. 20 m long and 3 m high clap net. The net was laid out on grassland used by foraging Ruffs and the wilsternetters used a flock of stuffed Ruffs as decoys to lure the birds near the net (Jukema et al. 2001a, Piersma et al. 2005). Each captured Ruff was marked with a unique combination of four colour rings and a coloured ‘flag’. Ruffs foraged in the morning and afternoons on the grasslands and roosted at the shores of Lake IJsselmeer and along smaller lakes for a few hours at midday and at night (unpubl. data) where they were checked daily for colour rings using binoculars and telescopes. Our resighting effort of colour-ringed birds was more or less constant between 15 March and 20 May and covered all known foraging and roosting sites in the study area (Fig. 1).

Figure 1.

The study area in southwest Fryslân, The Netherlands. Indicated are the main roosting sites used by the staging Ruffs. Black circles indicate the roosts where the automated receiver stations (ARTS) tracking the radio-tagged Ruffs were placed. The box indicates the core study area (comprising c. 200 km2) where roosts and feeding grounds were searched daily for colour-marked individuals and where the majority of catching sites was situated.

f01_21.eps

Additionally, radio transmitters (BD2 transmitters, with 11 weeks guaranteed battery life time, Holohil Systems Ltd. Carp, Ontario, Canada) were issued in 2005 and 2007 to 48 and 47 adult males, respectively. The transmitters weighed 1.8 g, at most 1% of the body mass of an adult male, and were glued to the clipped feather base and skin on the plateau below the spine on the lower back using Super Glue Gel. The transmitters would fall off at the summer body moult. Individuals selected for tagging were on average 1.9 g heavier than the average population (unpubl. data). In both seasons a single transmitter was used to test equipment. These test transmitters lasted at least 10 weeks, indicating that battery life covered the migration period easily.

Automated receiving stations (ARTS) were placed close to the nine known roosting sites, covering the vast majority of Ruffs in the study area (Fig. 1). The ARTS were scheduled to collect four signals per individual at 20 min intervals, 24 hours a day. An individual was considered present if at least 3 out of 4 recordings gave a signal 1.5x stronger than the ‘noise’ (Green et al. 2002, Rogers et al. 2006). The ARTS recordings were collected between 25 March and 1 June each year. Minimal staging durations, the time interval between the day of tagging and the last day of recording, were calculated for each individual.

On the basis of this material we explore (1) catching effects (a comparison of staying probabilities between previously and newly captured birds), (2) age effects, (3) the occurrence of transients (transients are observed only once and therefore have a zero staying probability after an encounter at Timei), and (4) the effect of encounter method (resightings vs. radio-detection) on resighting probability and estimates of staging duration.

Encounter histories

We collected individual encounter histories spanning a 52-day period (25 March until 15 May). In southwest Fryslân most Ruffs arrive after 15 March, and by 15 May 99% of the birds usually have left, thus our 52-day study period encompasses most of the migration. Ruffs still present in June, and Ruffs ringed at catching localities outside the core area that were not intensively searched (Fig. 1) were excluded from the analyses. Since sample sizes for females were very low, females were also excluded from the analyses. In 2004, 2005, 2006, 2007 and 2008, we obtained 855, 788, 1052, 924, and 744 encounter histories of colour-marked male Ruffs, of which up to 56% had been colour-ringed in a previous year. For radio-tagged Ruffs we collected 42 encounter histories in 2005 (four faeders — female mimics (Jukema & Piersma 2006) — were excluded from the analyses and two birds were found dead soon after release). In 2007 we obtained 47 encounter histories. Ruffs still present after 15 May were left out of the analyses as they probably were part of either the relict local breeding population or non-breeders who ‘summer’ in the region (in grey in Fig. 2). As capture-recapture methodology requires the number of encounter intervals to be lower than the number of encounter histories, the data were pooled per 2-day intervals to reduce number of intervals to 26. For comparison, and since observations were only made 6 days per week, the encounter histories of the colour-ringed birds were pooled in 2-day intervals as well. Pooling has the additional advantage to reduce the number of parameters in the models (White & Burnham 1999).

Survival analysis to estimate staging duration

Mark—recapture models have been developed to estimate survival (White & Burnham 1999). The global model usually applied is Φtpt, where Φ is the apparent survival probability, p is the resighting probability and t is time. Here we use mark—recapture models to estimate staging duration (Schaub et al. 2001). In the Schaub method, t is day and the ‘survival’ parameter Φ is the staying probability, the equivalent of day-to-day survival. During staging we assume mortality to be zero and hence true survival to be one. The staying probability (Φ) is the probability that an individual present in the population at Dayi+1 will remain in the study area until Dayi+1, which is the complement to the departure or emigration probability (1-Φ). To estimate total staging duration, seniority must be estimated also. Seniority (γ) is the probability that an individual was already present at the site on Dayi-1; the complement to seniority is arrival or immigration probability (1-γ). It is calculated by reversing the encounter history (reverse-time modelling; Schaub et al. 2001). γ is related to the proportion of new individuals in a time interval; if the fraction newly recruited individuals is large, then the average seniority of birds in the interval is small. Note that encounter histories can not be reversed when the assumption of equal capture rates for each interval is violated (see next section for further explanation). The staying probability Φ was estimated with the Cormack-Jolly-Seber models (CJS models, Lebreton et al. (1992)) or variations of this basic model structure. The seniority probability γ was estimated with the Pradel recruitment method (Pradel 1996). To calculate the total staging duration we used the program SODA which estimates the duration of stay before encounter, using γ and the duration of stay after encounter, using Φ (Schaub et al. 2001).

