Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 June 2014 Acoustic Detectability of Rhynchophorus cruentatus (Coleoptera: Dryophthoridae)
Omotola Gbemisola Dosunmu, Nathan J. Herrick, Muhammad Haseeb, Raymond L. Hix, Richard W. Mankin
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

The palmetto weevil, Rhynchophorus cruentatus (Fabricius), is a pest of cultivated palms in Florida. The larvae of this species and 2 other important Rhynchophorus pests, R. ferrugineus (Olivier) and R. palmarum (L.), feed internally and cause structural damage to palm fronds, trunks, and offshoots. Often, infestations are not detected until the tree suffers irreparable damage. Acoustic methods used previously to detect R. ferrugineus in field environments were applied to investigate detectability of early instar R. cruentatus larvae. Sounds from neonates inoculated into palm fronds were recorded for 120 s periods at 7-day intervals for 56 days in urban (exposed) and acoustically shielded (enclosed) conditions that might be encountered when screening offshoots for sale or transportation. The sounds were classified by automated spectral analyses into multiple categories, including vehicle noise, bird calls, and broadband, 0.3-3-ms larval sound impulses. Because spectral characteristics alone are not always reliable identifiers of larval signals in wood, the analyses also identified trains of 6 or more closely grouped larval impulses called bursts to help classify fronds as infested or uninfested. Larval bursts were produced at sufficient rates to classify inoculated fronds correctly each day of testing, although molting and resting behaviors resulted in the absence of bursts in 6–50% of individual, 2-min recordings. The rates of larval bursts were not significantly different in paired comparisons of recordings obtained from the same frond on the same day under urban and shielded conditions, which suggests that bursts are useful as indicators of Rhynchophorus infestations in a variety of conditions.

Several species of Rhynchophorus (Coleoptera: Dryophthoridae) palm weevils attack commercial and ornamental palms, including R. ferrugineus (Olivier), originally from Southern Asia, R. palmarum (L.) from South America, and R. cruentatus (Fabricius)(Wattanapongsiri 1966), native to the southeastern US (Thomas 2010). Rhynchophorus cruentatus is a pest of stressed or transplanted cabbage palms, Sabal palmetto; saw palmettos, Serrenoa repens, and Canary Island date palms, Phoenix canariensis (Giblin-Davis et al. 1989). In 2000 for example, R. cruentatus caused P. canariensis damage estimated at $285,000 – $380,000 in a nursery in South Florida (Hunsberger et al. 2000). Because R. cruentatus, R. ferrugineus, and R. palmarum have similar life cycles (Salama et al. 2009; Miguens et al. 2011), investigations on R. cruentatus biology and behavior are of potential interest not only for palm weevil management in Florida but also for Rhynchophorus spp. management in other regions where they cause substantial economic damage to cultivated palms.

Female R. cruentatus lay eggs singly in leaf bases or in wounds on the exterior of a palm tree trunk (Weissling & Giblin-Davis 1994, 1997). The oval-shaped, cream-colored eggs hatch in approximately 3 days (Hunsberger et al. 2000; Weissling & Giblin-Davis 1997). The larvae crawl into and feed within the trunk, damaging its structural integrity. Rhynchophorus cruentatus completes its life cycle from the egg to adult emergence in 84 days (Giblin-Davis & Howard 1989) or less (Hunsberger et al. 2000; Weissling & Giblin-Davis 1995). As with R. ferrugineus and R. palmarum (Thomas 2010), visible symptoms of R. cruentatus larval presence often appear too late to prevent an infested tree from dying (Weissling & Giblin-Davis 1997). Improved methods for earlier detection and treatment of R. cruentatus and other Rhynchophorus spp. could help prevent economic losses to cultivated palms (Herrick & Mankin 2012).

