Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
31 August 2020 Acoustic Repertoire and Calling Behavior of the Gliding Treefrog, Agalychnis spurrelli (Anura: Hylidae)
Ricardo Cossio, Eduardo Medina-Barcenas
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Acoustic communication is essential for reproductive success in frogs. Males produce different types of calls to attract females, advertise territoriality or location in a chorus, or communicate with heterospecifics. Quantitative descriptions of amphibian calls provide basic information for taxonomic, ecological, evolutionary, and conservation studies. Here, we describe the acoustic repertoire of Agalychnis spurrelli and discuss this species' reproductive behavior on the basis of observations made over the course of a breeding season in May–December 2007. Males produced one type of advertisement call and two types of aggressive calls (calls A and B) that differed in temporal and spectral frequency. The advertisement call was a single note. Aggressive call A was a long-pulsed single note, and call B was composed of more than two notes per call. Calling behavior was influenced by chorus size and male-to-male proximity, with males altering their advertisement calls with the increase of chorus size and eventually producing aggressive calls. These results suggest that A. spurrelli might exhibit graded aggressive signaling, helping males to delimit their calling site and reducing the number of agonistic encounters.

INTRODUCTION

Acoustic communication is an important component of reproductive success and social interaction in anurans (Wells, 1988; Wells and Schwartz, 2006). Most anuran species produce different calls that can vary in their functions (Littlejohn, 1977; Wells, 1977, 1988, 2007; Toledo et al., 2015; Köhler et al., 2017); nonetheless, advertisement and aggressive calls are the most commonly heard (Wells and Schwartz, 2006). The advertisement call is mainly emitted to attract mates (Wells, 1977; Rand and Ryan, 1981; Wells and Greer, 1981; Wells and Schwartz, 1984a), although it can also advertise location and maintain spatial separation between males in a breeding chorus (Wells, 1977; Wells and Greer, 1981; Wells, 2007). Aggressive calls serve as a warning to other males in order to defend territories or calling sites (Wells and Schwartz, 1984b; Pröhl, 1997; Bastos and Haddad, 2002). Additionally, the properties of the advertisement call can be used by females to assess male mate quality, while the aggressive call can contain cues on the size or fighting ability of the emitter (Gerhardt and Huber, 2002; Gerhardt and Bee, 2006; Wells, 2007).

Calling behavior in anurans can be influenced by the acoustic environment including background noise, acoustic interference, and vocal responsiveness of nearby competitors (Schwartz and Wells, 1984; Schwartz and Wells, 1985; Schwartz et al., 2002; Schwartz and Bee, 2013). Consequently, males can alter their call rate, complexity, duration, and intensity during choruses (Wells, 2007). Therefore, it is important to identify and describe the differences in anuran call repertoires as they are key factors that potentially influence the evolution of acoustic signaling and sexual selection in anurans. Additionally, it is important to establish how such differences vary among species.

Most species of Agalychnis Cope, 1864 are prolonged breeders (A. annae, A. callidryas, A. dacnicolor, A. granulosa, A. lemur, A. moreletii: Duellman, 1970; Pyburn, 1970; Gomez-Mestre et al., 2008; Vilela et al., 2015), with the exception of A. saltator and A. spurrelli, which show explosive breeding with an unusual aggregation behavior (Scott and Starret, 1974; Roberts, 1994; Ortega-Andrade et al., 2011). Nonetheless, different breeding patterns have been reported among populations of A. spurrelli (Vargas et al., 2000). The acoustic repertoires of Agalychnis species consist of advertisement calls of a single note; however, some species of this genus present advertisement calls of more than one note repeated at different intervals of a few seconds to several minutes (Duellman, 1970; Cannatella, 1980; Pimenta et al., 2007; Hertz and Lotzkat, 2010; Vilela et al., 2015). The vocal repertoire of A. spurrelli includes an advertisement call, described as a single, low-pitched groan (Duellman, 1970), and a second call, reported by Scott and Starret (1974). The description of Scott and Starret (1974) is limited to the call rate and provides no further details about any other acoustic parameter, leaving the functions and specific context of this call unknown. Most of the calling pattern of A. spurrelli is based on observations of few individuals (Duellman, 1970; Ortega-Andrade, 2008). Therefore, information about the calling behavior in this species is relatively limited. We describe the call properties and calling patterns of A. spurrelli in relation to its social behavior and compare them with those of other species of Agalychnis. We also assess the influence of environmental factors on the calling phenology of this species to determine possible correlations between calling males and abiotic variables.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

