Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 December 2013 First Report of the Mating Behavior of a Species of Frog-Biting Midge (Diptera: Corethrellidae)
Priyanka De Silva, Ximena E. Bernal
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Swarming is a common mating behavior present throughout Diptera and, in particular, in species of lower flies (Nematocerous Diptera). Mating aggregations have been observed in the wild and in laboratory colonies of mosquitoes (Culicidae) and phantom midges (Chaoboridae), but have been assumed to be absent in their sister taxon, the frog-biting midges (Corethrellidae). Corethrellidae is a monogeneric family that includes over 100 species of frog-biting midges (Corethrella Coquillett spp.). In contrast to mosquitoes, female frog-biting midges find their host by eavesdropping on the mating calls of anurans to obtain a blood meal for egg development. Here we report the previously unknown mating swarms of frog- biting midges based on a laboratory study of Corethrella appendiculata Grabham. Contrary to previous speculations, we report that frog-biting midges aggregate in mating swarms. We thoroughly characterize such formations describing size and duration of the aggregation, sex ratio of the swarm, flight patterns of individual midges, influence of light intensity on the formation of swarms and use of swarm markers. In addition, we determine pairing and copulation patterns. Males and females copulate in venter to venter position. During the swarm, females and males meet in the air and most pairs fall together out of the aggregation onto the floor. We confirmed that virgin females are inseminated at the swarm excluding alternative hypotheses to explain the aggregations. Overall, we reveal the mating behavior of frog-biting midges for the first time filling critical gaps in information about the reproductive biology of this family.

The mating behavior of given species is a unique, dynamic suite of interactions between the sexes. Several factors including ecological conditions prevailing in an animal's habitat and the animal's behavioral attributes determine the characteristics of the mating behavior of a given species (Emlen & Oring 1977; Thornhill et al. 1983). Understanding the mating behavior of a species is particularly important because it reflects the result of natural selection on mate choice, and ultimately on strategies for maximizing individual reproductive success. One of the major challenges encountered by sexually reproducing organisms is finding an individual of the opposite sex to court and ultimately mate. One strategy that has evolved to solve this problem is that members of one sex, usually the males, aggregate in a particular area to which the opposite sex is attracted. Such male aggregations are widespread across taxonomic groups and have been described in vertebrates (birds: Lill 1974; mammals: Bradbury & Vehrencamp 1977; amphibians: Arak 1983; fish: McKaye et al. 1990) and invertebrate species (insects: Snead & Alcock 1985; reviewed in Hoglund & Alatalo 1995; Shuster & Wade 2003).

Within insects, Diptera are different from most other insects in their mating behavior because of their ability to mate aerially, in swarms (reviewed by Downes 1969). Males of aerial swarming Diptera usually gather in discrete dancing swarms that use visual cues or landmarks as markers in the environment to determine swarm location (Nielsen et al. 1960; Downes 1969; Sullivan 1981; Blackwell et al. 1992; Clements 1999). Such swarms are seen in infraorders of nematocerous Diptera. In the Culicomorpha, of which Corethrel- lidae is a member family, aerial mating swarms are known for many species of Ceratopogonidae (Downes 1955; Downes 1958; Campbell & Kettle 1979; Zimmerman et al. 1982; Blackwell et al. 1992), Chaoboridae (Parma 1971; Moore 1986; Irvine 2000), Chironomidae (Gibson 1945; Young 1969; Oliver 1971; Paasivirta 1972; Kon 1987; Fyodorova & Azovsky 2003), Culicidae (Horsfall 1955; Nielsen et al. 1960; Downes 1969; Reisen et al. 1977; Clements 1999), and Simuliidae (Moor- house & Colbo 1973; Hunter 1979). Such swarms are often found at characteristic sites, presumably guided by a visual marker (Marchand 1984; Yuval & Bouskila 1993; Charlwood et al. 2002).