Because movement and feeding sounds transmit well in wood, early and later stages of all cryptic Rhynchophorus larvae are prospects for acoustic detection and monitoring with specialized instrumentation (Herrick et al. 2013). Rhynchophorus ferrugineus has been investigated by acoustic methods in both field and laboratory conditions (Soroker et al. 2004; Pinhas et al. 2008; Mankin et al. 2008a; Potamitis et al. 2009; Siriwardena et al. 2010; Fiaboe et al. 2011; Herrick & Mankin 2012; Herrick et al. 2013). In general, acoustic studies of Rhynchophorus spp. and other internal feeders have found that detection can be difficult in environments with high background noise, but usually can be achieved in moderate to low background noise. The use of an acoustically shielded enclosure enables detection of early instars over greater distances than in the absence of shielding (Mankin et al. 2011). In a study with R. ferrugineus for example, the use of an enclosure resulted in a reduction of background noise from ca. 62 to ca. 51 dB at frequencies above 1 kHz (Herrick & Mankin 2012), where dB is a logarithmic measure of the vibrational acceleration. Finally, it is known that spectral averages or profiles of insect sounds can serve as acoustic indicators of infestation (Mankin et al. 2007), but resonances in wood and plant structures can distort or obscure signals transmitted over long distances from an insect to a sensor (Mankin et al. 2008a). In these cases, it often is necessary to consider the temporal patterns of sounds in addition to their spectral patterns in determining whether they are produced by the target insect or by external noise sources.

Because it has been established already that early-instar R. cruentatus can be detected acoustically (Herrick et al. 2013), the focus of this experiment was not on proof of principle, but rather on a more practical consideration of the relative detectability of R. cruentatus larvae in urban conditions or in shielded environments that might be used when screening offshoots or small trees before transport to landscapes or commercial groves.

Materials and Methods

Insect Colony

Adults were collected with cruentol (5-methyl-4-octanol, ChemTica Internacional, S.A.) lures in Tallahassee, Florida. Individual male and female pairs were held in tagged glass jars containing an oviposition substrate, cotton balls dosed with 20% honey-water solution (Shahina et al. 2009). One day later, the males were removed (Kaakeh et al. 2001) to prevent interference with the egg-laying process and possible damage to eggs (Giblin-Davis et al. 1989; Weissling & Giblin-Davis 1994). The cotton balls were examined daily for the presence of eggs and replaced. The eggs were placed in petri dishes lined with moistened filter paper. The dishes were sealed with a clear wrap to prevent the larvae from crawling out after eclosion (Weissling & Giblin-Davis 1994). The petri dishes were labeled by oviposition date and female jar number. The total number of eggs laid by each female, the number of eggs that eclosed, and dates of eclosion were recorded.

Thereafter, the petri dishes were examined daily for eclosion of neonates and the filter papers were kept moist. Individual larvae from hatched eggs were introduced into small holes (≈2 mm) drilled into sections of sugarcane stem. The sugarcane stems were tagged with the inoculation date, waxed at both ends to conserve moisture, prevent rapid dehydration, and prevent fungal infection, and incubated in a growth chamber maintained at 30 °C and 70 ± 5% RH and a 13:11 h L:D photo-period (Weissling & Giblin-Davis 1995).

Acoustic Recordings from Inoculated Palm Fronds

Ten 12-cm-long Sabal palm fronds (replicates) were cut from an uninfested tree in Tallahassee, Florida. Three holes were inserted 1-cm apart at the base of each frond. A screw was inserted at the center of the frond to serve as a connection point for acoustic recordings. Neonates from the laboratory colony were inoculated into the cut base of the palm frond, one per hole. Each hole was plugged with paper towel to prevent larval escape. The fronds were placed into plastic containers with about 25 ml of water to prevent them from drying out and were maintained at ambient environmental conditions with periodic additions of water until the end of the 56-d experiment.

The setup for monitoring and recording larval signals was similar to that described in Herrick & Mankin (2012). A sensor-preamplifier module (model SP-1L Acoustic Emission Consulting [AEC], Sacramento, California) was attached magnetically to the screw in each palm frond and connected to an AED 2000 amplifier (AEC, Sacramento, California). The AED-2000 was connected to a digital audio recorder (model HD-P2, Tascam, Montebello, California) that enabled monitoring with headphones and stored the signals for subsequent computer assessments of the larval sounds (Mankin et al. 2008a, 2011). The digitization rate was 44.1 kHz. The AED-2000 amplifier filters out signals below 1 kHz to reduce traffic and wind noise (Mankin et al. 2011).

