Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
28 June 2022 Effects of Color Attributes on Trap Capture Rates of Chrysobothris femorata (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) and Related Species
Cynthia L. Perkovich, Karla M. Addesso, Joshua P. Basham, Donna C. Fare, Nadeer N. Youssef, Jason B. Oliver
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Chrysobothris spp. (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) and other closely related buprestids are common pests of fruit, shade, and nut trees in the United States. Many Chrysobothris spp., including Chrysobothris femorata, are polyphagous herbivores. Their wide host range leads to the destruction of numerous tree species in nurseries and orchards. Although problems caused by Chrysobothris are well known, there are no reliable monitoring methods to estimate local populations before substantial damage occurs. Other buprestid populations have been effectively estimated using colored sticky traps to capture beetles. However, the attraction of Chrysobothris to specific color attributes has not been directly assessed. A multi-color trapping system was utilized to determine color attraction of Chrysobothris spp. Specific color attributes (lightness [L*], red to green [a*], blue to yellow [b*], chroma [C*], hue [h*], and peak reflectance [PR]) were then evaluated to determine beetle responses. In initial experiments with mostly primary colors, Chrysobothris were most attracted to traps with red coloration. Thus, additional experiments were performed using a range of trap colors with red reflectance values. Among these red reflectance colors, it was determined that the violet range of the electromagnetic spectrum had greater attractance to Chrysobothris. Additionally, Chrysobothris attraction correlated with hue and b*, suggesting a preference for traps with hues between red to blue. However, males and females of some Chrysobothris species showed differentiated responses. These findings provide information on visual stimulants that can be used in Chrysobothris trapping and management. Furthermore, this information can be used in conjunction with ecological theory to understand host-location methods of Chrysobothris.

Metallic wood-boring beetles (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) are a large family with ∼15,000 species worldwide (750 in North America) (Bellamy and Volkovitsh 2016, Tong et al. 2021), some of which are significant pests of woody plants (Lebude and Adkins 2014, Haack and Petrice 2019). The larvae of buprestids cause the most damage, typically tunneling and feeding just beneath the bark of roots, trunks, and branches (Bellamy and Volkovitsh 2016). Most species attack dead or dying plant material, but some can girdle and kill apparently healthy trees (Moraal and Hilszczanski 2000, Redilla and McCullough 2017).

Most recent studies conducted on buprestid beetles have focused on a few economically important species like emerald ash borer (Agrilus plannipenis Fairmaire) (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) (Imrei et al. 2020a). However, several less documented buprestid species (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) such as the flatheaded appletree borer (Chrysobothris femorata Olivier) (Potter et al. 1988, Dawadi et al. 2019), the Pacific flatheaded borer (Chrysobothris mali Horn) (Acheampong et al. 2016), and the bronze birch borer (Agrilus anxius Gory) (Haack and Petrice 2019) routinely cause serious damage to production nursery or landscape plantings, often during the first season following transplant when trees are under establishment stress.

Closely related species in the C. femorata complex are serious pests of landscape and nursery trees (Potter et al. 1988, Oliver et al. 2010). These species frequently attack newly transplanted trees in nursery and orchard production and will also damage branches of established orchard trees (Baspinar et al. 2018). The damage from flatheaded borers is unapparent until the larva becomes large enough to produce visible injury on the trunk surface (Oliver et al. 2010, LeBude and Adkins 2014). Death of the subsurface vascular tissues by larval feeding often causes visible bark surface splits, darkened and discolored bark, or plant dieback. Infestations may not be apparent until after adult beetles emerge, which leave behind a characteristic D-shaped exit hole. Damage by the larvae can kill the tree or make nursery trees unmarketable. Other members of this family can reduce the value of harvested logs and lumber (Haack et al. 2002, 2014) or reduce yields of fruits and nuts when branches are damaged (Andrews et al. 2017, Beers 2017). In middle Tennessee, Woodiel (1979) reported infestation rates of maple liners by C. femorata to be from 0 to 100%, and Fare (2015) found nursery damage on flowering dogwood (Cornus florida L.) (Cornales: Cornaceae) and maple (Acer spp.) (Sapindales: Sapindaceae) trees ranging up to 26%. Furthermore, in the western United States, another Chrysobothris species (C. mali) has caused orchard losses ranging from 35% in hazelnut (Corylus avellana L. [Fagales: Betulaceae]) to 100% in Prunus spp. (Burke and Boving 1929, Wiman et al. 2019).

It is widely acknowledged that many wood-boring beetles use color as a visual cue to locate hosts and potential mates (e.g., Francese et al. 2005, Petrice et al. 2013, Imrei et al. 2020a,b). However, recent research suggests that understanding borer perception mechanisms and interpretation of color may be more important than color attraction alone (Crook et al. 2009, Domingue et al. 2016, Cavaletto et al. 2020). Color attributes, as defined by the CIE (Commission Internationale de I'Elcairage, or International Commission on Illumination) L*a*b*color space models (i.e., lightness [L*], red to green [a*], blue to yellow [b*], chroma [C*], hue [h*], and peak reflectance [PR]; see Table 1), are useful in parsing color preferences of buprestids and other wood-boring beetles. Beetle physiology allows for the recognition of color attributes, which may affect visual recognition (Meglic et al. 2020, Zhi-zhi et al. 2021). For example, in the case of Dendroctonus frontalis Zimmerman (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae), reflectance was a better predicter for capture rates than absolute color (Strom and Goyer 2001). Furthermore, perception of colors may catalyze behavioral responses (Francese et al. 2010, Domingue et al. 2016). Domingue et al. (2016) found that male Agrilus angustulus Illiger (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) have a spectral preference for female color morphs with higher chroma values. In this scenario, color attributes can act as a mechanism to alter male sexual behaviors.

Table 1.

Definitions and abbreviations of color attribute terms analyzed and discussed in this study

img-z2-10_737.gif

Much of our current understanding of color attraction and trapping methods for buprestids arose from studies performed on the genus Agrilus in forest systems (Imrei et al. 2020a). Many Agrilus spp. are attracted to green traps (Francese et al. 2010, Petrice et al. 2013, Petrice and Haack 2015, Rassati et al. 2019, Rutledge 2020). Trapping methods also have been researched on other buprestid genera such as Coreaubus in oak forests (e.g., Fürstenau et al. 2015) and Ovalisia (Nitzu et al. 2016). As with Agrilus, other buprestid genera show an attraction to specific colors (Petrice et al. 2013, Cavaletto et al. 2020). Much of this buprestid trapping research has been performed in forested systems (e.g., Fürstenau et al. 2015, Imrei et al. 2020b), where light penetration through thicker forest canopies can alter color reflectance and affect perception. It is important to determine if Chrysobothris attraction patterns also extend to more open landscape settings such as nurseries. There are several reports of Chrysobothris spp. being trapped in studies targeting other forest pests (e.g., Varandi et al. 2018, Westcott et al. 2018, Cavaletto et al. 2020). However, the Chrysobothris spp. caught were not the focus of the trapping study. Furthermore, studies have reported Chrysobothris spp. captures on very different trap types. For example, Varandi et al. (2018) reported catching Chrysobothris affinis Fabricius (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) most effectively on red sticky traps, and Westcott et al. (2018) found Chrysobothris rugosiceps Melsheimer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) in black Lindgren funnel traps, baited with α-pinene and ethanol.