Although there is ample knowledge about the mating behavior of most species of Culicomor- pha, no studies have investigated the mating and swarming behavior of Corethrellidae (frog-biting midges). This monogeneric family (Corethrelli- dae, Diptera) is an ancient group of small eavesdropping flies (oldest fossils are 122 million years old), which includes over 100 described species (Borkent 2008; Borkent & Grafe 2012). Frog- biting midges, are the sister taxon to phantom midges (Chaoboridae) + true mosquitoes (Culicidae) (Borkent 1989). Females of most species of the Corethrellidae are haematophagous and feed on anuran blood to obtain protein for egg development (Williams & Edman 1968; McKeever 1977; McKeever & French 1991). Some, at least, are facultatively autogenous and one species from Australia has reduced mouthparts and is, therefore, fully autogenous. Different from most other haematophagous insects, however, female Cor- ethrella use the mating calls of male anurans to find their hosts (McKeever 1977; Bernal et al. 2006; Borkent 2008; Grafe et al 2008). No information is known, however, about the mating behaviors of species in this group. Although it has been suggested that no mating swarms are formed in this family (Borkent 2008; Yuval & Bouskila 1993), no critical investigation has been pursued to date.

In this study we investigate the mating behavior of laboratory reared Corethrella appen- diculata Grabham to address the following questions: (i) does this species form aerial swarms during mating? (ii) what are the characteristics of mating swarms? and, (iii) what cues are used to initiate mating swarms? To our knowledge this study is the first attempt to investigate the mating behavior of frog-biting midges. We provide novel information about the reproductive biology of frog-biting-midges, a group receiving recent increased attention in recent years (Bernal et al. 2006; Borkent 2008; Borkent & Grafe 2012; de Silva et al in review).

Materials and Methods

Study Species

We obtained frog-biting midges from the C. appendiculata colony maintained at Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas (Bernal Lab, Department of Biological Sciences). Corethrella appen- diculata is facultatively haematophagous and thus females do not require a blood meal to lay their first batch of eggs. An initial batch of larvae to initiate the colony was obtained from Phil Lounibos at the University of Florida and reared to adulthood. Larvae were reared in white plastic trays (34.3 × 25.4 × 3.8 cm) filled with 800 mL distilled water. Developing larvae were kept in low densities (50 larvae per tray) and fed with live nematodes (Panagrellus redivivus L.; Rhabditi- da: Panagrolaimidae) twice a week following the methods described by Haramis & Foster (1983) to produce nearly synchronized emergence of uniformly-sized adults. Pupae were collected from the larval trays into clear vials and transferred to holding cages (BioQuip lightweight aluminum collapsible cages; 46 × 46 × 46 cm; 2 × 2mm mesh size) for adult emergence. Adults were reared at 27 ± 1 °C, 70–80% RH, and 10:14 h L:D. Midge cages were kept inside an environmental chamber (Power Scientific Inc, model DROS28SSD, LEVEL 2) where adults were provided continuous access to 10% sucrose solution from wicks.

Mating Behavior of the Midges

We used a four-camera infrared video system (Zmodo PKD-DK4216-500GB H.264) to monitor the behavior of the midges in the frog-biting midge colony. The activities of the midges were video recorded 24 h/day for 1 month to examine their mating behavior (7 Jun –7 Jul 2012). We characterized the mating swarms of frog-biting midges by analyzing their aggregations and flight pattern. We also examined the influence of light intensity on swarm formation. To do so, we gradually changed the light intensity inside the environmental chamber. The light intensity level was changed from standard colony light conditions (185 l×) to darkness in 5 steps : (i) 185 l× (darkest storm clouds midday), (ii) 67 l× (family living room light), (iii) 31 l× (fully overcast sunset), (iv) 2 l× (moon light), and (v) 0 l× (complete darkness) (Schlyter 2006; Pears 1998). At each light level, the light conditions were maintained constant for 12 min using a light transformer dimmer (Lutron Skylark Contour Dimmers). Light intensity levels inside the experimental cage were measured with a universal exposure meter (Extech LT300). Observations were made during this experiment at the standard time at which lights were progressively changed in the colony (1730 h –1830 h). Preliminary investigations revealed that changing the initial time at which light intensity decreases in the colony across a variety of times (1430 –1530, 1530 –1630, 1630 –1730, 1730 –1830 and 1830 –1930 h) does not affect the behavior of the midges.