For paired comparisons of replicates in enclosed and exposed environments, 2 to 5 recordings of 120 s were collected each day of testing from each frond in each environment. The recordings were collected between 11:00 h and 18:00 h at 7-day intervals over a 56-d period, beginning 1 day after neonate larval inoculation. The acoustically shielded enclosure was a windowless, 6 × 12 m room with a 2.5 m ceiling inside a building. All nearby mechanical equipment was silenced during testing. The exposed environment was in the yard of an apartment complex next to a city street.

Automated Classification of Larval and Background Noise Signals

The 2-min acoustic recordings were pre-screened using Raven (Charif et al. 2008) to locate periods of identifiable insect sounds and background noises and discard periods containing electrical interference and other loud signals that masked sounds of interest. As in previous studies with other stem borers (Mankin et al. 2011), larval sounds could be detected as trains (bursts) of short, 1–3 ms broadband impulses, while traffic noise, bird song, and other background noise often occurred as continuous signals with harmonic peaks that could be discriminated from larval sounds either by automated computer analysis or by experienced listeners.

For automated discrimination of larval sounds from noise, 4 spectral profiles (Mankin et al. 2011) were constructed as averages of multiple spectra from recordings of signals identified during pre-screening as those most frequently encountered during testing. The profiles were constructed using the custom-written insect signal analysis program: “Digitize, Analyze, View, Insect Sounds” (DAVIS) (Mankin et al. 2000; Herrick et al. 2013). Two larval profiles were average spectra of impulses in trains with distinctive high- or low-frequency characteristics identified from early-instar R. cruentatus larvae recorded in this study. A bird-noise profile was constructed as an average spectrum of 10-ms intervals of song recorded over a 15 s period from birds singing in trees near the experimental site. A traffic profile was used that had been constructed from low-frequency vehicular noise described previously in Herrick & Mankin (2012).

To match recorded signals with profiles, a 512-point Fourier transform was performed on each impulse of amplitude greater than a threshold set at 5% of the maximum signal level. Then the spectrum level at each frequency point (i.e., each multiple of 86.13 Hz) between 1 and 10 kHz was matched to the spectrum level of each profile (Mankin et al. 2011). The spectrum was categorized according to the profile to which it had the smallest total mean-square difference. If the smallest total mean square difference was greater than a preset threshold, typically 50 dB, the spectrum was classified as unspecified noise, matching none of the profiles.

The DAVIS program scanned through the complete set of recordings, classifying each impulse as matching one of the 2 larval profiles, a background noise profile, or as a non-match. It saved the time of occurrence and the type of each impulse in a spreadsheet. Consecutive larval impulses separated by < 150 ms were grouped together as impulse trains and classified according to the profile matched by a majority of the impulses. Based on inspection of listener-recognized signals, trains containing ≥ 6 and < 200 larval impulses were classified as larval sound bursts (Mankin et al. 2008a). For each recording, the rates of larval sound impulses, larval bursts, and noise impulses (impulses that matched neither larval sound profile), were calculated for statistical analyses.

Statistical Analyses

Paired 2-tailed t-tests were used to compare mean rates of larval impulses and bursts in exposed and enclosed environments recorded from individual fronds on different days after the beginning of testing. Also, a paired 2-tailed t-test was conducted to compare the mean rates of noise impulses in exposed and enclosed conditions over the 56-d duration of the experiment. The mean rates of noise impulses, larval impulses, and larval bursts were calculated for each frond as averages of the rates each day in enclosed and exposed recordings.

In a previous study of bursts produced by R. ferrugineus in sugarcane (Mankin et al. 2008a), the likelihood of larval infestation was considered low if the rate of larval bursts was < 1 per 50 s (0.02 bursts / s). For this experiment, it was convenient to set a similar criterion that < 1 larval burst per 120 s recording (0.0083 bursts / s) indicated low likelihood of infestation. In this case, an estimate of low likelihood of infestation would signify a detection error because all of the fronds in the experiment had been inoculated.