To expand knowledge on Chrysobothris spp. trap preference, experiments were designed to determine if Chrysobothris spp. and related buprestid nursery pests are attracted to specific color attributes as is the case with many related buprestids. The objective of this study was to evaluate Chrysobothris response to traps simulating nursery tree trunks with various primary colors (i.e., multi-color test). Following the initial multi-color test objective, a secondary objective was to evaluate Chrysobothris response to traps with a wider range of red reflectance attributes (i.e., red-reflectance color test). For both the multi-color and the red-reflectance tests, we predicted that color attributes (lightness [L*], red to green [a*], blue to yellow [b*], chroma [C*], hue [h*], and peak reflectance [PR]; see Table 1 for definitions) would be correlated with trap success for various Chrysobothris species. Understanding how color attributes affect buprestid trapping success may provide better monitoring tools for nursery, orchard, and landscape settings.

Methods

Multi-Colored Sticky Traps

To identify potential attractive trap colors for Chrysobothris and related buprestids under nursery field conditions in the initial study, primary color space colors of red, green, yellow, and blue, as well as white (reflects in all visible color wavelengths) and gray (to simulate a common bark color) were selected. Because this was an initial study to look at color cues, stock-vinyl wall liner (pattern# 85013, Graham & Brown Inc., Monroe Township, NJ) with a matt finish was used. The mean trap color attributes and peak reflectance spectrum for each color (mean reflectance values shown in Fig. 1a; mean L*, a*, b*, C*, and h* for these colors are given in Table 2) were recorded using a portable spectrophotometer (CM2600d, Konica Minolta Sensing Americas, Inc. Ramsey, NJ). Five scans of each color were averaged for the spectrophotometer measurements. The settings for the spectrophotometer were observer illuminant Daylight 65, observer angle at 10° (CIE1964), specular component included (SCI) and UV at 100% (see Werle et al. 2014 for further details). Spectrophotometer measurements were taken in stable laboratory conditions.

Fig. 1.

Average reflectance spectra of each trap color used (a) in the multicolor test and (b) the red color tests (n = 5). Spectral measurements were taken using a portable CM2600d spectrophotometer, Konica Minolta Sensing Americas, Inc. Ramsey, NJ, USA.

img-z3-2_737.jpg

Table 2.

Mean color attributes (±SE); red to green (a*), blue to yellow (b*), chroma (C*), hue (h*), and lightness (L*) of each color used in the multi-color trapping tests

img-z3-9_737.gif

Traps were established at the Tennessee State University Otis L. Floyd Nursery Research Center (NRC), McMinnville, TN from 2001 to 2002 using a sticky trap that simulated a sapling tree ( Supp Fig. 1 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_figure_s1.pdf) [online only]). Before installation, plots were initially cleared using Roundup (1.5% solution) (Bayer CropScience, St. Louis, MO). Once plots were sufficiently cleared, Surflan (applied at 4.48 kg active ingredient/hectare) (United Phosphorous, King of Prussia, PA), Pendulum 60 WDG (3.70 kg/hectare) (BASF, Research Triangle Park, NC), and Gallery (0.84 kg/hectare) (Corteva Agriscience, Indianapolis, IN) preemergent herbicides were applied. Individual sticky traps were made from 0.9 m long steel rods (i.e., 3 ft rebar). Wallpaper strips ∼7 cm wide were folded longitudinally with a 0.6 cm diameter (approximately the diameter of some nursery trees at that height) and stapled along the cut sides to form an elongated ‘tear-drop’ shaped tube. To simulate young tree silhouettes, wallpaper tubes were sheathed over metal stakes that had been driven into the ground two weeks after initial weed clearing. A 36 cm diameter geotextile disk was placed at the base of each metal stake and held in place by landscaping staples to prevent vegetation from growing around and obscuring the trap visibility ( Supp Fig. 1 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_figure_s1.pdf) [online only]). Rows were spaced 91 cm apart, and traps were spaced 58 cm apart within rows to simulate nursery crop systems that generally have greater distance between tree row spacing than within tree row spacing. Break through weed growth was spot treated with Roundup. Traps were covered with Pestick insect glue (Hummert International Horticultural Supplies, Earth City, MO). Sticky traps (wallpaper tubes with glue) were replaced weekly. To minimize positional effects, trap treatments were arranged in a completely randomized design using 25 and 15 replicates per treatment color in 2001 and 2002, respectively. In 2001, trapping was initiated on 1 May and continued until 5 September (2 wk after the last buprestid capture). In 2002, trapping began on 15 April and continued until 7 October (2 wk after last buprestid capture). Trapping was initiated earlier in the season during 2002, since buprestids were captured on the first trapping period of 2001 indicating the beginning of the flight period may have been missed.

Red-Reflecting Sticky Traps

During 2002 and 2003, the Royal Horticultural Colour Chart (Voss 1992, 2002) was referenced to select a wide range of trap treatment colors with greater red reflectance based on higher buprestid collections on the red trap color used in the multi-colored test. The selected color treatments (white, light pink, medium pink, dark pink, magenta, orange, purple, red, and dark red;  Supp Table 1 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s1.docx) [online only]) were then scanned to create custom matching paints (Sherwin-Williams, Cleveland, OH), which were painted onto white wallpaper strips (made from white wall liner used in multicolored study) and used to form tubes as previously described ( Supp Fig 2 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_figure_s2.pdf) [online only]). The red color used in the red-reflecting test was made from red paint that was created by scanning the red wallpaper used in the multicolor test; therefore, reflectance values for the red colors were not identical between the multicolor and red tests. The same mean trap color attributes previously described were determined for each trap color treatment (mean reflectance values shown in Fig. 1b, mean L*, a*, b*, C*, and h* for these colors are given in Table 3). For each season, trapping continued until no more buprestids were caught. In 2002, trapping ran in unison with the multi-colored test and began on 15 April and continued until 7 October (2 wk after last buprestid capture). Traps were established in a separate plot from the multi-colored test using 15 replicates per color treatment arranged in a completely randomized design using the same trap spacing, glue, and plot maintenance procedures previously described. All specimens were collected and processed, as previously described. In 2003, traps were established from early March through late September.

Table 3.

Mean color attributes (±SE); red to green (a*), blue to yellow (b*), chroma (C*), hue (h*), and lightness (L*) of each color used in the red color trapping tests

img-z4-2_737.gif

Trap Capture and Statistical Analyses

For both tests, all buprestid borers captured on traps were removed with forceps, positioned between a folded sheet of cut wax paper, and placed in a plastic sandwich bag labeled with the individual trap number and date. Specimens were stored frozen until they could be cleaned in Histo-Clear II (National Diagnostics, Atlanta, GA) for 24 h to remove glue, followed by 24 h in 90% ethyl alcohol to remove the Histo-Clear II. Buprestid specimens were pinned, labeled, and identified to species using Downie and Arnette (1995) with updates to the C. femorata species complex using Wellso and Manley (2007) in the fall of 2021. Specimens are currently housed in the NRC museum collection (codon TSRS; Evenhuis 2021). Data from both experiments were processed in the fall of 2021 when new funding allowed for an updated review of the museum collection.