All the video recordings were analyzed, 1 h at a time, to describe the behavior of the midges over the course of 1 month. We analyzed a total of 31 swarm formations. The midges formed mating aggregations, but only one each night. For each swarm, we report the aggregation pattern of the midges, the beginning and ending time of the aggregation, the duration of the aggregation and the effect of light intensity level on the aggregation behavior of midges. The number of male and female midges at the beginning (10 min), middle (25 min) and the end (50 min) of the aggregation was determined using samples of individuals obtained from a swarm with a mechanical aspirator (Hausherrs Machine Works, New Jersey). We obtained samples from 6 swarms, collecting a total of 1,236 individuals that were sexed to calculate the number of males and females during swarm formation. In addition, we set up and video recorded small swarms (around 20 individuals) to examine individual flight patterns in more detail. We then, analyzed the flight pattern of individual midges by tracking the flight paths frame by frame during 2 s intervals drawing the path directly from a computer monitor (Dell Precision T3500). We used a camcorder (Sony DCR-SR-220) to video record the swarms through a Plexiglas wall under red light to observe and quantify pairing and copulation.

To determine insemination while swarming, we separated 200 male and 200 female pupae and reared these in 2 separate rearing cages (20 × 20 × 20 cm; 2 × 2 mm mesh size). The experiment was set up with 10 small rearing cages (10 × 10 × 10 cm) each with fifteen 1-day old virgin males. Two of the experimental cages were monitored to observe the age at which males initiated swarming in the absence of females. Fifteen one- day old virgin females were introduced to 1 cage and monitored for pairing to determine the age at which they start mating. On the fifth day, fifteen five-day old virgin females were added to each of the remaining experimental cages (n = 7). Detailed observations of the swarm were performed in 5 of those 7 experimental cages. Immediately after the swarm ended, the females from each cage were transferred to 70% alcohol to dissect their spermathecae and determine the insemination rate (n = 75 females). The 2 remaining experimental cages were continuously observed for swarming and after 4 consecutive nights, we dissected 30 females to determine the percentage of individuals inseminated after 4 swarms. These experiments to examine the insemination rate of females in the swarms ultimately exclude alternative explanations for the formation of the aggregations (e.g., feeding aggregations, increased activity).

Results

Our results revealed swarm formation in a species of Corethrellidae, C. appendiculata. The aggregation was initiated by 1 –2 midges flying in small elliptical circles in the same area. Within 10 –15 min the swarm became dense with more midges joining it and reached its maximum size at about 25 min. Swarm size was estimated to be between 15 and over 800 individuals at its maximum. After 40 –55 min of continuous swarming, males stopped flying and most males rested on the cage floor. The flight patterns of individuals within the swarms followed elliptical trajectories in a helix-like pattern (Fig. 1). At the beginning of the swarm individuals flew following small elliptical paths in a single plane. As the swarm progressed, they formed larger ellipses and moved faster following a helical pattern to the bottom of the swarm and circle back up again. The midges paused briefly at the top of the swarm and then flew in a spiral down again. Each spiral down from the top to the bottom of the swarm was completed by an individual in about 2 s. When swarming, individuals flew with their antennal fibrillae erect. At the end of the swarm, however, the individuals landed on the ground with their antennal fibrillae decumbent.

Fig. 1.

Flight path of a representative Corethrella appendiculata midge in a swarm during about 2 s. (a) View from above (b) View from the side. Stars indicate the midge's position while the arrows indicate its direction of midges as they fly up and down the spiral.

f01_1522.jpg

When kept under a natural 10:14 dark: light cycle, C. appendiculata formed male aggregations exclusively under low light conditions (Fig. 2). The swarm was initiated soon after the chamber lights were turned off and the light intensity in their cage was 2 l× or less. Also, the swarm loosened and quickly disappeared when the light intensity of the cage gradually increased above 2 l×. A single mating aggregation was formed each night. Sometimes, after the main aggregation was discontinued, a few midges flew for a few minutes in a swarming pattern but the aggregation dissolved before reaching high densities. Extending daytime light conditions into the hours at which the swarms usually formed prevented swarm formation.