Results

Characteristics of Acoustic Recordings from Inoculated Palm Fronds

An example of signals recorded from larvae in frond #6 on day 7 after inoculation (Fig. 1) displays some of the similarities and differences frequently observed with signals recorded in the enclosed and exposed environments. Fig. 1A is an oscillogram of 120 s activity from the enclosed, acoustically shielded environment. The DAVIS program identified 337 larval impulses and 81 bird or traffic noise impulses. Four trains of larval impulses separated by < 150 ms (near 11.5, 19.5, 40 and 97 s) were classified as larval bursts.

One of the larval bursts is expanded in a 30-ms inset (Fig. 1A.a) for additional detail on typical ranges of amplitude and relative timing of impulses. The burst contains 2 larval impulses in the first 2 ms at its beginning, a third near its midpoint (near 13 ms) and 3 larval impulses near its end (20–30 ms).

Signals recorded 4 h earlier from the same frond in the exposed environment are displayed in Fig. 1C. There were 331 larval impulses and 81 noise impulses in this example, and 16 impulse trains met the criterion for larval bursts. The primary difference between the spectrograms in the enclosed (Fig. 1B) and exposed environments (Fig. 1D) was the noticeably higher energy below 4 kHz in the exposed environment. The latter shows a nearly continuous presence of signal between 2 and 4 kHz.

A series of samples recorded once each week from frond #3 over a 56-day period (Fig. 2) is representative of the overall range of amplitudes and temporal patterns of larval and noise impulses recorded in the exposed environment. Two examples of noise include an interval with traffic noise between 20 and 30 s on day 14, and an interval with persons talking between 5 and 15 s on day 28.

Mean Rates of Noise Impulses, Larval Impulses, and Larval Bursts in Enclosed and Exposed Environments

It was expected that the mean rate of noise impulses detected in the exposed environment would be greater than in the enclosed environment but the mean rate of larval bursts would be similar in the 2 environments. Indeed, in paired comparisons where the mean rate of noise impulses from each frond in the enclosed environment each day of testing was subtracted from the mean in the exposed environment, the mean difference ± standard error (SE), 4.213 ± 1.293 impulses / s, indicated a statistically significant difference between the 2 conditions (df = 62, t = 3.26, P = 0.002). Also as expected, in paired comparisons where the mean rate of larval bursts from each frond in the enclosed environment on each day of testing was subtracted from the mean rate in the exposed environment, the mean difference ± SE, 0.022 ± 0.027 bursts / s was not statistically significant (t = 0.823, df = 84, P = 0.412).

However, the mean rates of larval impulses in paired comparisons of recordings obtained from each frond each day under exposed and enclosed conditions were significantly different. The mean difference ± SE between larval sound impulse rates in exposed and enclosed conditions was (1.718 ± 0.849 impulses / s, t = 2.024, df = 84, P = 0.046).

Analysis of signals from different fronds on different days after inoculation (Table 1) indicated that, except on day 21 (Table 1), larval bursts were present in 70–90 percent of individual recordings. Under a criterion that absence of bursts in a 2-min period indicates low infestation likelihood, the tested samples would have been designated incorrectly as uninfested in 6 to 50 percent of the recordings. However, bursts were observed in at least one recording from each frond on each test day, which indicates that the probability of incorrectly classifying an infested sample as uninfested could be reduced by recording signals multiple times each day of testing.

Mean Rates of Larval Bursts from All Fronds on Different Days

Although each frond had different patterns of larval burst activity over the 56-day testing period, there was a trend of greatest activity on day 1 after inoculation and least activity on day 21 (Fig. 3). Otherwise, the mean activity levels remained in the range of 0.09 – 0.15 larval bursts / s. The rates of impulses in the example of Fig. 2 follow the trend seen in Fig. 3 except for day 1. The rate for this frond on day 1 was 0.134 bursts / s, somewhat less than the overall mean of 0.367 bursts / s for all fronds recorded on the first day (Fig. 3).