For analysis, trap captures were grouped as total buprestids, genus Chrysobothris, or C. femorata complex. The Chrysobothris and C. femorata complex groupings allowed a greater analysis focus on effective traps for nursery pest species. Count data are usually fitted with a Poisson or negative binomial regression. The data were fitted to both a Poisson regression and a negative binomial regression model with a Likelihood Ratio Test to determine which model had the best fit for the data (Lawless 1987, Lewis et al. 2010, Putri et al. 2020). The Poisson regression had the best fit (P = 0.233), probably due to the equality of variance to the means in the models. For analysis of buprestid attraction to trap colors, a GLIMMIX procedure was used with the Poisson distribution to analyze the effects of color, with year as a random effect on trap collections. Data were pooled from the 2001 and 2002, and the 2002 and 2003 trap periods for the multi-colored tests and red-reflectance tests, respectively. The GLIMMIX models were followed with an LSD post hoc test for differences among colors in each of the respective tests. Chi-square tests with Holm's Sequential Bonferroni correction to control Type I error rate were used for evaluations of sex ratios across the three principal groups (buprestids, Chrysobothris genus, and C. femorata complex). Total counts of adult buprestids, Chrysobothris genus, and C. femorata complex groupings were compared by color and sex using Wilcoxon Rank Sum Tests, with Holm's Sequential Bonferroni correction to control Type I error rate. Pearson correlations were performed among color attributes (L*, a*, b*, C*, and h*and peak relative reflectance) and counts of females, males, and total (female and male) for buprestids, Chrysobothris, and members of the C. femorata complex. All analyses were performed in SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Results

Multi-Colored Sticky Traps

During the spring and summer of 2001 and 2002, red-colored sticky traps caught the greatest number of buprestids (Fig. 2; Tables 4 and 5,  Supp Tables 2 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s2.docx)– 4 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s4.docx) [online only]), but significant differences were not detected for capture numbers by year (random effect) (Table 4). In total, 19 buprestid species were captured in middle Tennessee with these traps (see species list in  Supp Tables 2 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s2.docx) and  3 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s3.docx) [online only]).

During 2001, analysis of all buprestids, Chrysobothris genus, and C. femorata complex indicated greater numbers captured on red traps than the other colors (i.e., blue, green, gray, white, and yellow; H = 40.4, 46.2, 34.0, df = 5, P < 0.001, respectively, also see  Supp Table 2 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s2.docx) [online only]). However, in the 2002 test, buprestids and Chrysobothris species were captured in higher numbers on red traps (H = 27.4, df = 5, P < 0.001 and H = 19.0, df = 5, P = 0.002, respectively) than any other colors, but no differences were detected for C. femorata complex due to low trap collections (H = 6.9, df = 5, P = 0.227).

The study also found differences in male and female sex ratios and response to trap colors. In both 2001 and 2002, females were caught in greater numbers than males (2001: 189 females vs 91 males; and 2002: 91 females vs 44 males; χ2 = 16.02, df = 0.36, P = 0.002 [2001 and 2002 pooled data]). For female and male buprestid beetles, Chrysobothris, and members of the C. femorata complex, a negative relationship to color lightness was found (r = –0.56, P = 0.039; r = –0.55, P = 0.021; r = –0.51, P = 0.022, respectively, Table 6). There was also a negative relationship between female buprestids, and females in C. femorata complex and lightness (r = –0.62, P < 0.001; r = –0.61, P < 0.001, respectively, Table 6), indicating that less reflective colors were preferred. Male beetles from the three categories did not share this relationship (all r range –0.36 to –0.43, P = 0.246, Table 6). Additionally, a strong positive correlation with a* (green to red color space axis) was found between buprestids (total, female, and male), Chrysobothris spp. (total, female, and male), and individuals in the C. femorata complex (total, female, and male) (all r > 0.69, P < 0.05, Table 6), indicating a strong preference for red over green.

Fig. 2.

Total counts of buprestids, Chrysobothris spp., and members of the Chrysobothris femorata complex caught in each color trap pooled from the 2001 and 2002 multi-color trapping tests. Each color was replicated 25 times (see Table 2 for color attribute qualities) from stock wall liner (see Methods for details). Different letters represent significant differences between numbers caught in a specific group, determined by an LSD post hoc analysis.

img-z5-2_737.jpg

Table 4.

Regression summary for GLIMMIX model of effect of color, year, and the interaction of color and year on buprestid, Chrysobothris, and members of the C. femorata complex

img-z5-8_737.gif

Table 5.

Mean (±SE) buprestids, Chrysobothris, and members of the C. femorata complex caught in multi-colored sticky traps in 2001 and 2002

img-AJhm_737.gif

Red-Reflecting Sticky Traps

As in the multi-colored trap test, buprestid trap collections varied among trap colors (Table 4). However, overall numbers of buprestids collected were greater than in the multi-colored test. In both years, purple traps (i.e., traps that reflected in the violet range with reflectance peaking at 405 nm on the electromagnetic spectrum) caught the greatest number of total buprestids, Chrysobothris, or C. femorata complex species (Fig. 3; Table 7;  Supp Table 5 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s5.docx)– 7 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s7.docx) [online only]), but there were significantly greater numbers of buprestids, Chrysobothris, and members in the C. femorata complex caught in 2002 than in 2003 ( Supp Tables 5 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s5.docx) and  6 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s6.docx) [online only]). Using various shades of red sticky traps, significantly more female buprestids were caught than males in 2002 and 2003 (2002: 339 females and 195 males; 2003: 266 females and 102 males; χ2 = 100.12, df = 0.38, P = 0.009 [2002 and 2003 pooled data]).

The strength of the correlation changed from year 2002 to 2003 in the relationship between individual color attributes and the number of beetles caught (Table 8). However, the total number of buprestids (total, female, and male), Chrysobothris spp. (total, female, and male), and members in the C. femorata complex (total, female, and male) had a strong negative correlation with b* (yellow to blue color axis) (r range –0.60 to –0.88, all P < 0.05, Table 8), indicating a preference for more blue reflectance. The strength of this relationship was greater for males than females of all three groups (males: r range –0.70 to –0.76, all P < 0.05; female r range –0.60 to –0.72, all P < 0.05). Additionally, strong positive correlations were found for male buprestids and total count (females and males) from the C. femorata complex with h* (hue) (r = 0.67, P = 0.001 and r = 0.58, P = 0.003, respectively, Table 8), indicating a preference for violet base colors (Tables 7;  Supp Tables 5 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s5.docx)– 7 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s7.docx) [online only]). Males from the C. femorata complex also were positively correlated with peak reflectance (r = 0.62, P = 0.024).

Table 6.

Pearson correlations (r) between color attributes red to green (a*), blue to yellow (b*), chroma (C*), hue (h*), lightness (L*), and peak reflectance (PR) and sex of buprestids, Chrysobothris spp., and members of the C. femorata complex in the multi-color trapping test

img-z6-2_737.gif

Fig. 3.