Estimating staging duration by resightings

We collected three kinds of data that yield encounter histories (Table 1). The first data category consisted of the newly colour-marked males of all ages that were released with colour-marks in Yeari. The emigration probability of these birds could be affected by catching and handling. In each year this data category contained sufficient second-calendar years to test for a possible age affect. The second data category consisted of previously ringed birds: resighted in Yeari but marked earlier in Yearj<i; all birds are adults and these previously ringed birds were not affected by catching and handling. The third category is the ARTS data set of radio-tagged adult males released and recorded in Yeari (2005 and 2007). We used the staging duration of the radio-tagged birds ringed in the first two catching cohorts (caught before 2 April) as a minimum estimate of staging duration.

Figure 2.

Encounter histories of the radio-tagged adult male Ruffs staging in southwest Fryslân, The Netherlands, during northward migration in spring in 2005 (A) and 2007 (B). Black symbols indicate ARTS recordings. Symbols with a cross indicate the last encounter by observers using hand-held receivers. Open symbols indicate faeders (female mimics). The round symbol indicates a colour-ringed resighting of a radio-tagged individual. Radio-tagged individuals left out of the analyses are indicated in grey.

f02_21.eps

The staying probability can be estimated for all birds. However, the seniority probability, and hence total staging duration, can only be estimated for previously ringed birds as only for these birds all encounters are resightings. Encounter histories of newly ringed birds start with a catching event. For these newly ringed birds the calculation of seniority within the same season is not possible as the assumption of a constant recapture probability is violated. In reverse-time modelling the last interval in the reverse encounter history would lead to a catching event, which has an other probability than a resighting. For estimations we used the programs E-SURGE version 1.4.6, M7.2 (Choquet et al. 2009b), MARK version 4.3 (White & Burnham 1999) and SODA  http://esapubs.org/Archive/ecol/E082/008/suppl-1.htm (Schaub et al. 2001).

Testing goodness-of-fit

To test the goodness-of-fit (GOF) of the data to the CJS models, U-CARE version 2.3, M7.2 was used (Choquet et al. 2009a). For the colour-marked birds, the global GOF test for the most complex model was not significant (χ2 874 = 870.41, P = 0.528, ĉ= 1), indicating good fits. The subtests detected an apparent transients effect in previously ringed birds in 2006–08 (Table 2). When we left out the few individuals that were resighted very frequently (over 5 times), this transients effect disappeared. We therefore consider the apparent transients effect a consequence of somewhat stronger encounter heterogeneity in 2006–08, and excluded the possibility of transients in our migratory population which is supported by the resighting histories of the radio-tagged Ruffs (Fig. 2). We conclude that our data fit sufficiently to use the CJS model as a starting model. For the radio-tagged Ruffs a general lack of fit was indicated by the significance of the global test (χ2 72 = 181.63, P < 0.001, ĉ = 2.52). The overdispersion was due to trap-dependence (Table 2). Individuals encountered at t had a higher probability to be encountered at t+1 than individuals not encountered at t. To account for this trap-dependence we transformed the dataset in U-CARE, and used this decomposed dataset modelled for trap-dependence in E-SURGE.

Table 2.

Goodness-of-fit test results for the global model, Φ(t)p(t), for three data categories with encounter histories of Ruffs staging at a spring staging site in The Netherlands. Radio-tags were applied to adult males, and colour rings (CLR) to males of all ages. Previously ringed individuals were ringed in Yearj<i and not recaptured but only resighted in Yeari. Newly ringed and radio-tagged Ruffs were ringed in Yeari. Newly ringed males are divided in adults and second-calendar year males (2cy). n is the number of individual encounter histories.

t02_21.gif

Model selection

The capture—resighting data and the radio-tagging data were analysed separately to avoid missing data issues for the years without radio-tagging. To simplify model notation, previously marked birds are referred to as state 1 and newly marked (ringed or radio-tagged) birds as state 2. To test for effects of date (t), year (y), age (a) and state (s) on the staying probability Φ, we examined all hypotheses on additive and interactive effects of these four parameters. This resulted in 37 models to be tested for colour-ringed birds and 13 models for radio-tagged birds (which contained only adults and a single state, state 2).