We conducted this study in May–December 2007 at Kingfisher pond (09°09′24.6″N, 79°51′28.4″W), a seasonal pond located on Barro Colorado Island (BCI). BCI is a 1,560-ha reserve and research station located in the Panama Canal that is operated by the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute. The vegetation on BCI is characterized by a seasonal lowland moist forest. The island receives approximately 2,623 mm of precipitation during the rainy season (mid-May to mid-December), with a pronounced dry season from mid-December to mid-April (Leigh, 1999). Kingfisher pond has an oval shape and area of approximately 338 m2, with a maximum depth of 0.9 m. The vegetation immediately surrounding the pond is formed mainly by Elaeis oleifera (HBK) Cortes (1897) a palm tree not exceeding 20 m in height, and shrubs, grass, and old-growth forest.

We visited Kingfisher pond monthly for 5–7 nights per visit. We sampled following the new moon lunar phase, as suggested by Kubicki (2004). At the beginning of our study, behavioral sampling was carried out from 19:00–06:00 or until calling activity diminished or ended. We observed the first males calling around 21:30, so we modified our sampling methods to start at this time. We recorded vocalizations from 35 calling males using a Panasonic digital recorder-RRUS395PS with a Sony ECM-C115 microphone at about 1 m from the individual. After recording, we marked each focal calling male by a unique toe-clip combination (up to three clips per frog) following Donnelly's scheme (Heyer et al., 2001), and all frogs were released at the same point of capture within 24 h.

Vocalizations were digitized at 22 kHz and 16 bit resolution and analyzed using the software Raven Pro version 1.5 (Bioacoustics Research Program, 2011) through Fast Fourier Transformation at 512-point width, Hanning window type, and overlapped at 90%. We measured the following call variables: call duration (s), call repetition rate (calls/min), number of notes per call, note duration (s), pulses/note, and dominant frequency (Hz). Measurements are presented as mean ± SE (range). We classified calls on the basis of the terminology proposed by Wells (2007) and Toledo et al. (2015). Additionally, we recorded behavioral observations of calling males during courtship and agonistic interactions to categorize the vocalizations emitted by Agalychnis spurrelli according to the social context. Some sound files were deposited in Fonoteca Zoologica (FZ 10052, 10053), Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales de Madrid.

Climate data such as humidity, temperature, and precipitation were taken for every day of recording from the meteorological station on BCI, called “El Claro,” which is located 2.2 km from the study site. To determine whether the density of males increased the aggressive call rate, we divided the total number of aggressive calls emitted by all males by the total number of males for each night and compared this value with the total number of males in the pond. We used the Shapiro-Wilk test to check for normality. Due to violation of residual normality, we used Spear-man's rank correlation. Data were analyzed using SPSS 21.0 (IBM Corporation, 2012), and statistical significance was considered at P ≤ 0.05.

RESULTS

Vocalization

Three types of calls of Agalychnis spurrelli could be clearly differentiated in our recordings on the basis of differences in call structure and social context. Call parameters are shown in Table 1. Males produced an advertisement call (Fig. 1A) consisting of a single, simple note (n = 96 calls; 16 males) of 0.20 ± 0.01 s (0.16–0.30 s) duration and emitted at a rate of 3.1 calls/min. This call had a pulse rate of 81.9 ± 2.9 s (61–99 s) and a dominant frequency of 884.2 ± 25.6 Hz (750–1,054.7 Hz). Figure 1A shows the high energy at the beginning of the call, with less energy after the middle of the note.

The other two aggressive calls (calls A and B) were produced by males during agonistic encounters (Fig. 1B–C). Aggressive call A was emitted at rate of about 3.5 ± 0.6 calls/min (n = 28 calls; seven males), with a duration of 0.59 ± 0.03 s (0.47–0.69 s), and a pulse rate of 133.3 ± 9.4 s (44–241 s). This call was emitted at a dominant frequency of 904 ± 28.2 Hz (843–1,062.5 Hz). Aggressive call B was a multi-note call consisting of 2–22 notes (n = 33 calls; 12 males), with longer call duration (0.97 ± 0.2 s), and emitted at shorter call intervals (4.05 ± 0.7 s) compared to the advertisement call. Call B was emitted at a frequency of 809 ± 11.5 Hz (750–847.7 Hz). Aggressive call A was produced singly, but occasionally in combination with call B (Fig. 2).