Total counts of buprestids, Chrysobothris spp., and members of the Chrysobothris femorata complex found in each color trap pooled from the 2002 and 2003 red-reflectance trapping tests. Each color was replicated 15 times (see Table 3 for color attribute qualities, and  Supp Table 1 (nvac038_suppl_supplementary_table_s1.docx) [online only] for details on paints used on wall liner). Different letters represent significant differences between numbers caught in a specific group, determined by an LSD post hoc analysis.

img-z6-4_737.jpg

Discussion

Ornamental tree nurseries are a commonly exploited larval habitat resource by Chrysobothris beetles. Research indicates Chrysobothris prefer to oviposit on the sunny sides of trees (Oliver et al. 2010, Seagraves et al. 2012, Dawadi et al. 2019). In lab observations, Chrysobothris adults are generally more active in sunny weather (C.L.P. and K.M.A., personal observations) and beetles in the genus are also less responsive to traps on cloudy days (J.B.O. and D.C.F., personal observations). Nursery rows allow more sunlight to reach trunk surfaces than in forested areas (Seagraves et al. 2012, Dawadi et al. 2019), potentially making nursery trees more attractive and suitable hosts. Management strategies need to account for mechanisms of host location in Chrysobothris to protect nursery trees. However, identifying host selection strategies in polyphagous insects can be difficult because of their ability to shift hosts based on local plant population densities and host quality (Jaenike 1990, Mitra et al. 2021). Because nurseries often plant the same species in designated blocks, they may provide a stronger host location signal (i.e., plant apparency, sensu Feeny 1976) than nearby forests that often have interspersed tree species.

Color Preference

It is important to note that the glue used may have had an impact on the reflectance and visual cues (Crook et al. 2009). However, Crook et al. (2009) found that the effect of glue was negligible. Furthermore, the spectrophotometer measurements were recorded with the SCI function, which negates surface appearance effects to measure true color. The conclusions from this study are therefore drawn with the assumption that differences in trap success are due to the base color of traps in these studies. In the multi-colored trapping test, genus Chrysobothris and buprestids at the family taxonomic level preferred a red trap color (peak reflectance of 700 nm) over other colors. For both the genus Chrysobothris and family Buprestidae levels, there was a strong, positive correlation with a*, indicating higher values of a* (i.e., more red reflectance) were more attractive traps. Contrastingly, in the red-reflectance test, the amount of red reflectance (indicated by positive a* values) in a trap color did not correlate with trap captures. Instead, trap collections indicate preferences of buprestids to purple (peak reflectance of 405 nm) and those in the genus Chrysobothris (including those in the C. femorata complex) also preferred purple in addition to other colors (i.e., magenta [peak reflectance of 670 nm] and medium pink [peak reflectance of 660 nm]).

There was a divergence in the importance of individual color attributes at the buprestid family, Chrysobothris genus, and C. femorata complex taxonomic levels as well as between females and males in these levels. In the red-reflectance test, all trap colors had high red reflectance (except for white) compared to trap colors in the multi-colored test. Additionally, in the red-reflectance test, all three taxonomic levels were negatively correlated with b*, suggesting a greater preference for traps with higher red and blue reflectance (i.e., hence the attraction to violet range colors like purple that fall in between red and blue on the L*a*b* color space sphere). Changes from color preferences in the two experiments have shown that the preference for blue reflectance is context-dependent on red reflectance. However, the opposite is not true, and red reflectance is consistently important, where blue alone is not. Interestingly, hue and peak reflectance become correlated with trap captures in the red-reflectance tests. Reflectance is the amount of light that is reflected off the surface of an object, whereas peak reflectance refers to the primary wavelength that is being reflected. Males from the C. femorata complex showed a strong, positive correlation with the peak reflectance (Table 8), indicating an attraction to reflectance at longer wavelengths (i.e., red reflectance). Males from all three groups also were positively correlated with hue (Table 8; i.e., preferred a purer red base color). Amongst females, the genus Chrysobothris was the only group analyzed that was not correlated with hue (Table 8). Hue refers to the origin of the color or the underlying base color. In the red-color test, the hue was red (dark pink, dark red, light pink, light red, medium pink, and red) or violet (magenta and purple) for most of the colors (excluding orange and white). C. femorata complex members were more attracted to violet hues, but males of this group were more sensitive to the peak reflectance from the hue red.

Table 7.

Mean (±SE) buprestids, Chrysobothris, and members of the C. femorata complex caught in red-reflectance sticky traps in 2002 and 2003

img-Aq16_737.gif

Table 8.

Pearson correlations (r) between color attributes red to green (a*), blue to yellow (b*), chroma (C*), hue (h*), lightness (L*), and peak reflectance (PR) and sex of buprestids, Chrysobothris spp., and members of the C. femorata complex in the red color trapping test

img-z7-5_737.gif

Using Ecological Theory to Understand Color Preference

Ecological theories on host selection and insect physiology can be used to inform research decisions on pest-management and trapping strategies of Chrysobothris in nurseries. The sequential cues hypothesis (SCH) suggests that generalist insects initially use common cues of potential hosts to locate a general host habitat (Bruce and Pickett 2011, Wang et al. 2017, Silva and Clarke 2019). Once they are within the preferred habitat, generalists may use more specific components of the common attractant cues to locate hosts or use a completely different set of cues to orient to preferred hosts (Agrawal 1998, Silva and Clarke 2019). The SCH may provide a framework to explain variations in buprestid preference for individual color attributes between the multicolor and red-reflectance tests. However, to apply host locating theory, affinity for specific color attributes must first be understood. The trap collection totals among the various colors tested provide indirect empirical evidence of affinity for certain color attributes.

General correlations exist between the ecology of beetle families and colors that provide spatial orientation within the environment (Atkinson et al. 1988, Safranyik et al. 2000, Francese et al. 2008). Many studies have been successful at trapping wood boring beetles with red lures (e.g., Kerr et al. 2017, Cavaletto et al. 2021). Congruent with SCH, red reflectance may function as a common cue for locating potential host habitats because red reflectance increases in stressed plants (Carter and Estep 2002), which are often preferred hosts of buprestid beetles (Potter et al. 1988, McCullough et al. 2009). However, once inside a potential habitat, Chrysobothris and buprestids in general may be using a wider range of visual cues to locate specific host plants based on their ecological niche. For example, Poland et al. (2019) found that emerald ash borer females were attracted to purple traps hung in tree canopies whereas males were attracted to green traps. Male foraging and mate seeking behaviors may explain the attraction to green because males often forage and find females in tree canopies.