In staging areas with seasonally peaked abundances, the emigration probabilities will increase over the season; this makes Φ date-specific (t). In this study the number of observers increased each year leading to an increase in resighting rates; this makes p year dependent. Hence the model Φtpy was considered the biologically relevant initial model. Subsequently several models with additive and interactive effects between date and year were tested. Next, the interactive effects of age and state (Φ(t+y).a or Φ(t+y).s) were considered. Also models that assumed age and state effects to be additive to the year and date effect (Φt+y+.a+s) were considered. And it was examined how sensitive the supposed effects of state and age on staying probability were to differences in resighting probabilities between these groups, by examining various models with interactive and additive effects of the four parameters (e.g. Pt.y(s+a), P(t.y)+s+a)· As there were no young birds in the state ‘previously ringed’, the interaction between s and a could not be estimated.

The candidate models were ranked according to the modified Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), and corrected for ĉ values of the global models to account for overdispersion and sample size (QAICc) (Burnham & Anderson 1998).

As the bootstrapping procedure in SODA was run for each year separately, model selection was repeated for each year. SODA also estimates the time present before encounter, so we needed to select models for both Φ and γ. For all years, except 2005, the most parsimonious models were Φtpt and γtpt. In 2005 the resighting probability was constant, with Φtp and γtp being the most parsimonious models.

Estimates of staging duration

In SODA, for each of the 26 time intervals within each year, 300 estimates for duration of stay before encounter (for previously ringed birds only) and duration of stay after encounter (for each method in each year) were obtained by bootstrapping (Schaub et al. 2001). As the estimates converged at 200 iterations after testing 50, 100, 200 iterations, the number of iterations was set safely to 300. The estimates obtained followed a normal distribution. In all years the intervals 20–26 (4–15 May) had less than 10 resightings and to prevent overestimating total staging duration, all estimates for the period from 4 May onwards were left out (Morris et al. 2006).

RESULTS

Encounter rates

In newly ringed and previously ringed Ruffs, the proportion of individuals resighted after the initial catch or after the initial resighting after returning was not significantly different (χ2 3 = 4.77, P = 0.11), being 22% in 2004, 26% in 2005, 47% in 2006, 43% in 2007 and 36% in 2008. The most parsimonious model did not include the state parameter (s) in the resighting probability (p) term (Tables 1 and 3), indicating that p was not significantly different between newly ringed and previously ringed Ruffs. Over the 2-day intervals p was on average 0.14 for all birds. For adult males only, mean p over all 26 time interval was 0.11 (95% CI 0.03–0.19) in 2004, 0.13 (0.02–0.21) in 2005, 0.17 (0.13–0.22) in 2006, 0.15 (0.12–0.18) in 2007, and 0.15 (0.09–0.21) in 2008.

The proportion of radio-tagged birds re-encountered after release was 99%; only one individual was never reported and many individuals were recorded daily (Fig. 2) and often several times per day. The resighting probability over the 2-day intervals was 0.67 (0.61–0.71) in 2005 and 0.80 (0.75–0.83) in 2007; the difference between the years was significant (Table 4). This is most likely a consequence of birds spending more time at the roosts during day-time in 2007 compared with 2005 (unpubl. data).

Staying probabilities and catching effects

The staying probability Φof the colour-ringed birds varied significantly with date, year, state (newly or previously marked) and age (Table 3). The additive models including year and date (Φy+t) were more strongly supported than the interactive models (Φy.t) (Table 3). This indicates that there were annual differences in the overall staying probabilities, but no annual differences in the seasonal pattern of emigration. The staying probability declined with date, which means that emigration rates increased over the season (Fig. 3).

By comparing newly colour-ringed with previously ringed Ruffs, we detected indications of a catching effect on staying probability (Table 3, Fig. 3). The model separating the two states (newly or previously ringed) had a better fit, although the confidence intervals of the estimates overlapped (Fig. 3B). Throughout the season, the newly ringed adults had on average a 1.9% higher staying probability than the previously ringed adults. The radio-tagged adult males had on average 9.4% higher staying probabilities than the previously ringed adult males, and this probability did not vary between years (Table 4).

Figure 3.

(A) Seasonal changes in staying probability (Φ) for four different groups: newly radio-tagged adult males, newly colour-ringed (CLR) second-calendar (2cy) year males, newly colour-ringed adult males, and previously colour-ringed adult males. 95% CIs are calculated over years. (B) Differences in staying probability between the four groups as obtained from the most parsimonious models using the program E-SURGE. 95% CIs are means for each time interval. Data meeting the requirements to estimate the total staging duration (see text) indicated in dark grey.

f03_21.eps

Table 3.

Model selection for staying and resighting probabilities of colour-ringed male Ruffs at a spring staging site in The Netherlands, with respect to date and year (2004–08), age (second-calendar year and adult) and data category (state, newly and previously ringed). See Fig. 3 for graphical representation of parameter differences in estimations of Φ. To lead the eye, the initial models including date and year only are indicated with *. Indicated are the deviance, the number of estimated parameters in the model (NP), QAICC, ΔQAICC and QAICC weights.

t03_21.gif

Figure 4.

Estimates of staging duration of male Ruffs at a spring staging site in The Netherlands between 2005 and 2008. Based on resightings of Ruffs marked in a previous year. Estimates (mean ± 95% CI of bootstrap estimates) were obtained by running 300 iterations per interval in the program SODA. (A) Duration of stay before encounter estimated with the seniority probability (γ); (B) Duration of stay after encounter as estimated with the staying probability (Φ). Note that 95% CI's are very small, except in 2005.

f04_21.eps

Table 4.