Calling behavior

The breeding season of Agalychnis spurrelli on BCI took place from mid-June to late October, but reproductive activity usually lasted 3–4 d. The highest number of calling males at Kingfisher pond occurred in July–September; thereafter, the number of individuals decreased dramatically (Fig. 3). The number of males calling at Kingfisher pond per month was positively correlated with relative humidity (rs = 0.381; P = 0.034; n = 31) and rainfall (rs = 0.378; P = 0.036; n = 31), but not with temperature (rs = -0.275; P = 0.135; n = 31).

Figure 1.

Waveform (top) and spectrogram (bottom) of calls produced by different males of Agalychnis spurrelli. (A) Advertisement call. (B) Aggressive call A. (C) Aggressive call B. Note the difference in call duration among calls. Spectrogram produced with Hanning window function, 512 point width; recordings made at 25°C and 24°C air temperature.

img-z3-6_71.jpg

Figure 2.

Agonistic interaction between two male Agalychnis spurrelli in Kingfisher pond: Male (1) and Male (2). Waveform (top) and spectrogram (bottom). Male 1 produced two advertisement calls, while male 2 emitted a combination of aggressive calls: call A followed by call B. Spectrogram produced with Hanning window function, 512 point width; recordings made at 25°C air temperature.

img-z3-8_71.jpg

Calling activity almost always began after 00:00 and continued until 06:00. Advertisement calls were emitted across the night or during nights with a lower number of calling males. In contrast, males emitted aggressive calls on nights with high activity. The number of aggressive calls emitted per male increased with the number of calling males in the pond (rs = 0.846, P < 0.001, n = 31). Males produced exclusively aggressive calls when facing other males in calling bouts, especially when they were close to each other (0.23–1.2 m; n = 6 observations), even without physical interaction between them. On one occasion, we observed a male in amplexus producing aggressive call B while other males in the surrounding area were also producing the same call. We observed only one fight between two males, which lasted few seconds. Both males were emitting combinations of aggressive calls A and B before the physical combat.

Table 1.

Acoustic parameters of Agalychnis spurrelli. Values are presented as mean ± SE.

img-z4-2_71.gif

Figure 3.

Number of males observed calling during 8 months at Kingfisher pond. Breeding season of Agalychnis spurrelli is from May to December 2007. The highest numbers of males calling were between July and September.

img-z4-3_71.jpg

At the beginning of chorus activity, a few males produced advertisement calls, which were followed by the rest of the males throughout the night. As the number of calling males increased, we observed that males reduced the call rate per minute and produced aggressive call A before emitting call B. There were moments in which all males emitted only aggressive call B. No displacements of amplectant pairs were observed. We observed the presence of amplexus after 01:30 (n = 10). Throughout the night, there were two peaks in calling activity, one at 02:30 and other at 04:30. The chorus began to decline after 04:30, when the only males calling were those close to a female. At 06:00, there were few if any males calling.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we describe the acoustic repertoire and calling behavior of Agalychnis spurrelli. We provide details about this species' advertisement call and describe two types of aggressive calls with information about the behavioral context in which they were displayed. The characteristics of the advertisement call reported here differ from those reported by Ortega-Andrade (2008) and Duellman (1970). We recorded advertisement calls with similar pulse rates but shorter note duration and higher dominant frequency than those described previously by Duellman (1970), who described the advertisement call of A. spurrelli as a single note with duration of 0.34–0.40 s, a pulse rate of about 60–90 s, and a dominant frequency of 435–750 Hz. These differences in note length and spectral frequency are likely related to the small sample size and regional and size differences between the individuals studied by Duellman (1970; n = 2 frogs, Panama), Ortega-Andrade (2008; n = 1 frog, Ecuador), and us (n = 16 frogs) in Panama.

Males produced two calls to which we have assigned aggressive functions. These calls differ from the advertisement call in acoustical properties and the behavioral context in which they were emitted. Scott and Starret (1974) described call B as a weak call emitted constantly, with note rate (150 notes/min) being the only acoustic parameter they mentioned. They interpreted this call as a bluff attempt by a male in the presence of another male. In contrast, Ibáñez et al. (1999) suggested it might be an aggressive call but added no detail. We also detected an additional aggressive call (call A, Fig. 1B). Males produced these calls only on nights with a high number of calling males, when males were in close proximity to each other or during agonistic encounters. Males emitting combinations of calls A and B before a physical combat support the general assumption of aggressive call function described elsewhere (Wells, 2007; Toledo et al., 2015). In addition, our field observations suggest that these calls are likely used to maintain distance between males during agonistic interactions.