The attraction to purple cannot easily be explained by naturally occurring cues such as host or mate coloration. Some studies have suggested that the reflectance of bark (common hosts of buprestids) reflects similarly to the purple color cues (see Prokopy and Owens 1983, Strom et al. 1999, Campbell and Borden 2005). Many visual stimulants are associated with mate- or host-seeking behaviors (Wynne et al. 2020, van der Kooi et al. 2021). Polyphagous insects have evolved the ability to use color as a visual cue during mate and host selection (Bernays 2001), but their ability to process visual stimuli is limited to a specific range of the optical spectrum (Chittka and Menzel 1992, Kevan et al. 1996). Insect opsins, which are protein-coupled receptors, allow for the expression of sensitivity to various wavelengths. Opsins for detecting long wavelengths (infra-reds) are commonly found in many insect orders, but opsins that produce a sensitivity to short wavelengths (ultra-violet) are much less common (Briscoe and Chittka 2001, Porter et al. 2007). Recent studies of coleopteran opsin classes show a great diversity of spectral sensitivities (Crook et al. 2009, Jiang et al. 2014, Lord et al. 2016), and the buprestid family demonstrate unique opsin expressions that allow them to detect ultra-violet wavelengths (340 nm) (Crook et al. 2009, Lord et al. 2016). Furthermore, Lord et al. (2016) found that buprestids express multiple opsin classes resulting in many peak color sensitivities, suggesting visual stimulation from multiple colors. Ecological factors that favored the evolution of opsin adaptations are still not understood but other studies have produced similar results as this study, suggesting that buprestid beetles use multiple reflectance peaks as visual stimuli (e.g., Francese et al. 2010). Francese et al. (2010) noted that a blue color in their study had similar reflectance (430 nm) to the purple paint color used in this study but concluded the 3 times higher trap capture rate of emerald ash borer on the ‘TSU purple’ color was due to an additional visible red reflectance peak in the 650–670 nm range. Thus, Francese et al. (2010) findings were similar to this study in that both red and blue colors enhanced buprestid trap response, but blue alone was much less attractive.

To understand why purple is an attractant, more research is needed to analyze ecological pressures that caused adaptations to specific visual cues and stimulants. It may be that purple is reflected by conspecific Chrysobothris. If light reflects into the ultra-violet spectra (340 nm) from Chrysobothris elytra, this suggests that the attraction to purple is related to mate-selecting behaviors. For example, Crook et al. (2009) found that female emerald ash borers responded to ultra-violet wavelengths (340 nm) and that the abdomens of male emerald ash borers peaked in reflectance in the same spectral region. There are no studies that have investigated the reflection of Chrysobothris anatomy.

Possible Secondary Cues for Host Selection Inside the Suitable Environment

The SCH model suggests there may be additional environmental cues that are used by generalist herbivores, such as Chrysobothris, after locating a suitable host habitat. Other environmental cues may include plant volatile emissions (kairomones) or localized visual stimulants that are required for a beetle to orient to a potential host (Pierik et al. 2014). Several studies have had success trapping other buprestid beetles by using a combination of visual and olfactory plant stimuli (Ryall et al. 2013, Ryall 2015, Silk et al. 2020). Alternatively, these cues may not necessarily be related to host detection (Pureswaran and Poland 2009, Domingue et al. 2011, Silk et al. 2011). Beetles also may use conspecific visual, audio, and olfactory stimuli (Fenton and Maxwell 1937, Pureswaran and Poland 2009, Silk and Ryall 2015). For example, Chrysobothris oviposition occurs on the sunny sides of trees (Oliver et al. 2010, Seagraves et al. 2012, Dawadi et al. 2019), and groups of beetles have been observed flying above exposed wood piles. Sunlight reflected from conspecifics may act as a visual cue, guiding individuals to specific tree hosts. Additionally, C. femorata display an abdomen tapping behavior (Fenton and Maxwell 1937), and this audible tapping may act as a signal to conspecifics once beetles have entered a suitable habitat. Olfactory stimuli also have been observed in host-locating behaviors (e.g., Grant et al. 2011, Silk et al. 2011). Silk et al. (2011) found that a volatile pheromone emitted by emerald ash borer increased attraction to specific host foliar volatiles. Female and male emerald ash borers were successfully trapped using different combinations of visual and olfactory cues (Grant et al. 2011), indicating secondary visual stimuli may also influence beetle preference.

Additional visual or olfactory cues may be necessary to increase the attractiveness of traps monitoring Chrysobothris in nursery systems. Our study lays a foundation to improve trap optimization for Chrysobothris in nursery settings. Pest management strategies that incorporate traps with a violet base color should see improved Chrysobothris captures, particularly females. Further research is needed to elucidate the relationship between violet hue, female Chrysobothris behavior and physiology. For now, there are commercially available plastic traps that are used for emerald ash borer (as described in Francese et al. 2010; 2013a,b) that are similar in color as the purple used in the red-reflectance tests of this study. However, additional spectral analysis of hosts, habitats and conspecific anatomy may identify a source of selective pressure responsible for the observed sex differences in Chrysobothris visual preferences. Such information will both increase our understanding of the complex nature of color preference in wood boring beetles and aid in the development of better tools to manage them.

Acknowledgments

We thank Benji Moore, Sue Scholl, Gary Clendenon, Edgar Davis, Adam Newby, and Steve Mullican (USDA-ARS National Arboretum) and Crystal Lemings, Caleb West, Jason Blalock, and Jessie Basham (Tennessee State University [TSU]) for their assistance in conducting these trials and the TSU Otis Floyd Nursery Research Center, McMinnville, TN, for allowing us to conduct these tests on nursery grounds. We thank Richard Westcott, Entomologist Emeritus, Oregon Department of Agriculture, Plant Division, Entomology Museum 635 Capitol NE Salem, OR 97301-2532, for his help in identifying beetles. We also thank the Horticultural Research Institute Project (No. 1029). This work is also supported by Specialty Crop Research Initiative (2020-51181-32199) from the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture, the Tennessee Nursery and Landscape Association, USDA-APHIS, and the USDA-NIFA Evans-Allen. Any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the view of the USDA. Trade names mentioned are for informational purposes only and do not imply an endorsement by TSU or USDA.

Data Availability

Data will be made available upon acceptance of manuscript.

Supplementary Data

Supplementary data are available at Environmental Entomology online.

References Cited

1.

Acheampong, S., G. M. G. Zilahi-Balogh, G. J. R. Judd, and T. Dimaria. 2016. Pacific flatheaded borer, Chrysobothris mali Horn (Coleoptera: Buprestidae), found attacking apple saplings in the Southern Interior of British Columbia. J. Entomol. Soc. Brit. Columbia. 113: 71–73. Google Scholar

2.

Agrawal, A. A. 1998. Induced responses to herbivory and increased plant performance. Science. 279: 1201–1202. Google Scholar

3.

Andrews, H. E., K. Transue, K. Mendonca, and N. G. Wiman. 2017. Rising hazelnut acreage could increase area wide pest pressure from wood boring beetles Pacific flatheaded borer Chrysobothris mali Horn, and the ambrosia beetles Anisandrus dispar (Fabricius) and Xyleborinus saxesenii (Ratzeburg). In Proceedings, 2017 Annual Meeting of the Entomological Society of America Pacific Branch, 2-5 April 2017, Lanham, MD. Google Scholar

4.

Atkinson, T. H., J. L. Foltz, and M. D. Connor. 1988. Flight patterns of phloem- and wood-boring Coleoptera (Scolytidae, Platypopidae, Curculionidae, Buprestidae, Cerambycidae) in North Florida slash pine plantation. Environ. Entomol. 17: 259–265. Google Scholar

5.