Model selection for staying and resighting probabilities of radio-tagged male Ruffs at a spring staging site in The Netherlands, with respect to date and year (2005 and 2007). See Fig. 3 for graphical representation of parameter differences in estimates for Φ. Indicated are the deviance, the number of estimated parameters in the model (NP), QAICC, ΔQAICC and QAICC weights.

t04_21.gif

Each year approximately 15% of newly caught males were second-calendar year males. That models including age fitted better (Table 3) suggests a significant age effect. The second-calendar year birds had a 4.9% higher staying probability than newly caught adults and 6.9% higher staying probability than returning adults (Fig. 3), which means that young males had a 4.9% longer staging duration than adults and that the catching and handling effect increased staging duration with 2%.

Estimates of staging duration

The average total staging duration of colour-ringed birds in 2006 was 23.2 ± 2.8 days, 21.1 ± 3.0 d in 2007 and 18.5 ± 3.9 d in 2008. The estimated duration of stay before encounter increased over the season and duration of stay after encounter decreased as the season progressed (Fig. 4). In 2005, the staging duration estimate was very low (14.0 ± 3.5 d). Especially the estimates of time before encounter were considerably lower than in other years, while the time after encounter fell within the distribution of the other years. In contrast, minimal staging durations of radio-tagged birds were similar in 2005 and 2007 (see below). The low resighting probability and low sample size in 2005 may have created a false signal of recruitment and hence an underestimate of the seniority. We conclude that in 2005 time before encounter was underestimated due to the low resighting rates and low samples size and we consider estimates of total staging duration obtained from colour-ringed birds in 2005 unreliable.

Minimal staging duration of radio-tagged birds was not significantly different between years (F 1,82 = 0.71, P = 0.40); however, the interaction between tagging cohort and year was significant (F 3,82 = 2.65, P = 0.05) with minimal staging duration estimates in the first two cohorts being higher in 2005 than in 2007 (Fig. 5). We consider the staging duration of the first cohorts to be a close approximation of the total staging duration as birds were tagged soon after arrival. The staging duration of these first cohorts was 24 days in 2005 and 19 days in 2007 (Table 5, Fig. 5).

Figure 5.

Minimal staging duration (the time interval between tagging and the last recording) of adult male Ruffs radio-tagged in 2005 and 2007. Sample sizes are indicated below box plots. Boxes represents the 95% CI intervals, horizontal lines represent the median, bars represent the range, solid dots are mean values and open dots are outliers.

f05_21.eps

Table 5.

The minimal staging duration of radio-tagged Ruffs (mean ± SD), which is the time interval between the day of tagging and the last day of recording as presented in Fig. 2. The minimal staging duration is given for three groups. The birds tagged before 2 April are supposed to belong to the first arrival cohort. n is sample size.

t05_21.gif

DISCUSSION

We explored possible catching and handling effects on staging duration of staging Ruffs in The Netherlands. Models allowing newly and previously colour-ringed birds to have different staying probabilities had better fits (Table 3) and subsequent comparison of model estimates showed that the staying probability was highest in newly ringed and radio-tagged Ruffs (Fig. 3). However, as the confidence intervals overlap (Fig. 3) it remains to be seen whether these differences are relevant. Here we explore what our findings would mean for staging duration if true. Model estimates indicated that staging durations of newly colour-ringed adult males were 0.4–0.5 days longer than birds ringed in previous years. This longer time span might represent the time needed to recover from capture and handling and given the average total staging duration of 18.5–23.3 days this cost is rather small. The second-year birds would stay 1.0 days longer than adults, which is biologically relevant as younger, inexperienced birds might need more time to accumulate new body stores.

The 9% difference in staying probability between adult males radio-tagged in Yeari and adult males colour-ringed in Yearj<i, would translate in an estimated prolonging of the staging duration by 1.8–2.3 d. Subtracting the value we found above for the time cost of capture and handling indicates that the effects of the radio transmitter would add 1.4–1.8 d to the staging duration. Is there any indication why radio-tagged birds would increase their staging duration more than colour-ringed birds? The weight of the tag added only 1% to the adult males' body mass and we do not expect them to compensate for this weight gain. However, radio-tagged birds were brought to the field station to be processed and thus were handled for longer than birds that only received colour-rings who were mostly processed in the field. We suggest that this longer timelag between catching and release could lead to a longer staging duration. In Dunlin Calidris alpina on northward migration a similar effect was found when the length of stay at marking sites was compared with more northerly staging sites (Warnock et al. 2004). However, part of this particular variation might be attributed to faster migration later in spring and/or a much lower probability to re-encounter a bird during later stages of migration (Warnock et al. 2004). In summary, the detected catching effects were very small and might be insignificant to Ruffs.