Corethrella appendiculata used visual markers for swarm formation. In 74% of the cases the swarm was initiated over the black circular sugar wick (23 out of 31 observed swarms).The swarm was initiated close to the ground (2 –3 cm from the sugar wick). As the swarming progressed, however, the midges spread throughout the whole cage. When the sugar wick was moved randomly inside the cage, in 100% of the cases male midges initiated the swarm over the newly positioned swarm marker (n = 8).

Fig. 2.

Swarm formation under different light intensities. Light intensities:185 l× (darkest storm clouds midday), 67 l× (family living room light), 31 l× (fully overcast sunset), 2 l× (moon light), and 0 l× (complete darkness). N = 8 trials for each light intensity.

f02_1522.jpg

The aggregations were formed mainly by males with an increasing number of females as the swarm progresses (t-test, df = 5, P < 0.001; Fig. 3). At the beginning of the swarm, 99% of the individuals were males. The proportion of females increased from 1 to 5% from the beginning to the end of the swarm. It was not possible to see the arrival of females to the swarm but pairs in copula first appeared at about 20 min after the initiation of the swarm. In each swarm, the number of pairings rapidly increased to reach a maximum within 40–55 min (Fig. 4). Mating pairs of C. appendiculata were commonly observed falling out of swarms onto the floor of the cage. In the 6 swarms we investigated in detail, we observed the formation of 430 total pairs (72 ± 11 pairs per swarm, n = 6). About 47% of the pairs continued to copulate for a few seconds after falling onto the ground (17.3 ± 5.6 sec, n = 34 pairs). Pairs copulated in venter to venter position (12 ± 2 pairs observed, n = 6 swarms). Copulation took about 24.9 ± 5.5 s (n = 12 pairs). Pairing, however, was hard to distinguish in the dense swarm and the actual copulation time is probably slightly higher than recorded here.

Fig. 3.

Number of males (gray) and females (black) in the swarm near the beginning (10 min), middle (25 min) and end (50 min) from the start of mating pair formation. T-tests, df = 5,* denotes P < 0.001.

f03_1522.jpg

Experiments with 15 virgin males revealed that they start participating in swarms within 2 days of emergence (n = 2 swarms). In the cage with 1-day old females, however, coupling only took place 4 days after emergence (n = 1 swarm). The insemination rate of 5-day old virgin females during a single swarming opportunity was 30% (23 out of 75 females inseminated, n = 5 swarms). The insemination rate, however, increased tremendously as females participated in additional swarms. We observed that 80% of virgin females were inseminated after swarming on 4 consecutive nights (24 out of 30 females, n = 2 swarms).

Discussion

In general, our results revealed and characterized the formation of mating aggregations in C. appendiculata under laboratory conditions. Clements (1999) defines mating swarms in mosquitoes as aggregations with the following characteristics: (i) individuals flying within a limited space located in relation to some feature of the environment and individuals following a looping or zigzagging pattern characteristic of the species, (ii) the swarms consisting predominantly of males, and, (iii) swarming is limited to times of the day characteristic of the species, usually close to dusk or dawn. Our results provide evidence that the mating swarms of C. appendiculata are consistent with Clements (1999) definition of such aggregations in mosquitoes.

Fig. 4.

Temporal distribution of male-female pairs of Corethrella appendiculata during mating swarms (n = 6 swarms).

f04_1522.jpg

Based on the presence of 2 C. cretacea males preserved in close proximity in amber, Borkent (2008) suggested ancestral lineages in this group may had formed mating swarms. Given the limited evidence of this behavior in extant Corethrella species, however, Borkent (2008) concluded that this behavior may be now absent. Contrary to previous predictions suggesting lack of swarm formation in Corethrellidae (Yuval & Bouskila 1993; Borkent 2008), we report strong evidence revealing the formation of mating swarms in frog- biting midges as described below. In C. appen- diculata the flight pattern during swarming has similar characteristic elements as those described for most swarming insects (reviewed in Clements 1999; Sullivan 1981), including persistent flight pattern, formalized movements (elliptical trajectories in a helical pattern) and restriction to a certain space. Similarly, size and duration of the swarm as well as when it is formed are consistent with the general characteristics described for mating swarms in other dipterans (Nielsen & Haeger 1960; Charlwood et al. 1980; Reisen et al. 1985). In general, swarm formation in dipterans occurs at sunset under low light conditions (0–2 l×) along areas of female flight activity given their biting habits (Downes 1969; Nielsen & Nielsen 1962). We observed that C. appendiculata aggregate in mating swarms shortly after the light intensity has decreased below 2 l×, i.e., light conditions consistent with the time of the day when females initiate feeding.