Baspinar, H., D. Doll, and J. Rijal. 2018. Pest management in organic almond, pp. 328–347. In V. Vacante and S. Kreiter (eds.), Handbook of pest management in organic farming. CAB International, Boston, MA. Google Scholar

6.

Beers, E. H. 2017. Sustainable arthropod management for apples, pp. 449–484. In K. Evans (ed.), Achieving sustainable cultivation of apples. Burleigh Dodds Science Publishing, London, UK. Google Scholar

7.

Bellamy, C., and M. Volkovitsh. 2016. Buprestodea Crowson, 1955, pp. 543–552. In R. Beutel, and R. Leschen (eds.), Coleoptera, Beetles. Morphology and Systematics. De Gruyter, Boston, MA. Google Scholar

8.

Bernays, E. A. 2001. Neural limitations in phytophagous insects: implications for diet breadth and evolution of host affiliation. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46: 703–727. Google Scholar

9.

Briscoe, A. D., and L. Chittka. 2001. Evolution of color vision in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 46: 471571–471510. Google Scholar

10.

Bruce, T. J. A., and J. A. Pickett. 2011. Perception of plant volatile blends by herbivorous insects—finding the right mix. Phytochem. 72: 1605–1611. Google Scholar

11.

Burke, H. E., and A. G. Boving. 1929. The Pacific flathead borer. U.S. Department of Agric. Tech. Bull. No. 83. U.S. Gov. Printing Office. Washington, DC. Google Scholar

12.

Campbell, S. A., and J. H. Borden. 2005. Bark reflectance spectra of conifers and angiosperms: implications for host discrimination by coniferophagus bark and timber beetles. Can. Entomol. 137: 719–722. Google Scholar

13.

Carter, G.A., and L. Estep. 2002. General spectral characteristics of leaf reflectance responses to plant stress and their manifestation at the landscape scale, pp. 271–293. In R.S. Muttiah (ed.), From laboratory spectroscopy to remotely sensed spectra of terrestrial ecosystems. Springer, Dordrecht, NL. Google Scholar

14.

Cavaletto, G., M. Faccoli, L. Marini, J. Spaethe, G. Magnani, and D. Rassati. 2020. Effect of trap color on captures of bark- and wood-boring beetles (Coleoptera; Buprestidae and Scolytinae) and associated predators. Insects. 11: 749e749. Google Scholar

15.

Cavaletto, G., F. Massimo, L. Marini, J. Spaethe, F. Giannone, S. Moino, and D. Rassati. 2021. Exploiting trap color to improve surveys of longhorn beetles. J. Pest Sci. 94: 871–883. Google Scholar

16.

Chittka, L., and R. Menzel. 1992. The evolutionary adaptation of flower colours and the insect pollinators' colour vision. J. Comp. Physiol. A. 171: 171–181. Google Scholar

17.

Crook, D. J., J. A. Francese, K. E. Zylstra, I. Fraser, A. J. Sawyer, D. W. Bartels, D. R. Lance, and V. C. Mastro. 2009. Laboratory and field response of the emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae), to selected regions of the electromagnetic spectrum. For. Entomol. 102: 2160–2169. Google Scholar

18.

Dawadi, S., J. B. Oliver, P. O'Neal, and K. M. Addesso. 2019. Impact of cover cropping on non-target arthropod pests of red maple trees in nursery production. Fla. Entomol. 102: 187–193. Google Scholar

19.

Domingue, M. J., G. Csoka, M. Toth, G. Vetek, B. Penzes, V. Mastro, and T. C. Baker. 2011. Field observations of visual attraction of three European oak buprestid beetles toward conspecific and heterospecific models. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 140: 112–121. Google Scholar

20.

Domingue, M. J., J. P. Lelito, A. J. Myrick, G. Csóka, L. Szöcs, Z. Imrei, and T. C. Baker. 2016. Differences in spectral selectivity between stages of visually guided mating approaches in a buprestid beetle. J. Exper. Biol. 219: 2837–2843. Google Scholar

21.

Downie, N. M., and R. H. Arnette, Jr. 1995. The beetles of northeastern North America, vol. 1. Sandhill Crane Press, Gainesville, FL. Google Scholar

22.

Evenhuis, N.L. 2021. The insect and spider collections of the world website. Available from  http://hbs.bishopmuseum.org/codens/ . Accessed 29 Nov. 2021. Google Scholar

23.

Fare, D. 2015. Field production. In Proceedings, Southern Nursery Research Conference, vol. 60. Google Scholar

24.

Feeny, P. 1976. Plant apparency and chemical defense, pp. 1–40. In Rhoades et al. (eds.), Biochemical interaction between plants and insects. Springer, Boston, MA. Google Scholar

25.

Fenton, F. A., and J. W. Maxwell. 1937. Flat-headed apple tree borer in Oklahoma. J. Econ. Entomol. 30: 748–750. Google Scholar

26.

Francese, J. A., V. C. Mastro, J. B. Oliver, D. R. Lance, N. Youssef, and S. G. Lavalle. 2005. Evaluation of colors for trapping Agrilus planipennis (Coleoptera: Buprestidae). J. Entomol. Sci. 40: 93–95. Google Scholar

27.

Francese, J. A., J. B. Oliver, I. Fraser, D. R. Lance, N. Youssef, A. J. Sawyer, and V. C. Mastro. 2008. Influence of trap placement and design on capture of emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 101: 1831–1837. Google Scholar

28.

Francese, J. A., D. J. Crook, I. Fraser, D. R. Lance, A. J. Sawyer, and V. C. Mastro. 2010. Optimization of trap color for emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae). J. Econ. Entomol. 103: 1235–1241. Google Scholar

29.

Francese, J. A., M. L. Rietz, D. J. Crook, I. Fraser, D. R. Lance, and V. C. Mastro. 2013a. Improving detection tools for the emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae): comparison of prism and multifunnel traps at varying population densities. Environ. Entomol. 106: 2407–2414. Google Scholar

30.

Francese, J. A., M. L. Rietz, and V. C. Mastro. 2013b. Optimization of multifunnel traps for emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae): influence of size, trap, coating, and color. J. Econ. Entomol. 106: 2415–2423. Google Scholar

31.

Fürstenau, B., C. Quero, J. M. Riba, G. Rosell, and A. Guerrero. 2015. Field trapping of the flathead borer Coroebus undatus (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) with different traps and volatile lures. Insect Sci. 22: 139–149. Google Scholar

32.

Grant, G. C., T. M. Poland, T. Ciaramitaro, D. B. Lyons, and G. C. Jones. 2011. Comparison of male and female emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) responses to phoebe oil and (Z)-3-hexanol lures in light green prism traps. J. Econ. Entomol. 104: 173–179. Google Scholar

33.

Haack, R. A., and T. R. Petrice. 2019. Historical population increases and related inciting factors of Agrilus anxius, Agrilus bilineatus, and Agrilus granulatus (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) in the Lake States (Michigan, Minnesota, and Wisconsin). Gt. Lakes Entomol. 52: 21–33. Google Scholar

34.