As we did not find significant transient effects in the newly ringed birds, captured Ruffs did not seem to emigrate from the catching area soon after the event (Table 2). We did not find indications for higher mortality rates in newly ringed birds or radio-tagged birds. The staying probability Φ is a parameter with two components: the site fidelity, Φf, the probability to remain in the study area, and the true survival Φs. A high mortality rate of new catches (a low Φs) can not be detected when it is obscured by a extended staging duration of survivors (a high Φr). However, the proportion of birds resighted after the first encounter was equal in newly and previously ringed Ruffs, and 99% of the radio-tagged males were detected after release, which suggest that mortality due to catching was low for colour-ringed and radio-tagged Ruffs.

The second goal of this paper was to assess methodological caveats in estimations of staging duration by comparing data categories of colour-marked and radio-tagged Ruffs. Firstly, we want to stress that when applying the method implemented in SODA to estimate the time before encounter, resightings of newly captured birds have to be omitted at each time interval, as the encounter history cannot contain different encounter methods with different probabilities (Schaub et al. 2001). If newly marked individuals are not excluded, they create a false signal of recruitment or immigration in the population, which will lead to an underestimation of the seniority probability. This implies that for analyses of staging duration using resightings, newly marked birds cannot be used. However when after initial capture, the subsequent encounters are captures as well, there is no problem (Schaub & Jenni 2001, Schaub et al. 2004).

Secondly, we confirmed with empirical data that estimates of staging duration are sensitive to a low resighting probability, which is in line with a sensitivity analysis for a general case (Calvert et al. 2009). In 2005 the sample size of 113 previously colour-ringed birds was insufficient to yield reliable estimates of seniority, which led to low estimates of the duration of stay which were incongruent with telemetry data. In 2007, when p had increased, the colour-ring data yielded estimates that were comparable to other years and to the telemetry data. One of the basic assumptions of capture—recapture analyses is that the resighting probability should not affect estimates of Φ and γ. Note that it has been suggested before that very large sample sizes are required to overcome a low resighting probability (Sandercock 2003, Calvert et al. 2009).

The methods as implemented in the software SODA (Schaub et al. 2001) and the MARK and E-SURGE model selection (White & Burnham 1999, Choquet et al. 2009b) are useful for ecologists to estimate staging duration, but have their shortcomings. SODA does not yet allow for capture—recapture models that incorporate strong heterogeneity in encounter rates that is not due to transients but to variance in ‘trap-happiness’. Another problem is that SODA assumes a normal distribution of staging durations and when staging duration follows alternative distributions this may result in large overestimates (up to 100%) (Efford 2005). However, as shown by an extensive sensitivity analysis on Red Knots Calidris canutus, the Schaub method is relatively insensitive to different cohorts within the passage population and estimates of staging duration were fairly robust between scenarios (Gillings et al. 2009).

We therefore conclude that (1) estimates of staging duration can be affected by catching, ringing and radio-tagging, and that (2) low resighting probabilities can lead to underestimates in staging duration. For our study system, we conclude that the colour-mark data of the previously ringed Ruffs from 2006–08 yield biologically reliable estimates of staging duration as the data had sufficiently high resighting probabilities to meet the available mark—recapture model assumptions and estimates were not affected by catching/handling.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study would have been impossible without the Friesian Wilsternetters, who caught all our Ruffs: Piet Vlas, Joop Jukema, Douwe en Rinkje van der Zee, Jaap Strikwerda, Catharinus Monkel, Albert A. Mulder, Fons Baarsma, Hette Couperus, Doede H. Mulder, Rein H. Mulder, Doede A. Mulder, Eeltje A. Mulder, Albert H. Mulder, Douwe de Jager, Willem Louwsma, Bauke de Jong, Bram van der Veen, Jappie Boersma, Bauke Kuipers, Cees Dekker, Sierd Visser and Piet Feenstra. We spent many interesting and fun hours behind their ‘skûles’. Catching, ringing and tagging activities were covered by required licences under de Dutch law. Job ten Horn, Elske Schut, Francisco Encinas, Claudia Burger, Kathryn Hine, Angela Medina, Monika Parsons, Emily Morris, Scott Davies, Lucie Schmaltz and Robbie Watts ringed many birds and contributed many resightings to the database. Kathryn Hine processed two years of radio-telemetry data. We also thank the godwit-team, especially Pedro Lourenço, and the many other observers for all the Ruff resightings. Luuk Postuma kindly provided his time and processor speed to run SODA analyses. Erich Bächler, and Allan J. Baker, Lucie Schmaltz, Cedrik Juillet and Tamar Lok and other participants of the Global Flyway Network demographics workshop advised on the analyses. We thank Jeroen Reneerkens, Jutta Leyrer, Cedrik Juillet, Simon Gillings and the anonymous referee for their helpful comments on earlier versions. This study was financed by a start-up grant to TP from the University of Groningen and by the GUF-Gratama Foundation (project 04.05) and the Schure-Beijerinck Popping Foundation (SBP/JK2006-39 and SBP/JK2007-34). We dedicate this study to the memories of Douwe van der Zee, who maintained his position as top ‘Ruff-netter’ until three years after his untimely death in 2005, and of Hette Couperus, who passed on his skills and passion to a younger generation of wilsternetters.

REFERENCES

1.