Fig. 1.

Examples of oscillograms (A and C) and spectrograms (B and D) of sounds recorded from an inoculated frond on day 7 in enclosed and exposed environments. Inset (a) displays a 30-ms expansion of a larval impulse burst. Darker areas in spectrograms indicate higher relative energy at those frequencies and times.

f01_431.jpg

Fig. 2.

Oscillograms of sound impulses recorded from an inoculated frond over a 56 d period in the exposed environment. Vertical scales are relative amplitude, held constant for all recordings.

f02_431.jpg

Table 1.

Comparisons of the percentages of 2-min acoustic recordings without detectable R. cruentatus larval bursts at different times after neonate inoculation.

f02_431.jpg

Discussion

It was of biological interest that the signals from all fronds on days 14 and 21 were considerably lower than during the rest of the test period. It is possible that the low level of activity on these days coincided with a period when multiple larvae had molted. A similar phenomenon was noticed by Shade et al. (1990) when monitoring acoustic activity of individual Callosobruchus maculatus (F.) larvae in cowpea seeds, and by Pittendrigh et al. (1997), who monitored activity of individual Sitophilus oryzae (L.) larvae in maize. After the low point of activity on day 21, the activity levels generally trended higher. This result might be expected if older, larger instars produced more frequent, louder sounds than the younger, smaller instars, as was observed by Shade et al. (1990) and Pittendrigh et al. (1997). However, there are numerous factors that could increase or decrease activity rates at any given time; consequently, there is often only a weak relationship between the activity rate and the number or size of insects in a sample (Mankin et al. 2011).

Fig. 3.

Mean rates of larval bursts detected each test day in individual samples recorded from ten palm frond replicates. Vertical bars indicate Standard Errors.

f03_431.jpg

An important result of this study was the finding that there were no significant differences between larval burst rates in exposed and enclosed conditions. This result is similar to those in previous investigations of wood-feeding insects, where specification of a temporal pattern of impulses in addition to a spectral pattern enabled improved discrimination of insect sounds from background noise (Mankin et al. (2008b). The result that significant differences were found between rates of larval impulses in exposed and enclosed conditions suggests that some of the background noise impulses in this study had spectra that were similar to the larval profiles and were counted as larval impulses in error. A potential cause of such similarities is resonance within the structure of the frond (Mankin et al. 2008a). A candidate for one of the resonance frequencies is seen as a continuous band between 11.5–12 kHz in the examples of Fig. 1B and 1D.

The results of this experiment combined with results of several investigations on R. ferrugineus and other internally feeding beetles (Mankin et al. 2011) suggest that all of the large Rhynchophorus weevils can be detected acoustically in host trees and fronds in both exposed and enclosed environments. The capability for early detection of infestation of all the large Rhynchophorus offers potential for effective management and reduction of inadvertent spread of these pests through movement of infested offshoots.

Acknowledgments

We greatly appreciate the assistance of Alex Bolques (Gadsden County Cooperative Extension), Ronald Rice (Palm Beach County Cooperative Extension) and Leslie Baucum (Hendry County Cooperative Extension) for providing and harvesting sugarcane. We thank Moses Kairo for assistance in the early part of this study. The senior author acknowledges funding from the USDA, APHIS Farm Bill 2008 for this study. The use of trade, firm, or corporation names in this publication is for the information and convenience of the reader. Such use does not constitute an official endorsement or approval by the USDA of any product or service to the exclusion of others that may be suitable. The USDA is an equal opportunity provider and employer.

References Cited

1.

R. A. Charif , A. M. Waack , and L. M. Strickman 2008. Raven Pro 1.3 user's manual. Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY. Google Scholar

2.

K. K. M. Fiaboe , R. W. Mankin , A. L. Roda , M. T. K. Kairo , and C. Johanns 2011. Pheromone-food-bait trap and acoustic surveys of Rhynchophorus ferrugineus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) in Curacao. Florida Entomol. 94: 766–773. Google Scholar

3.

