Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 March 2011 Pollen Preference for Psychotria sp. is Not Learned in the Passion Flower Butterfly, Heliconius erato
Christian Salcedo
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Heliconius butterflies are known to maximize fitness by feeding on pollen from Gurania sp. and Psiguria sp. (Cucurbitales: Curcurbitaceae), and Psychotria sp. (Gentianales: Rubiaceae). This specialization involves specific physical, physiological, and behavioral adaptations including efficient search strategies in the forest to locate pollen host plants, pollen removal, and pollen external digestion. Reducing pollen host plant search time is crucial to out-compete other flower visitors and to reduce exposure to predators. One way in which this can be achieved is by using chemical cues to learn from experienced foragers in roosting aggregations. Similar strategies have been documented in bumblebees, where inexperienced individuals learn floral odors from experienced foragers. Behavioral experiments using plants preferred by Heliconius erato suggest that pollen preference in H. erato is an innate trait and consequently learning of chemical cues at roosting aggregations is unlikely.

Introduction

Heliconius butterflies are known to rely significantly on their natural pollen host plants in order to acquire essential amino acids that improve life span and egg production (Brown et al. 1991; Dunlap-Pianka et al. 1977). Preferred pollen host plants include several species from the genera Gurania sp. and Psiguria sp. (Cucurbitaceae) and several species from the genus Psychotria sp. (Rubiaceae). These plants have inflorescences that bear bright orange and red colors, which are important long range cues used by Heliconius adults to locate the plants when they are navigating the forest when foraging. Location of these plants has been suggested to be tightly linked with the home range of Heliconius populations (Gilbert 1991). By dwelling close to their most important nutritional resource adults maximize their fitness. However, inexperienced adults may be at risk when searching these conspicuous plants in the forest by increasing exposure to predators. In addition, Heliconius pollen host plants are also pollinated by hummingbirds (Cardoso de Castro and Cardoso Araujo 2004; Murawski and Gilbert 1986; Stone 1996) and with this competition minimizing search time is important. Pollen host plant search time can be decreased in several ways. One of them assumes that the butterflies are born with no specific preference for a particular pollen host plant and hence they need to learn this preference. Recent evidence in bumblebees shows that inexperienced individuals learn floral odors from experienced foragers by associating flower scented nectar, brought to the nest by the experienced foragers, with a specific chemical cue (Molet et al. 2009). Heliconius butterflies form nocturnal aggregations (Wallace 1870), where males and females perch gregariously night after night. These aggregations are stable and often are located near pollen host plants (Mallet 1986). New observations have revealed that females arrive to roost sites with loads of pollen (Salcedo 2010). Young inexperienced Heliconius butterflies may be learning pollen odors or taste from experienced foragers that arrive to nocturnal roost sites bearing loads of pollen. Alternatively, preference for natural pollen host plants may be innate so learning would not be necessary. To evaluate if pollen preference in Heliconius erato L. (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae) is innate or learned, choice experiments using Psychotria sp. pollen and naïve H. erato butterflies were done.

Materials and Methods

H. erato butterflies were reared with Passiflora biflora as pollen host plant and held in a 2 × 2 × 3 m outdoor cage. The butterflies were never exposed to their preferred pollen host plants (i.e. Psychotria sp., Gurania sp. or Psiguria sp.) and were trained to feed on red colored feeders with sugar water solution. Based on preliminary tests, colored feeders were used because the butterflies need the color cue to be able to recognize the feeders as a foraging source. In each trial one individual was exposed to two feeding choices (% weight): (a) 30% bee collected pollen (Apiarios Malivern, Panama), 10% sugar, 60% water; and (b) 30% Psychotria sp. pollen, 10% sugar, 60% water. In real flowers pollen is offered together with a nectar reward (which is rich in sugars), so the sugar-water solution in the experimental choices was meant to play this role. Psychotria sp. pollen was collected from local flowers. Each experiment was carried out in a 2 × 2 × 1.5 m outdoor cage. The solutions were placed in identical red colored feeders

Table 1.

Number and duration of feeding events in pollen-feeding choice tests with naïve Heliconius erato butterflies. Feeding choices were artificial red colored feeder with bee collected pollen in water-sugar solution, and artificial red color feeder with manually collected Psychotria sp. pollen in sugar-water solution. Each trial lasted 10 min and one individual was used per trial. Whenever an individual started to feed in one of the choices time was recorded with a stopwatch. Total time spent feeding on Psychotria sp. pollen is significantly higher compared to time spent in bee pollen (Wilcoxon SR test W = -115 P = 0.0034).