E. Bächler & M. Schaub 2007. The effects of permanent local emigration and encounter technique on stopover duration estimates as revealed by telemetry and mark—recapture. Condor 109: 142–154. Google Scholar

2.

R.F. Battley , T. Piersma , D.I. Rogers , A. Dekinga , B. Spaans & J.A. van Gils 2004. Do body condition and plumage during fuelling predict northwards departure dates of Great Knots Calidris tenuirostris from north-west Australia? Ibis 146: 46–60. Google Scholar

3.

M.A. Bishop , P.M. Meyers & R.F. McNeley 2000. A method to estimate migrant shorebird numbers on the Copper River Delta, Alaska. J. Field Ornithol. 71: 627–637. Google Scholar

4.

K.P. Burnham & D.R. Anderson 1998. Model selection and interference: a practical information-theoretic approach. Springer, New York. Google Scholar

5.

A.M. Calvert , S.J. Bonner , I.D. Jonsen , J.M. Flemming , S.J. Walde & P.D. Taylor 2009. A hierarchical Bayesian approach to multi-state mark—recapture: simulations and applications. J. Appl. Ecol. 46: 610–620. Google Scholar

6.

R. Choquet , J.D. Lebreton , O. Gimenez , A.M. Reboulet & R. Pradel 2009a. U-CARE: Utilities for performing goodness of fit tests and manipulating CApture—REcapture data. Ecography 32: 1071–1074. Google Scholar

7.

R. Choquet , L. Rouan & R. Pradel 2009b. Program E-SURGE: a software application for fitting multievent models. In: D.L Thomson , E.G. Cooch & M.J. Conroy (eds) Environmental and Ecological Statistics Vol.3: Modeling demographic processes in marked populations. Springer, pp. 845–865. Google Scholar

8.

M.G. Efford 2005. Migrating birds stop over longer than usually thought: Comment. Ecology 86: 3415–3418. Google Scholar

9.

M. Frederiksen , A.D. Fox , J. Madsen & K. Colhoun 2001. Estimating the total number of birds using a staging site. J. Wildl. Manage. 65: 282–289. Google Scholar

10.

S. Gillings , P.W. Atkinson , A.J. Baker , K.A. Bennett , N.A. Clark , K.B. Cole , P.M. González , K.S. Kalasz , C.D.T. Minton , L.J. Niles , R.C. Porter , I. De Lima Serrano , H.P. Sitters & J.L. Woods 2009. Staging behavior in Red Knot (Calidris canutus) in Delaware Bay: implications for monitoring mass and population size. Auk 126: 54–63. Google Scholar

11.

M. Green , T. Piersma , J. Jukema , P. de Goeij , B. Spaans & J. van Gils 2002. Radio-telemetry observations of the first 650 km of the migration of Bar-tailed Godwits Limosa lapponica from the Wadden Sea to the Russian Arctic. Ardea 90: 71–80. Google Scholar

12.

A. Hedenström 2008. Adaptations to migration in birds: behavioural strategies, morphology and scaling effects. Phil. Trans. Biol. Sci. 363: 287–299. Google Scholar

13.

A. Hedenström & T. Alerstam 1997. Optimum fuel loads in migratory birds: Distinguishing between time and energy minimization. J. Theor. Biol. 189: 227–234. Google Scholar

14.

A.J. Hogan-Warburg 1966. Social behavior of Ruff Philomachus pugnax. Ardea 54: 109–229. Google Scholar

15.

J. Jukema & T. Piersma 2006. Permanent female mimics in a lekking shorebird. Biol. Lett. 2: 161–164. Google Scholar

16.

J. Jukema , T. Piersma , J.B. Hulscher , E.J. Bunskoeke , A. Koolhaas & A. Veenstra 2001a. Golden Plovers and wilsternetters: a deeply rooted fascination with migrating birds. Fryske Academy/KNNV Uitgeverij, Ljouwert/Utrecht, The Netherlands. Google Scholar

17.

J. Jukema , T. Piersma , L. Louwsma , C. Monkel , U. Rijpma , K. Visser & D. van der Zee 1995. Moult and body mass changes of migrating Ruffs in Friesland in 1993 and 1994. Vanellus 48: 55–61 . (In Dutch)  Google Scholar

18.

J. Jukema & T. Piersma 2000. Contour feather moult of Ruffs Philomachus pugnax during northward migration, with notes on homology of nuptial plumages in scolopacid waders. Ibis 142: 289–296. Google Scholar

19.

J. Jukema , E. Wymenga & T. Piersma 2001b. Stopping over in SW Friesland: fattening and moulting in Ruffs Philomachus pugnax during northward migration in The Netherlands. Limosa 74: 17–26. (In Dutch) Google Scholar

20.

M. Kersten & C.J. Smit 1984. The Atlantic coast of Morocco. In: P.R. Evans , J.D. Goss-Custard & W.G. Hale (eds) Coastal Waders and Wildfowl in Winter. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 276–292. Google Scholar

21.

D.B. Lank & J. Dale 2001. Visual signals for individual identification: The silent “song” of Ruffs. Auk 118: 759–765. Google Scholar

22.