Haack, R. A., E. Jendek, H. Liu, L. Houping, K. R. Marchant, T. R. Petrice, T. M. Poland, and H. Ye. 2002. The emerald ash borer: a new exotic pest in North America , vol. 47. Newsletter of the Michigan Entomological Society, Ann Arbor, MI, pp. 1–5. Google Scholar

35.

Haack, R. A., K. O. Britton, E. G. Brockerhoff, J. F. Cavey, L. J. Garrett, M. Kimberley, F. Lowenstein, A. Nuding, L. J. Olson, and J. Turner , et al. 2014. Effectiveness of the International Phytosanitary Standard ISPM No. 15 on reducing wood borer infestation rates in wood packaging material entering the United States. PLoS One. 9: e96611. Google Scholar

36.

Imrei, Z., Z. Lohonayii, G. Csoka, J. Muskovits, S. Szanyi, M. Toth, and M. J. Domingue. 2020a. Improving trapping methods for buprestid beetles to enhance monitoring of native and invasive species. Forestry. 93: 254–264. Google Scholar

37.

Imrei, Z., Z. Lohonayii, G. Muskovits, E. Matula, J. Vuts, J. Fail, P. J. L. Gould, M. A. Birkett, M. Toth, and M. J. Domingue. 2020b. Developing a non-sticky trap design for monitoring jewel beetles. J. Appl. Entomol. 144: 224–231. Google Scholar

38.

Jaenike, J. 1990. Host specialization in phytophagous insects. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 21: 243–273. Google Scholar

39.

Jiang, Y., Y. Guo, Y. Wu, T. Li, Y. Duan, J. Miao, G. Zhong-jun, and H. Zhi-juan. 2014. Spectral sensitivity of the compound eyes of Anomala corpulenta Motschulsky (Coleoptera: Scarabaeoidea). J. Int. Agri. 14: 706–713. Google Scholar

40.

Kerr, J. L., K. D. Bader, and E. G. Brockerhoff. 2017. Olfactory cues, visual cues, and semiochemical diversity interact during host location by invasive forest beetles. J. Chem. Ecol. 43: 17–25. Google Scholar

41.

Kevan, P., M. Giurfa, and L. Chittka. 1996. Why are there so many and so few white flowers? Trends in Plant Sci . 1: e252. Google Scholar

42.

van der Kooi, C. J., D. G. Stavenga, K. Arikawa, G. Belusic, and A. Kelber. 2021. Evolution of insect color vision: from spectral sensitivity to visual ecology. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 66: 435–461. Google Scholar

43.

Lawless, J. F. 1987. Negative binomial and mixed Poisson regression. Can. J. Stat. 15: 209–225. Google Scholar

44.

Lebude, A., and C. Adkins. 2014. Incidence and severity of buprestid infestation in field-grown Acer platanoides related to cardinal orientation of understock bud union. J. Environ. Hort. 4: 215–218. Google Scholar

45.

Lewis, F., A. Butler, and L. Gilbert. 2010. A unified approach to model selection using the likelihood ratio test. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2: 155–162. Google Scholar

46.

Lord, N. P., R. L. Plimpton, C. R. Sharkey, A. Suvorov, J. P. Lelito, B. M. Willardson, and S. M. Bybee. 2016. A cure for the blues: opsin duplication and subfunctionalization for short-wavelength sensitivity in jewel beetles (Coleoptera: Buprestidae). BMC Evol. Biol. 107: 1–17. Google Scholar

47.

McCullough, D. G., T. M. Poland, and D. Cappaert. 2009. Attraction of emerald ash borer to ash trees stressed by girdling, herbicide treatment, or wounding. Can. J. For. Res. 39: 1331–1345. Google Scholar

48.

Meglic, A., M. Illic, C. Quero, K. Arikawa, and G. Belusic. 2020. Two chiral types of randomly rotated ommatidia are distributed across the retina of the flathead oak borer Coraebus undatus (Coleoptera: Buprestidae). J. Exp. Biol. 223: e225920. Google Scholar

49.

Mitra, S., D. M. Firake, K. P. Umesh, P. P. Pandey, and S. Pandit. 2021. Polyphagous caterpillars of Spodoptera litura switch from trap crop to the main crop, improve fitness, and shorten generation time. J. Pest Sci. 94: 1091–1103. Google Scholar

50.

Moraal, L. G., and J. Hilszczanski. 2000. The oak buprestid beetle, Agrilus biguttatus (F.) (Col., Buprestidae), a recent factor in oak decline in Europe. J. Pest Sci. 73: 134–138. Google Scholar

51.

Nitzu, E., I. Dobrin, M. Dumbrava, and M. Gutue. 2016. The range expansion of Ovalisia festiva (Linneaus, 1767) (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) in Eastern Europe and its potential for cpressaceae. Trav. du Mus. Natl. Hist. Nat. 58: 51–57. Google Scholar

52.

Oliver, J. B., D. C. Fare, N. Youssef, S. S. Scholl, M. E. Reding, C. M. Ranger, J. J. Moyseenko, and M. A. Holcomb. 2010. Evaluation of single application of neonicotinoid and multi-application contact insecticides for flatheaded borer management in field grown red maple cultivars. J. Environ. Hort. 28: 135–149. Google Scholar

53.

Petrice, T. R., and R. A. Haack. 2015. Comparison of different trap colors and types for capturing adult Agrilus (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) and other buprestids. Gt. Lakes Entomol. 48: 45–66. Google Scholar

54.

Petrice, T. R., R. A. Haack, and T. M. Poland. 2013. Attraction of Agrilus planipennis (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) and other buprestids to sticky traps of various colors and shapes. Gt. Lakes Entomol. 46: 13–30. Google Scholar

55.

Pierik, R., C. L. Ballare, and M. Dicke. 2014. Ecology of plant volatiles: taking a plant community perspective. Plant Cell Environ. 37: 1845–1853. Google Scholar

56.

Poland, T. M., T. R. Petrice, and T. M. Ciaramitaro. 2019. Trap designs, colors, and lures for emerald ash borer detection. Front. For. Glob. Change. 2: e80. Google Scholar

57.

Porter, M. L., T. W. Cronin, D. A. McClellan, and K. A. Crandall. 2007. Molecular characterization of crustacean visual pigments and the evolution of pancrustacean origin. Mol. Biol. Evol. 24: 253–268. Google Scholar

58.

Potter, D., G. M. Timmons, and F. C. Gordon. 1988. Flatheaded apple tree borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) in nursery-grown red maples: phenology of emergence, treatment timing, and response to stressed trees. J. Environ. Hort. 6: 18–22. Google Scholar

59.

Prokopy, R. J., and E. D. Owens. 1983. Visual detection of plants by herbivorous insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 28: 337–364. Google Scholar

60.

Pureswaran, D. S., and T. M. Poland. 2009. Host selection and feeding preference of Agrilus planipennis (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) on ash (Fraxinus spp.). Environ. Entomol. 38: 757–765. Google Scholar

61.