R. M. Giblin-Davis , K. Gerber , and R. Griffith 1989. Laboratory rearing of Rhynchophorus cruentatus and R. palmarum (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Florida Entomol. 72: 480–488. Google Scholar

4.

R. M. Giblin-Davis , and F. W. Howard 1989. Vulnerability of stressed palms to attack by Rhynchophorus cruentatus (Fabricius) (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) and insecticidal control of the pest. J. Econ. Entomol. 82: 1185–1190. Google Scholar

5.

R. M. Giblin-Davis , A. C. Oehlschlager , A. Perez , G. Gries , R. Gries , T. J. Weissling , C. M. Chinchilla , J. E. Pena , R. H. Hallett , H. D. Pierce Jr , and L. M. Gonzalez 1996. Chemical and behavioral ecology of palm weevils (Curculionidae: Rhynchophorinae). Florida Entomol. 79: 153–167. Google Scholar

6.

N. J. Herrick , and R. W. Mankin 2012. Acoustical detection of early instar Rhynchophorus ferrugineus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) in Canary Island date palm, Phoenix canariensis (Arecales: Arecaceae). Florida Entomol. 95(4): 983–990. Google Scholar

7.

N. J. Herrick , R. W. Mankin , O. G. Dosunmu , and M. T. K. Kairo 2013. Ecology and detection of the red palm weevil, Rhynchophorus ferrugineus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae), and related weevils for the protection of palm tree species in the United States, pp. 269–283 In AFPP- Colloque Mediterraneen sur les ravageurs des palmiers., Nice, France 16–18 Jan 2013. Google Scholar

8.

A. G. B. Hunsberger , R. M. Giblin-Davis , and T. J. Weissling 2000. Symptoms and population dynamics of Rhynchophorus cruentatus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) in Canary Island date palms. Florida Entomol. 83: 290–303. Google Scholar

9.

W. Kaakeh , M. M. Abou-Nour , and A. A. Khamis 2001. Mass rearing of the red palm weevil, Rhynchophorus ferrugineus Olivier, on sugarcane and artificial diets for laboratory studies: Illustration of methodology, pp. 344–357 In Proc. 2nd Intl. Conf. Date Palms, Al-Ain, UAE 25–27 Mar 2001. Google Scholar

10.

R. W. Mankin , J. Brandhorst-Hubbard , K. L. Flanders , M. Zhang , R. L. Crocker , S. L. Lapointe , C. W. Mccoy , J. R. Fisher , and D. K. Weaver 2000. Eavesdropping on insects hidden in soil and interior structures of plants. J. Econ. Entomol. 95: 981–988. Google Scholar

11.

R. W. Mankin , D. W. Hagstrum , M. T. Smith , A. L. Roda , and M. T. K. Kairo 2011. Perspective and promise: a century of insect acoustic detection and monitoring. American Entomol. 57: 30–44. Google Scholar

12.

R. W. Mankin , J. L. Hubbard , and K. L. Flanders 2007. Acoustic indicators for mapping infestation probabilities of soil invertebrates. J. Econ. Entomol. 100: 790–800. Google Scholar

13.

R. W. Mankin , A. Mizrach , A. Hetzroni , S. Levsky , Y. Nakache , and V. Soroker 2008a. Temporal and spectral features of sounds of wood-boring beetle larvae: Identifiable patterns of activity enable improved discrimination from background noise. Florida Entomol. 91: 241–248. Google Scholar

14.

R. W. Mankin , M. T. Smith , J. M. Tropp , E. B. Atkinson , and D. Y. Jong 2008b. Detection of Anoplophora glabripennis (Coleoptera: Cerambycidae) larvae in different host trees and tissues by automated analyses of sound- impulse frequency and temporal patterns. J. Econ. Entomol. 101: 838–849. Google Scholar

15.

F. C. Miguens , J. A. Sacramento De Magalhaes , L. Melo De Amorim , V. R. Goebel , N. Le Coustour , M. Lummerzheim , J. I. L. Moura , and R. M. Costa 2011. Mass trapping and biological control of Rhynchophorus palmarum L.: A hypothesis based on morphological evidences. Entomobrasilis 4: 49–55. Google Scholar

16.