Table 2.

Acoustic parameters of the advertisement call of frogs of the genus Agalychnis.

img-z5-2_71.gif

The calling behavior of Agalychnis spurrelli is similar to that of several species of Agalychnis (Table 2). Most species present an advertisement call composed of a single note, except for A. callidryas, A. saltator, and A. medinae (Duellman, 1970; Hertz and Lotzkat, 2010). The advertisement call of A. spurrelli has the highest value of pulses per note and lowest dominant frequency of the genus. Advertisement call duration in A. spurrelli is most similar to that of A. annae and A. dacnicolor.

Interestingly, only four species of Agalychnis (among the 11 described) present another type of call besides the advertisement call. Jungfer and Weygoldt (1994) described an encounter call in captive individuals of A. lemur, which they characterized as a short note with duration of 0.05–0.15 s, 2–4 pulses per note, and 800–2,750 Hz of dominant frequency. Similar to our observation in A. spurrelli, Jungfer and Weygoldt (1994) mentioned that this encounter call was emitted when a male was near another one. Similarly, A. callidryas emits a “chuckle” aggressive call that might reinforce the boundaries of calling territories (Pyburn, 1970; Caldwell et al., 2010). Caldwell et al. (2010) observed that male A. callidryas displayed vibrational signals (tremulations) and emitted aggressive calls during agonistic interactions, but they did not provide any information about the properties of this call. D'Orgeix (1996) also found that A. callidryas, besides chuckle calls and vibrations, produced a “soft tlock call.” He described the chuckle call as having 3–5 notes, with duration of 0.35 s, and dominant frequency of 1,285 Hz and the “soft tlock call” as a single note of 0.028 s duration and mean dominant frequency of 1,565 Hz. As in A. callidryas, males of A. moreletii use tremulation displays and aggressive chuckle calls during agonistic interactions that might be important to defend territories or calling sites (Serrano et al., 2018). In A. saltator, Roberts (1994) reported that during breeding aggregations males produced a “soft squeaking and chuckling noise” that differed from the advertisement call. Despite the reports of different types of calls in A. moreletii and A. saltator, only the advertisement call has been described in these species (Duellman, 1970; Briggs, 2010).

We observed a male emitting aggressive call B while amplecting a female. Similar behavior has been observed in other phyllomedusine species. Pyburn (1970) observed that Agalychnis callidryas emitted chuckle calls when unattached males approached amplectant pairs, and Pyburn (1970) and Bagnara et al. (1986) observed that A. dacnicolor produced calls when unattached males approached amplectant pairs. A possible explanation is that males use this call to evaluate other males before engaging in escalated combats (see below). Among phyllomedusine, it is common for males in amplexus to emit aggressive calls when solitary males attempt to displace amplecting pairs (Martins et al., 1998; Abrunhosa and Wogel, 2004; Wogel et al., 2005; Venâncio and Melo-Sampaio, 2010; Oliveira et al., 2012; Dias et al., 2017).

When producing aggressive calls, males usually alter some parameters of the advertisement call, such as duration, pulse rate, and spectral frequency. It is well-known that males can alter some acoustic structures of the call in relation to chorus size in order to increase its relative attractiveness (Schwartz and Wells, 1985; Schwartz et al., 2002). Males can present an aggressive threshold relative to the call amplitude of neighbors that can be used to repel neighboring males from their immediate vicinity (Brenowitz, 1989). For instance, when an intruder male produces calls above the resident's aggressive threshold, the resident male will produce aggressive calls. If the amplitude of the intruder male rises, the resident will increase the number of aggressive calls and finally repel the intruder or engage in physical combat (Rose and Brenowitz, 1991; Brenowitz and Rose, 1994).

Although playback experiments were not carried out in this study, we speculate that a similar pattern may explain the calling behavior of Agalychnis spurrelli. We observed that males increase their advertisement call rate when they are near one another. Thereafter, males produce call A, evoking an aggressive response, and finally switch to call B. However in this study, we observed that agonistic interactions are not constant in A. spurrelli. When a neighbor male stops the aggressive calls or begins emitting advertisement calls again the others return to the production of advertisement calls. This may suggest that A. spurrelli also presents an aggressive threshold and that males can adjust their aggressive behavior based on the calling intensity within the chorus. Playback experiments in males considering all call components and responses to the different call types of A. spurrelli are needed to test this hypothesis.