Putri, G. N., S. Nurrohmah, and I. Fithriani. 2020. Comparing Poisson-inverse Gaussian model and negative binomial model on case study: horseshoe crabs data. J. Physics: Conf. Ser. 1442: e012028. Google Scholar

62.

Rassati, D., L. Marini, M. Marchioro, P. Rapuzzi, G. Magnani, R. Poloni, F. Di Giovanni, P. Mayo, and J. Sweeney. 2019. Developing trapping protocols for wood-boring beetles associated with broadleaf trees. J. Pest Sci. 92: 267–279. Google Scholar

63.

Redilla, K., and D. McCullough. 2017. Species assemblage of buprestid beetle in four hardwood cover types in Michigan. Can. J. For. Res. 47: 1131–1139. Google Scholar

64.

Rutledge, C. E. 2020. Preliminary studies on using emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) monitoring tools for bronze birch borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) detection and management. Forestry. 93: 297–304. Google Scholar

65.

Ryall, K. 2015. Detection and sampling of emerald ash borer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) infestations. Can. Entomol. 147: 290–299. Google Scholar

66.

Ryall, K., J. G. Fidgen, P. J. Silk, and T. A. Scarr. 2013. Efficacy of the pheromone (3Z)-lactone and the host kairomone (3Z)-hexenol at detecting early infestation of the emerald ash borer, Agrilus planipennis. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 147: 126–131. Google Scholar

67.

Safranyik, L., D. A. Linton, and T. L. Shore. 2000. Temporal and vertical distribution of bark beetles (Coleoptera: Scolytidae) captured in barrier traps at baited and unbaited lodgepole pines the year following an attack by the mountain pine beetle. Can. Entomol. 132: 799–810. Google Scholar

68.

Seagraves, B. L., C. T. Redmond, and D. A. Potter. 2012. Relative resistance or susceptibility of maple (Acer) species, hybrids and cultivars to six arthropod pests of production nurseries. Pest Manag. Sci. 69: 112–119. Google Scholar

69.

Silk, P., and K. Ryall. 2015. Semiochemistry and chemical ecology of the emerald ash borer Agrilus planipennis (Coleoptera: Buprestidae). Can. Entomol. 147: 277–289. Google Scholar

70.

Silk, P., K. Ryall, P. Mayo, M. A. Lemay, G. Grant, D. Crook, A. Cosse, I. Fraser, J. D. Sweeney, and D. B. Lyons , et al. 2011. Evidence for a volatile pheromone in Agrilus planipennis Fairmaire (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) that increases attraction to a host foliar volatile. Environ. Entomol. 40: 904–916. Google Scholar

71.

Silk, P., K. Ryall, and L. Roscoe. 2020. Emerald ash borer, Agrilus planipennis (Coleoptera: Buprestidae), detection and monitoring in Canada. Int. J. For. Res. 93: 273–279. Google Scholar

72.

Silva, R., and A. R. Clarke. 2019. The ‘sequential cues hypothesis’: a concept model to explain host location and ranking by polyphagous herbivores. Insect Sci. 27: 1136–1147. Google Scholar

73.

Strom, B., and R. Goyer. 2001. Effect of silhouette color on trap catches of Dendroctonus frontalis (Coleoptera: Scolytidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am. 94: 948–953. Google Scholar

74.

Strom, B. L., L. M. Roton, R. A. Goyer, and J. R. Meeker. 1999. Visual and semiochemicals disruption of host finding in the southern pine beetle. Ecol. Appl. 9: 1028–1038. Google Scholar

75.

Tong, Y., H. Yang, J. Shaw, X. Yang, and M. Bai. 2021. The relationship between genus/species richness and morphological diversity among subfamilies of jewel beetles. Insects. 12: 1–15. Google Scholar

76.

Varandi, H. B., M. Kalashian, and S. A. R. Taleshi. 2018. The diversity of wood-boring beetles caught by different traps in northern forests of Iran. Trop. Dry. 2: 65–74. Google Scholar

77.

Voss, D. H. 1992. Relating colorimeter measurement of plant color to the Royal Horticultural Society Colour Chart. Hort. Sci. 27: 1256–1260. Google Scholar

78.

Voss, D. H. 2002. The Royal Horticultural Society Colour Chart 2001Google Scholar

79.

Wang, Y., Y. Ma, D. S. Zhou, S. X. Gao, X. C. Zhao, Q. B. Tang, C. Z. Wang, and J. A. Joop. 2017. Higher plasticity in feeding preference of generalist than a specialist: experiments with two closely related Helicoverpa spp. Sci. Rep. 7: 1–12. Google Scholar

80.

Wellso, S. G., and G. V. Manley. 2007. A revision of the Chrysobothris femorata (Olivier, 1790) species group from North America, north of Mexico (Coleoptera: Buprestidae). Zootaxa. 1652: 1–26. Google Scholar

81.

Werle, C. T., A. M. Bray, J. B. Oliver, E. K. Blythe, and B. J. Simpson. 2014. Ambrosia beetle (Coleoptera: Curculionidae: Scolytinae) captures using colored traps in southeast Tennessee and south Mississippi. J. Entomol. Sci. 49: 373–382. Google Scholar

82.

Westcott, R. L., W. Williams, and G. Kohler. 2018. Chrysobothris rugosiceps Melsheimer (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) found in Washington State. Insecta Mundi 0653: 1–3. Google Scholar

83.

Wiman, N., H. Andrews, A. Mugica, E. Rudolph, and T. Chase. 2019. Pacific flatheaded borer ecology and knowledge gaps in western Oregon orchard crops, pp. 28–30. In J. Oliver and K. Addesso (eds.), Proceedings of the Flatheaded Borer Workshop, 1–2 July 2019. McMinnville, TN. Available from  https://southernipm.org/partners/working-groups/wood-borers/flatheaded-borer/ . Accessed 3 June 2022. Google Scholar

84.

Woodiel, N. L. 1979. The appearance of the flat-headed apple tree borer in maples in Tennessee. Proc. Southern Nur. Assoc. Res. Conf. 24: 97. Google Scholar

85.

Wynne, N. E., M. G. Lorenzo, and C. Vinauger. 2020. Mechanism and plasticity of vectors' host-seeking behavior. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 40: 1–5. Google Scholar

86.

Zhi-Zhi, F., Z. Zhang, and S. F. Zhang. 2021. A review of study on olfaction and vision of Agrilus planipennis Fairmaire. For. Res. 34: 166–174. Google Scholar
© The Author(s) 2022. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of Entomological Society of America.
Cynthia L. Perkovich, Karla M. Addesso, Joshua P. Basham, Donna C. Fare, Nadeer N. Youssef, and Jason B. Oliver "Effects of Color Attributes on Trap Capture Rates of Chrysobothris femorata (Coleoptera: Buprestidae) and Related Species," Environmental Entomology 51(4), 737-746, (28 June 2022). https://doi.org/10.1093/ee/nvac038
Received: 26 January 2022; Accepted: 13 April 2022; Published: 28 June 2022
KEYWORDS
beetle
flatheaded borer
reflectance
sequential cues hypothesis
trapping method
Back to Top