In agreement with reports for other Culico- morpha where swarms are commonly formed over conspicuous visual markers (Downes 1969; reviewed in Clements 1999), our results suggest C. appendiculata likely also use visual markers to initiate their swarms. Corethrella appendicu- lata form mating aggregations in almost complete darkness similar to those of some anoph- eline mosquitoes which do the same (Charlwood & Jones 1980; Reisen et al. 1985; McDaniel 1986; Charlwood, 2003). Land et al. (1999) reported that the eyes of anophelines are extremely sensitive to low light levels equivalent to nocturnal light conditions. It is possible that C. appendicu- lata also have a visual system that allows them to use visual markers to form aggregations under low light conditions. Although chemical cuescould also potentially be used in swarm formation, the limited use of pheromones in this context in mosquitoes (Takken & Knols 1999) suggests that semiochemicals are unlikely to be involved. Corethrella appendiculata swarm formation is elicited by the onset of a dark photophase and visual cues (landmarks) that are likely involved in determining the location of where the swarm is formed. As in the colony, males in nature probably fly and aggregate near the ground in damp moist areas. We suggest that in the wild mating swarms of frog-biting midges are formed around small water pools that might be habitats for anurans. Such swarms might not have been noticed in the field before due to the small size of the midges, the proximity of the aggregation to the water surface and the crepuscular or nocturnal aggregating behavior similar to that of most other Culicomorpha.

Mating swarms result from sexual selection favoring a lek-like system (Bradbury & Davies, 1987; reviewed in Clements 1999). As proposed for other culicid species (Yuval & Bouskila 1993), the male-biased sex ratio and increased number of females as the swarm progresses strongly suggest the mating aggregations of C. appendiculata also act as a lek system. Accordingly, in the swarms of C. appendiculata a small proportion of males mate while almost all females achieve a mating. We observed high number of pairs formed towards the end of the swarms (last 15 min). Around 30% of the virgin females were inseminated during a single swarming event. This insemination rate is comparable to the rate described for a laboratory colony of Aedes communis (McDaniel 1986). Not surprisingly, we observed that multiple swarms increased the insemination rate of females. Around 80% of the females were inseminated after 4 consecutive swarming nights. This higher rate is also comparable to the insemination rate of Ae. communis after mating swarm events (70%; McDaniel 1986). The finding that virgin females are inseminated during swarming is robust evidence that swarm formation in this species occurs in a mating context.

It is unknown how Corethrellidae midges communicate while swarming. Acoustic signals, however, are likely to be involved. Sound is used in mating interactions in many species of Culicidae (reviewed in Gibson 1985; Clements 1999; Cator 2010), which together with Chaoboridae form the sister group of frog-biting midges (Borkent 2008). Male mosquitoes have more elaborate antennae than females that are involved in the detection of the wing beats of conspecific mates and play a critical role in courtship behavior (reviewed in Clements 1999; Hart et al. 2010). The pronounced sexual dimorphism of the antenna of C. appendiculata (Bernal et al. unpublished data) is reminiscent of such differences between the sexes in mosquitoes and strongly suggests acoustic signals are also used in Corethrellidae in a mating context. In addition, across several Diptera species there is an association between erect anten- nal hairs in males while swarming and the use of flight tones to identify conspecific females (Roth 1948; reviewed in Clements 1999). Similarly to most swarming mosquitoes (Charlwood & Jones 1980; Clements 1999), we found that almost all male C. appendiculata swarm with their anten- nal fibrillae erect. Further research that directly examines the use of acoustic mating signals, however, is necessary to confirm that sound is used in the swarming of frog-biting midges. We are currently investigating antennal hearing and the communication system of frog-biting midges during swarming to explore the use of sound in mating in this family.