D.B. Lank , C.M. Smith , O. Hanotte , T. Burke & F. Cooke 1995. Genetic polymorphism for alternative mating behavior in lekking male Ruff Philomachus pugnax. Nature 378: 5–62. Google Scholar

23.

J.D. Lebreton , K.P. Burnham , J. Clobert & D.R. Anderson 1992. Modeling survival and testing biological hypotheses using marked animals - a unified approach with case-studies. Ecol. Monogr. 62: 67–118. Google Scholar

24.

D.E. Lee , J.M. Black , J.E. Moore & J.S. Sedinger 2007. Age-specific stopover ecology of Black Brant at Humboldt Bay, California. Wilson J. Ornithol. 119: 9–22. Google Scholar

25.

J. Melter & H.H. Bergmann 1996. Annual pattern of locomotor activity, moult and body mass in captive Ruffs (Philomachus pugnax). Vogelwarte 38: 169–179. Google Scholar

26.

S.R. Morris , A.M. Larracuente , K.M. Covino , M.S. Mustillo , K.E. Mattern , D.A. Liebner & H.D. Sheets 2006. Utility of open population models: limitations posed by parameter estimability in the study of migratory stopover. Wilson Bull. 118: 513–526. Google Scholar

27.

S. Nebel , T. Piersma , J.A. van Gils , A. Dekinga & B. Spaans 2000. Length of stopover, fuel storage and a sex-bias in the occurrence of red knots Calidris c. canutus and C.c. islandica in the Wadden Sea during southward migration. Ardea 88: 165–176. Google Scholar

28.

OAG Münster 1989. Observations on the spring migration of Ruffs Philomachus pugnax. J. Ornithol. 130: 175–182. Google Scholar

29.

OAG Münster 1992. Numbers of Ruffs during autumn migration in Germany, 1990. Vogelwelt 113: 102–113. Google Scholar

30.

OAG Münster 1996. Do females really outnumber males in Ruff Philomachus pugnax wintering in Africa? J. Ornithol. 137: 91–100. Google Scholar

31.

T. Piersma 1987. Hop, skip or jump? Constraints on migration of arctic waders by feeding, fattening and flight speed. Limosa 60: 185–194. In Dutch Google Scholar

32.

T. Piersma , K.G. Rogers , H. Boyd , E.J. Bunskoeke & J. Jukema 2005. Demography of Eurasian Golden Plovers Pluvialis apricaria staging in The Netherlands, 1949–2000. Ardea 93: 49–64. Google Scholar

33.

R. Pradel 1996. Utilization of capture—mark—recapture for the study of recruitment and population growth rate. Biometrics 52: 703–709. Google Scholar

34.

J.G. van Rhijn 1991. The Ruff. Individuality in a gregarious wading bird. Poyser, London. Google Scholar

35.

S.M. Rice , J.A. Collazo , M.W. Alldredge , B.A. Harrington & A.R. Lewis 2007. Local annual survival and seasonal residency rates of semipalmated sandpipers (Calidris pusilla) in Puerto Rico. Auk 124: 1397–1406. Google Scholar

36.

D.I. Rogers , P.F. Battley , T. Piersma , J.A. van Gils & K.G. Rogers 2006. High-tide habitat choice: insights from modelling roost selection by shorebirds around a tropical bay. Anim. Behav. 72: 563–575. Google Scholar

37.

V. Salewski , M. Thoma & M. Schaub 2007. Stopover of migrating birds: simultaneous analysis of different marking methods enhances the power of capture-recapture analyses. J. Ornithol. 148: 29–37. Google Scholar

38.

B.K. Sandercock 2003. Estimation of survival rates for wader populations: a review of mark—recapture methods. Wader Study Group Bull. 100: 163–174. Google Scholar

39.

M. Schaub , O. Gimenez , B.R. Schmidt & R. Pradel 2004. Estimating survival and temporary emigration in the multistate capture-recapture framework. Ecology 85: 2107–2113. Google Scholar

40.

M. Schaub & L. Jenni 2001. Stopover durations of three warbler species along their autumn migration route. Oecologia 128: 217–227. Google Scholar

41.

M. Schaub , R. Pradel , L. Jenni & J.D. Lebreton 2001. Migrating birds stop over longer than usually thought: An improved capture—recapture analysis. Ecology 82: 852–859. Google Scholar

42.

Y. Verkuil & P. de Goeij 2003. Do Reeves make different choices? Meadow selection by spring staging Ruffs Philomachus pugnax in Southwest Friesland. Limosa 76: 157–168. (In Dutch) Google Scholar

43.

N. Warnock 2009. Stopping vs. staging sites: a difference between a hop and a jump. Wader Study Group Bull. 116: 213. Google Scholar

44.

N. Warnock , J.Y. Takekawa & M.A. Bishop 2004. Migration and stopover strategies of individual Dunlin along the Pacific coast of North America. Can. J. Zool. 82: 1687–1697. Google Scholar

45.

G.C. White & K.P. Burnham 1999. Program MARK: survival estimation from populations of marked animals. Bird Study 46: 120–139. Google Scholar

46.