Despite Agalychnis spurrelli having been categorized as an explosive breeder with individuals that congregate in aquatic breeding sites for short periods of time (Scott and Starret, 1974; Wells, 2007), dissimilar breeding behaviors have been observed in populations from different places. Vargas et al. (2000) mentioned that this species has a prolonged breeding behavior on the basis of the year-round presence of adults and tadpoles in different larval stages in a population at Anchicaya, Colombia. In our study, the breeding behavior of A. spurrelli on BCI occurred over 4 months during the rainy season. At the end of the rainy season, no individuals of A. spurrelli were found at Kingfisher pond. This breeding behavior is similar to that report by Scott and Starrett (1974) in Costa Rica and Ortega-Andrade et al. (2011) in Ecuador. Based on the reproductive pattern observed on BCI, we support the assumption that this species is an opportunistic explosive breeder, as previously suggested for populations in Costa Rica and Ecuador (Gray, 1997; Ortega-Andrade et al., 2011). These variations among populations suggest that the reproductive pattern displayed by A. spurrelli might be influenced by ecological factors present at the breeding sites (Scott and Starret, 1974). It is well known that in the tropics ecological factors such as rainfall seasonality influence the reproductive behavior of anurans that depend on water for reproduction (Donnelly and Guyer, 1994; Bertoluci, 1998; Protázio et al., 2015). Therefore, A. spurrelli is likely able to combine both prolonged and explosive breeding patterns depending on whether weather conditions are favorable for reproduction and water availability is restricted for a short period (Gray, 1997). This opportunistic reproductive behavior suggests that in June sufficient water had accumulated in the pond to ensure the successful development of A. spurrelli tadpoles. In addition, frogs may decrease their reproductive effort at the end of the season to avoid the risk of larval mortality caused by the drying of the pond before completing their metamorphosis. Similar patterns have been observed in other hylids that breed in temporary ponds (Donnelly and Guyer, 1994).

In summary, we presented a detailed description of the vocalizations and calling behavior of Agalychnis spurrelli. Our results show that the acoustic signal and social interaction of this species are considerably more complex than those of other Agalychnis species. Males may have a graded aggressive signaling depending on the neighboring males' calling intensity. Further studies are required to determine if this behavior is advantageous for the reproductive success of males. Finally, this species presents a highly adapted reproductive behavior that is influenced by the ecological factors of its breeding localities.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank M. Rivera, A. Sosa, and D. Medina for their invaluable help during field observations. Also, we thank C. Villarreal, R. Beatty, J. Kyle, A. Rodriguez, and H. Pröhl for their comments that improved the manuscript. Funding was provided by a fellowship to R. Cossio and E. Medina-Barcenas from the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute.

REFERENCES

1.

Abrunhosa P.A., Wogel H. 2004. Breeding behavior of the leaf-frog Phyllomedusa burmeisteri (Anura: Hylidae). Amphibia-Reptilia 25:125–135.  DOI  Google Scholar

2.

Bagnara J.T., Iela L., Morrisett F., Rastogi R.K. 1986. Reproduction in the Mexican leaf frog (Pachymedusa dacnicolor) I. Behavioral and morphological aspects. Occasional Papers of the Museum of Natural History, University of Kansas 121:1–31. Google Scholar

3.

Bastos R.P., Haddad C.F.B. 2002. Acoustic and aggressive interactions in Scinax rizibilis (Anura: Hylidae) during the reproductive activity in southeastern Brazil. Amphibia-Reptilia 23:97–104. Google Scholar

4.

Bertoluci J. 1998. Annual patterns of breeding activity in Atlantic rainforest anurans. Journal of Herpetology 32:607–611.  DOI  Google Scholar

5.

Bioacoustics Research Program. 2011. Raven Pro: Interactive Sound Analysis Software Version 1.5. Available from:    http://www.birds.cornell.edu/ravenGoogle Scholar

6.

Boulenger G.A. 1882. Catalogue of the Batrachia Salientia s. Ecaudata in the Collection of the British Museum. Second Edition. Taylor and Francis, London.  DOI  Google Scholar

7.

Boulenger G.A. 1913. On a collection of batrachians and reptiles made by Dr. H.G.F. Spurrell, F.Z.S., in the Choco, Colombia. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 1913:1019–1038.  DOI  Google Scholar

8.