t01_01.gif

Results and Discussion

In all trials individual H. erato were tested on both choices at least once. The number of feeding events was not significantly different between the choices (Wilcoxon signed rank test: W = -54, P = 0.064), however time spent feeding on the Psychothria sp. pollen feeder was significantly higher than time spent feeding on the bee pollen feeder (Wilcoxon SR test: W = -115, P = 0.0034) (Table 1). This suggests that the butterflies naturally prefer to feed on Psychotria sp. pollen even having an alternative feeding pollen source available. It is likely then that preference for natural pollen host plants is innate in H. erato. Subsequently, learning of pollen preference at roost sites is unlikely. Due to the uneven number of males and females used in the trials (5 males and 12 females) it is difficult to draw conclusions on sex-based preferences. Under natural conditions females tend to forage more because of their physiological and ecological needs in order to increase egg production and have prolonged lifespan (Dunlap-Pianka et al. 1977; O'Brien et al. 2003). The results herein do not follow this trend, overall males spent more time in either of the two choices (Figure 1). It is unknown if H. erato are using chemical cues to locate the preferred pollen feeder. Based on field and in-cage observations of feeding behavior, they first use color in the long range (2–10 m) to recognize their potential pollen host plants, then fly towards the flower and hover over before landing to start feeding. Hovering may be used to detect short-range chemical cues. Pollen host plants may have short-range volatile chemical cues that could be produced by the flowers. Recent data demonstrates that H. erato is attracted to (E)-β-ocimene (unpublished data), a very common semiochemical emitted by flowers to attract pollinators (Knudsen et al. 1993). Pollen grains themselves can also emit volatile chemical cues (Dobson and Bergström 2000). The experimental choices presented here had only a visual cue (red-colored feeders) and two types of pollen, so a plausible explanation for the extended feeding periods on Psychotria sp. may be a contact chemical cue (taste) as the major factor in producing the observed results.

Figure 1.

Average feeding time per trial for naïve Heliconius erato males and females in pollen-feeding choice experiments. Feeding choices were (A) artificial red colored feeder with bee collected pollen in water-sugar solution, and (B) artificial red color feeder with manually collected Psychotria sp. pollen in sugar-water solution. Each trial lasted 10 min and one individual was used per trial. Error bars show standard deviation. High quality figures are available online.

f01_01.eps

The results presented here suggest that once a color cue is used to identify pollen-feeding sources, taste from Psychotria sp. pollen grains is enough to assure preference. Nevertheless a combination of flower and pollen—emitted volatile chemical cues is probably necessary in the forest, where chemical noise from numerous other sources is present. Further analyses need to include volatile collection from Heliconius pollen host plants, identification of volatiles and evaluation of their role in foraging ecology in cage and field bioassays.

Acknowledgements

I wish to thank the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Panama Canal Authority (ACP), and Panama Environmental Authorities (ANAM) for processing and facilitating permits to access field sites. Funding for this work came from a Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute shortterm Fellowship and from a Explorers Club Research Grant.

References

1.

KS Brown , JR Trigo , RB Francini , de Morais AB Barros , PC Motta . 1991. Aposematic insects on toxic host plants: Coevolution, colonization, and chemical emancipation. In: PW Price , TM Lewinsohn , GW Fernandes , WW Benson , editors. Plant-animal interactions: Evolutionary ecology in tropical and temperate regions , pp. 375–402. John Wiley & Sons. Google Scholar

2.

C Cardoso de Castro , A Cardoso Araujo. 2004. Distyly and sequential pollinators of Psychotria nuda (rubiaceae) in the atlantic rain forest, Brazil. Plant Systematics and Evolution 244: 131–139. Google Scholar

3.

HEM Dobson , G Bergström . 2000. The ecology and evolution of pollen odors. Plant Systematics and Evolution 222: 63–87. Google Scholar

4.

H Dunlap-Pianka , CL Boggs , LE Gilbert . 1977. Ovarian dynamics in heliconiine butterflies: Programmed senescence versus eternal youth. Science 197: 487–490. Google Scholar

5.

LE Gilbert . 1991. Biodiversity of a central american Heliconius community: Pattern, process, and problems. In: PW Price , TM Lewinsohn , GW Fernandes , WW Benson , editors. Plant-animal interactions: Evolutionary ccology in tropical and temperate regions , pp. 403–427. John Wiley & Sons. Google Scholar

6.

JT Knudsen , L Tollsten , LG Bergström . 1993. Floral scents--a checklist of volatile compounds isolated by head-space techniques. Phytochemistry 33: 253–280. Google Scholar

7.

J Mallet . 1986. Gregarious roosting and home range in Heliconius butterflies. National Geoographic Research 2: 198–215. Google Scholar

8.

M Molet , L Chittka , N Raine . 2009. How floral odours are learned inside the bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) nest. Naturwissenschaften 96: 213–219. Google Scholar

9.

DA Murawski , LE Gilbert . 1986. Pollen flow in psiguria warscewiczii: A comparison of Heliconius butterflies and hummingbirds. Oecologia 68: 161–167. Google Scholar

10.

DM O'Brien , CL Boggs , ML Fogel . 2003. Pollen feeding in the butterfly Heliconius charitonia: Isotopic evidence for essential amino acid transfer from pollen to eggs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270: 2631–2636. Google Scholar

11.

C Salcedo . 2010. Evidence of pollen digestion at nocturnal aggregations of Heliconius sara in Costa Rica (Lepidoptera: Nymphalidae). Tropical Lepidoptera. 20 (1): 35–37. Google Scholar

12.

JL Stone . 1996. Components of pollination effectiveness in Psychotria suerrensis, a tropical distylous shrub. Oecologia 107: 504– 512. Google Scholar

13.

AR Wallace . 1870. Contributions to the theory of natural selection. London: McMillan. Google Scholar
This is an open access paper. We use the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 license that permits unrestricted use, provided that the paper is properly attributed.
Christian Salcedo "Pollen Preference for Psychotria sp. is Not Learned in the Passion Flower Butterfly, Heliconius erato," Journal of Insect Science 11(25), 1-5, (1 March 2011). https://doi.org/10.1673/031.011.0125
Received: 4 January 2010; Accepted: 1 May 2010; Published: 1 March 2011
KEYWORDS
aggregations
behavior
pollen feeding
Back to Top