F. Widemo 1997. The social implications of traditional use of lek sites in the Ruff Philomachus pugnax. Behav. Ecol. 8: 211–217. Google Scholar

47.

E. Wymenga 1999. Migrating Ruffs Philomachus pugnax through Europe in spring. Wader Study Group Bull. 88: 43–48. Google Scholar

48.

R.C. Ydenberg , R.W. Butler , D.B. Lank , B.D. Smith & J. Ireland 2004. Western sandpipers have altered migration tactics as peregrine falcon populations have recovered. Proc. Biol. Sci. 271: 1263–1269. Google Scholar

49.

C. Zöckler 2002. Declining Ruff Philomachus pugnax populations: a response to global warming? Wader Study Group Bull. 97: 19–29. Google Scholar

50.

L. Zwarts , R. Bijlsma , J. van der Kamp & E. Wymenga 2009. Living on the edge. Wetlands and birds in changing Sahel. KNNV Publishing, Zeist, The Netherlands. Google Scholar

Appendices

SAMENVATTING

Langeafstandstrekkers onderbreken onderweg vaak de trek om nieuwe lichaamsreserves op te slaan. De tijd die wordt doorgebracht op de rustplaatsen bepaalt in belangrijke mate de snelheid waarmee de hele trekweg wordt afgelegd. Wij hebben in 2004–08 met behulp van de ‘merk-terugvangst’ methode van Schaub et al. (2001) een schatting gemaakt van de tijd die Kemphanen Philomachus pugnax, die in grote aantallen hun voorjaarstrek in Friesland onderbreken, op de Friese pleisterplaatsen verbleven. Met de Schaub-methode wordt de totale verblijfsduur geschat door twee kansen te berekenen: (1) de zogenaamde senioriteit, de kans dat de vogel al aanwezig was voor de vangst of waarneming (gelijk aan 1-immigratiekans) en (2) de blijfkans, de kans dat een vogel na de vangst of waarneming blijft (1-emigratiekans). In totaal werden 4363 manlijke Kemphanen van kleurringen voorzien (voor de schaarse vrouwtjes en faren werden onvoldoende waarnemingen verzameld: deze zijn hier verder buiten beschouwing gelaten). In 2005 en 2007 werden bovendien 95 mannetjes van een zendertje voorzien. We vergelijken in ons artikel drie sets met gegevens: (1) waarnemingen aan Kemphanen die in het betreffende voorjaar van kleurringen waren voorzien (de nieuw-gekleurringde groep), (2) waarnemingen aan Kemphanen die in voorafgaande jaren waren gemerkt en opnieuw Friesland aandeden (de reedsgekleurringde groep) en (3) de geautomatiseerde registraties (van negen vangstations) van Kemphanen met zenders (de gezenderde groep). Voor de gezenderde vogels werd de minimale verblijfsduur berekend (de tijdsperiode tussen het aanbrengen van de zender en het laatste vastgelegde signaal). Deze minimale verblijfsduur werd gebruikt om de verblijfsduurschattingen van de vogels met kleurringen te evalueren. Voor nieuw-gekleurringde vogels kon de totale verblijfsduur niet worden bepaald omdat de senioriteit niet kan worden geschat. De blijfkans na vangst of waarneming kan wel voor beide groepen worden bepaald. De blijfkansmodellen detecteerden een vangeffect: nieuw-gekleurringde vogels hadden een grotere blijfkans dan de reeds-gekleurringde vogels. Het verschil, omgerekend naar de duur van het verblijf na vangst of waarneming, was echter slechts 0,4–0,5 dagen. Het vangeffect was dus heel klein. In 2007 was de minimale verblijfsduur van de gezenderde vogels (19,1 ± 9,7 dagen) iets lager dan de totale verblijfsduur van de reeds-gekleurringde vogels (21,1 ± 3,0 dagen). In 2005 was de minimale verblijfsduur van de gezenderde vogels echter veel hoger (21,1 ± 11,3 dagen tegen 14,0 ± 6,3 dagen). In 2005 was de waarnemingskans van vogels met kleurringen echter laag en de steekproef klein. Dit leidde tot lage waarden voor de aanwezigheidsduur. De totale verblijfsduur van 2005 is daarom hoogstwaarschijnlijk een onderschatting. We concluderen dat voor het beschrijven van de totale verblijfsduur van Kemphanen in Friesland alleen de reeds-gekleurringde vogels van 2006–08 betrouwbare schattingen van de verblijfsduur (19–23 dagen) hebben opgeleverd.

Yvonne I. Verkuil, Jan J. Wijmenga, Jos C.E.W. Hooijmeijer, and Theunis Piersma "Spring Migration of Ruffs Philomachus pugnax in Fryslân: Estimates of Staging Duration Using Resighting Data," Ardea 98(1), 21-33, (1 March 2010). https://doi.org/10.5253/078.098.0104
Received: 26 February 2009; Accepted: 1 February 2010; Published: 1 March 2010
KEYWORDS
capture—recapture models
catching effects
colour-ringing
encounter methods
radio telemetry
resighting data
shorebird migration
Back to Top