Our results discovered the mating behavior of frog-biting midges, thereby filling a large gap in information about the reproductive biology and mating behavior of a family of Culicomorpha group. The current results also demonstrate the importance of swarm formation in the mating behavior of C. appendiculata. This contribution to the current knowledge about the mating behavior in this family provides basic information required to understand the evolution of such mating strategies. Finally this study provides an impetus to explore the use of sound in mating behavior in these midges.

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Phil Lounibos, University of Florida, for providing larvae to initiate the frog-biting midge colony at Texas Tech University. We are also grateful to Art Borkent (Salmon Arm, BC, Canada) and an additional anonymous reviewer who provided valuable comments to improve this manuscript. This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (NSF) grant IOS-1258039 to X.E.B.

References Cited

1.

A. Arak 1983. Male-male competition and mate choice in anuran amphibians Mate choice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Google Scholar

2.

X. E. Bernal , A. S. Rand , and M. J. Ryan 2006. Acoustic preferences and localization performance of blood-sucking flies (Corethrella Coquillett) to tungara frog calls. Behav. Ecol. 17: 709–715.  Google Scholar

3.

A. Blackwell , A. J. Mordue , M. R. Young , and W. Mordue 1992. The swarming behavior of the scottish biting midge, Culicoides impunctatus (Diptera, Ceratopogonidae). Ecol. Entomol. 17: 319–325. Google Scholar

4.

A. Borkent 2008. The frog-biting midges of the world (Corethrellidae: Diptera). Zootaxa: 1–456. Google Scholar

5.

A. Borkent , and D. A. Craig 2004. Austroconops Wirth and Lee, a lower cretaceous genus of biting midges yet living in western Australia: a new species, first description of the immatures and discussion of their biology and phylogeny (Diptera : Ceratopogonidae). American Mus. Novit. 3449: 1–67 Google Scholar

6.

A. Borkent , and T. U. Grafe 2012. The frog-biting midges of Borneo-from two to eleven species (Corethrellidae: Diptera). Zootaxa: 1–45. Google Scholar

7.

A. Borkent 2012. The pupae of Culicomorpha-morphology and a new phylogenetic tree. Zootaxa 3396: 1–98. Google Scholar

8.

J. W. Bradbury , and S. L. Verhrencamp 1977. Social organization and foraging in emballonurid bats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2: 1–17. Google Scholar

9.

J. W. Bradbury , and N. B. Davies 1987. Relative roles of intra-and intersexual selection, pp. 143–163 In J. W. Bradbury and M. B. Andersson [eds.], Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives. John Wiley & Sons, Chichester. Google Scholar

10.

M. M. Campbell , and D. S. Kettle 1979. Swarming of Culicoides Brevitarsis kieffer (Diptera: Cerato- pogonidae) with reference to markers, swarm size, proximity of cattle, and weather. Australian. J. Zool. 27: 17–30. Google Scholar

11.

L. J. Cator , B. J. Arthur , L. C. Harrington , and R. R. Hoy 2009. Harmonic convergence in the love songs of the dengue vector mosquito. Science 323: 1077–1079. Google Scholar

12.

J. D. Charlwood , and M. D. R. Jones 1980. Mating in the mosquito, Anopheles gambiae sl. Physiol. Ento- mol. 5: 315–320. Google Scholar

13.

J. D. Charlwood , J. Pinto , C. A. Sousa , H. Madsen , C. D. O. Ferreira , and V. E. Rosario 2002. The swarming and mating behaviour of Anopheles gam- biae ss (Diptera: Culicidae) from Sao Tome Island. J. Vect. Ecol. 27: 178–183. Google Scholar

14.

J. D. Charlwood , R. Thompson , and H. Madsen 2003. Observations on the swarming and mating behaviour of Anopheles funestus from southern Mozambique. Malaria J. 2: 1–10. Google Scholar

15.

A. N. Clements 1999. The biology of mosquitoes; Sensory reception and behaviour. CABI Publishing Wallingford, Oxon. Google Scholar

16.

J. A. Downes 1955. Observations on the swarming flight and mating of Culicoides (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae). Trans. R. Entomol. Soc. London 106: 213–236. Google Scholar

17.

J. A. Downes 1958. The feeding habits of biting flies and their significance in classification. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 3: 249–266. Google Scholar

18.

J. A. Downes 1969. The swarming and mating flight of Diptera. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 14: 271–298. Google Scholar

19.