J Pinhas , V. Soroker ., A. Hetzroni , A. Mizrach , M. Teicher , and J. Goldberger 2008. Automated acoustic detection of the red palm weevil. Comput. Electronics Agrie. 63: 131–139. Google Scholar

17.

B. R. Pittendrigh , J. E. Huesing , R. E. Shade , and L. L. Murdock 1997. Monitoring of rice weevil, Sitophilus oryzae, feeding behavior in maize seeds and the occurrence of supernumerary molts in low humidity conditions. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 83: 225–231. Google Scholar

18.

I. T. Potamitis , T. Ganchev , and D. Kontodimas 2009. On automatic bioacoustic detection of pests: the cases of Rhynchophorus ferrugineus and Sitophilus oryzae. J. Econ. Entomol. 102: 1681–1690. Google Scholar

19.

H. S. Salama , F. N. Zaki , and A. S. Abdel-Razek 2009. Ecological and biological studies on the red palm weevil Rhynchophorus ferrugineus (Olivier). Arch. Phytopath. Plant Prot. 42: 392–399. Google Scholar

20.

R. E. Shade , E. S. Furgason , and L. L. Murdock 1990. Detection of hidden insect infestations by feeding-generated ultrasonic signals. American Entomol. 36: 231–234. Google Scholar

21.

F. Shahina , J. Salma , G. Mehreen , M. I. Bhatti , and K. A. Tabassum 2009. Rearing of Rhynchophorus ferrugineus (Oliv.) in laboratory and field conditions for carrying out various efficacy studies using EPNs. Pakistan J. Nematol. 27(2): 221–231. Google Scholar

22.

K. A. P. Siriwardena , C. P. Fernando , N. Nanayakkara , K. E. G. Perera , A. D. N. T. Kumara , and T. Nanayakkara 2010. Portable acoustic device for detection of coconut palms infested by Rynchophorus ferrugineus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Crop. Prot. 29: 25–29. Google Scholar

23.

V. Soroker , Y. Nakache , U. Landau , A. Mizrach , A. Hetzroni , and D. Gerling 2004. Utilization of sounding methodology to detect infestation by Rhynchophorus ferrugineus on palm offshoots. Phytoparasitica 32: 1–3. Google Scholar

24.

M. C. Thomas 2010. Giant palm weevils of the genus Rhynchophorus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae) and their threat to Florida palms. Florida Dept. Agric. Consumer Serv., Division of Plant Industry. DACSP-01682:1-2.  http://www.freshfromflorida.com/pi/pest-alerts/pdf/giantpalmweevils.pdf  Google Scholar

25.

A. Wattanapongsiri 1966. A revision of the genera Rhynchophorus and Dynamis (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Dept. Agric. Sci. Bull., Bangkok 1: 1–328. Google Scholar

26.

T. J. Weissling , and R. M. Giblin-Davis 1994. Fecundity and fertility of Rhynchophorus cruentatus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Florida Entomol. 77: 373–376. Google Scholar

27.

T. J. Weissling , and R. M. Giblin-Davis 1995. Oligidic diets for culture of Rhynchophorus cruentatus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Florida Entomol. 78: 225–234. Google Scholar

28.

T. J. Weissling , and R. M. Giblin-Davis 1997. Palmetto weevil, Rhynchophorus cruentatus Fabricius. Univ. Florida IFAS Ext. EENY-0013:1–6. Google Scholar
Omotola Gbemisola Dosunmu, Nathan J. Herrick, Muhammad Haseeb, Raymond L. Hix, and Richard W. Mankin "Acoustic Detectability of Rhynchophorus cruentatus (Coleoptera: Dryophthoridae)," Florida Entomologist 97(2), 431-438, (1 June 2014). https://doi.org/10.1653/024.097.0213
Published: 1 June 2014
KEYWORDS
barrenador críptico del tallo
cryptic stem borer
gorgojo del palmetto
monitoreo
monitoring
palma Sabal
palmetto weevil
Back to Top