S. T. Emlen , and L. W. Oring 1977. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science 197: 215–223. Google Scholar

20.

M. V. Fyodorova , and A. I. Azovsky 2003. Interactions between swarming Chironomus annularius (Diptera: Chironomidae) males: role of acoustic behavior. J. Insect Behav. 16: 295–306. Google Scholar

21.

N. H. E. Gibson 1945. On the mating swarms of certain Chironomidae (Diptera). Trans. R. Entomol. Soc. London 95: 263–294. Google Scholar

22.

G. Gibson 1985. Swarming behavior of the mosquito Culex pipiens quinquefasciatus: a quantitative analysis. Physiol. Entomol. 10: 283–296. Google Scholar

23.

T. U. Grafe , M. Saat , H. Binti , N. Hagen , B. Kaluza , H. Berudin , and M. A. Bin 2008. Acoustic localization of frog hosts by blood sucking flies Corethrella Coquillet (Diptera:Corethrellidae) in Borneo. Australian J. Entomol. 47: 350–354 Google Scholar

24.

L. D. Haramis , and W. A. Foster 1983. Survival and population density of Aedes triseriatus (Diptera: Culicidae) in a woodlot in central Ohio , USA. J. Med. Entomol. 20: 391–398. Google Scholar

25.

M. Hart , P. Belton , and G. Gries 2010. The effect of fibril erection on hearing in male Aedes togoi: an open and shut case. Canadian Acoust. 38: 36–37. Google Scholar

26.

J. HöGlund , and R. V. Alatalo 1995. Leks. Princeton University Press. Google Scholar

27.

W. E. Horsfall 1955. Mosquitoes. Their Bionomics and Relation to Disease. Ronald Press Co., New York. Google Scholar

28.

D. M. Hunter 1979. Swarming, mating and resting behaviour of three species of black fly (Diptera: Simu- liidae). Australian J. Entomol. 18: 1–6. Google Scholar

29.

K. Irvine 2000. Macrodistribution, swarming behaviour and production estimates of the lakefly Chaoborus edulis (Diptera: Chaoboridae) in Lake Malawi, pp. 431–448 In A. Rossiter and H. Kawanabe [eds.], Advances in Ecological Research. Academic Press, Google Scholar

30.

J. D. Kraus 1966. Radio Astronomy. McGraw-Hill, New York. Google Scholar

31.

M. Kon 1987. The mating system of chironomid midges (Diptera: Chironomidae). Memmoirs of the Faculty of Science, Koyoto University. Series of Biology 12: 129–134. Google Scholar

32.

A. M. Lill 1974. Social organization and space utilization in the lek-forming white-bearded manakin, M. manacus trinitatis Hartert. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 36: 513–530. Google Scholar

33.

M. F. Land , G. Gibson , J. Horwood , and J. Zeil 1999. Fundamental differences in the optical structure of the eyes of nocturnal and diurnal mosquitoes. J. Comp. Physiol. 185: 91–103. Google Scholar

34.

R. P. Marchand 1984. Field observations on swarming and mating in Anopheles gambiae mosquitoes in Tanzania. Netherlands J. Zool. 34: 367–387. Google Scholar

35.

I. N. McDaniel 1986. Swarming and mating of uni- voltine Aedes mosquitoes in the laboratory. J. American Mosquito Control Assoc. 2: 321. Google Scholar

36.

K. R. McKaye , S. M. Louda , and J. R. Stauer 1990. Bower size and male reproductive success in a cichlid fish lek. American Nat. 135: 597–613. Google Scholar

37.

S. McKeever 1977. Observations of Corethrella feeding on tree frogs (Hyla). Mosquito News 37: 522–523. Google Scholar

38.

S. McKeever , and F. E. French 1991. Corethrella (Diptera, Corethrellidae) of eastern North-America -Laboratory life history and field responses to anuran calls. Ann. Entomol. Soc. America 84: 493–497. Google Scholar

39.

M. V. Moore 1986. Method for culturing the phantom midge, Chaoborus (Diptera: Chaoboridae), in the laboratory. Aquaculture 56: 307–316. Google Scholar

40.

D. E. Moorhouse , and M. H. Colbo 1973. On the swarming of Austrosimulium pestilens MacKerras and MacKerras (Diptera: Simuliidae). Australian J. Entomol. 12: 127–130. Google Scholar

41.

E. T. Nielsen , J. S Haeger , and R. T. Bell 1960. Swarming and mating in mosquitoes. Entomol. Soc. America Misc. Publ. 1: 71–95. Google Scholar

42.

H. T. Nielsen , and E. T. Nielsen 1962. Swarming of mosquitoes. Laboratory experiments under controlled conditions. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 5: 14–32. Google Scholar

43.

D. R. Oliver 1971. Life history of the Chironomidae. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 16: 211–230. Google Scholar

44.

L. Paasivirta 1972. Taxonomy, ecology and swarming behaviour of Tanytarsus gracilentus Holmgr. (Diptera, Chironomidae) in Valassaaret, Gulf of Bothnia, Finland. Ann. Zool. Fennici 9: 255–264. Google Scholar

45.

S. Parma 1971. Chaoborus flavicans (Meigen)(Diptera, Chaoboridae), an autecological study. Univ. Groningen, Groningen. 128 pp. Google Scholar

46.

A. Pears 1998. Strategic study of household energy and greenhouse issues, pp. 61–63 In Sustainable Solutions, Australia. Google Scholar

47.

W. K. Reisen , Y. Aslam , and T. F. Siddiqui 1977. Observations on the swarming and mating of some Pakistan mosquitoes in nature. Ann. Entomol. Soc. America 70: 988–995. Google Scholar

48.

W. K. Reisen , N. F. Knop , and J. J. Peloquin 1985. Swarming and mating behavior of laboratory and field strains of Culex tarsalis (Diptera: Culicidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. America 78: 667–673. Google Scholar

49.

L. M. Roth 1948. A study of mosquito behavior. An experimental laboratory study of the sexual behavior of Aedes aegypti (Linnaeus). American Midl. Nat. 40: 265–352. Google Scholar

50.

P. Schlyter 2006. Radiometry and photometry in astronomy FAQ In Stockholm, Paul Schlyter Home Page.  http://stjarnhimlen.se/comp/radfaq.html  Google Scholar

51.

S. M. Shuster , and M. J. Wade 2003. Mating systems and strategies. Princeton University Press. Google Scholar

52.

J. S. Snead , and J. Alcock 1985. Aggregation formation and assortative mating in two meloid beetles. Evolution 39: 1123–1131. Google Scholar

53.

R. T. Sullivan 1981. Insect swarming and mating. Florida Entomol. 64: 44–65. Google Scholar

54.

W. Takken , and B. G. Knols 1999. Odor-mediated behavior of Afrotropical malaria mosquitoes. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 44: 131–157. Google Scholar

55.

R. Thornhill , G. Dodson , and L. Marshall 1983. Sexual selection and insect mating behavior. American Biol. Teach. 45: 310–319. Google Scholar

56.

J. A. Williams , and J. D. Edman 1968. Occurrence of blood meals in two species of Corethrella in Florida. Ann. Entomol. Soc. America 61: 1336–1336. Google Scholar

57.

R. M. Young 1969. Field observations on a midwinter breeding flight of Diamesa arctica (Diptera: Chiron- omidae). Ann. Entomol. Soc. America 6: 1204.  Google Scholar

58.

B. Yuval , and A. Bouskila 1993. Temporal dynamics of mating and predation in mosquito swarms. Oecologia 95: 65–69. Google Scholar

59.

R. H. Zimmerman , S. J. Barker , and E. C. Turner 1982. Swarming and mating behavior of a natural population of Culicoides variipennis (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae). J. Med. Entomol. 19: 151–156. Google Scholar
Priyanka De Silva and Ximena E. Bernal "First Report of the Mating Behavior of a Species of Frog-Biting Midge (Diptera: Corethrellidae)," Florida Entomologist 96(4), 1522-1529, (1 December 2013). https://doi.org/10.1653/024.096.0434
Published: 1 December 2013
KEYWORDS
apareamiento
Corethrellidae
enjambre reproductivo
mating aggregation
nematóceros
nematocerous Diptera
swarm formation
Back to Top