Brenowitz E.A. 1989. Neighbor call amplitude influences aggressive behavior and intermale spacing in choruses of the Pacific treefrog (Hyla regilla). Ethology 83:69–79.  DOI  Google Scholar

9.

Brenowitz E.A., Rose G.J. 1994. Behavioural plasticity mediates aggression in choruses of the Pacific treefrog. Animal Behaviour 47:633–641.  DOI  Google Scholar

10.

Briggs V.S. 2010. Call trait variation in Morelett's tree frog, Agalychnis moreletti, of Belize. Herpetologica 66:241–249.  DOI  Google Scholar

11.

Caldwell M.S., Johnston G.R., McDaniel J.G., Warkentin K.M. 2010. Vibrational signaling in the agonistic interactions of red-eyed treefrogs. Current Biology 20:1012–1017.  DOI  Google Scholar

12.

Cannatella D.C. 1980. A review of the Phyllomedusa buckleyi Group (Anura: Hylidae). Occasional Papers of the Museum of Natural History, The University of Kansas 87:1–40. Google Scholar

13.

Cope E.D. 1862. Catalogues of the reptiles obtained during the explorations of the Parana, Paraguay, Vermejo and Uraguay Rivers, by Capt. Thos. J. Page, U.S.N.; and of those procured by Lieut. N. Michler, U.S. Top. Eng., Commander of the expedition conducting the survey of the Atrato River. Proceedings of the Academy of Natural Sciences of Philadelphia 14:346–359. Google Scholar

14.

Cope E.D. 1864. Contributions to the herpetology of tropical America. Proceedings of the Academy of Natural Sciences of Philadelphia 16:166–181. Google Scholar

15.

Cortes S. 1897. Flora de Colombia. Papelería, imprenta y litografia de Samper Matiz, Bogotá. Google Scholar

16.

Cruz C.A.G. 1988. Sobre Phyllomedusa aspera e a descrição de uma espécie nova desse gênero (Amphibia, Anura, Hylidae). Arquivos Da Uni-versidade Federal Rural Do Rio de Janeiro 11:39–44. Google Scholar

17.

D'Orgeix C.A. 1996. Multiple Paternity and the Breeding Biology of the Red-eyed Treefrog, Agalychnis callidryas. Ph.D. Dissertation, Virginia Polytechnic Institute, USA. Google Scholar

18.

Dias T.M., dos Santos T.G., Maragno F.P., Oliveira V.F., Lima C., Cechin S.Z. 2017. Breeding biology, territoriality, and reproductive site use by Phyllomedusa iheringii (Anura: Phyllomedusidae) from the South American Pampa in Brazil. Salamandra 53:257–266. Google Scholar

19.

Donnelly M.A., Guyer C. 1994. Patterns of reproduction and habitat use in an assemblage of Neotropical hylid frogs. Oecologia 98:291–302.  DOI  Google Scholar

20.

Duellman W.E. 1963. A new species of tree frog, genus Phyllomedusa, from Costa Rica. Revista de Biología Tropical 11:1–23. Google Scholar

21.

Duellman W.E. 1970. The hylid frogs of Middle America. Vol. 1. Monographs of the Museum of Natural History, University of Kansas 1:1–753.  DOI  Google Scholar

22.

Duméril A.H.A. 1853. Mémoire sur les batraciens anoures, de la famille des hylaeformes ou rainettes, comprenent la description d'un genre nouveau et de onze espèces nouvelles. Annales des Sciences Na-turelles, Zoologie et Biologie Animale Serie 3 19:135–179. Google Scholar

23.

Funkhouser A. 1962. A new Phyllomedusa from Venezuela. Copeia 1962:588–590.  DOI  Google Scholar

24.

Gerhardt H.C., Bee M.A. 2006. Recognition and localization of acoustic signals. Pp. 113–146, in Narins P.M., Feng A.S., Fay R.R., Popper A.N. (Eds.), Hearing and Sound Communication in Amphibians. Springer, New York. Google Scholar

25.

Gerhardt H.C., Hubber F. 2002. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and Diverse Solutions. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Google Scholar

26.

Gomez-Mestre I., Wiens J.J., Warkentin K.M. 2008. Evolution of adaptive plasticity: risk-sensitive hatching in Neotropical leaf-breeding treefrogs (Agalychnis, Hylidae). Ecological Monographs 78:205–224.  DOI  Google Scholar

27.

Gray A.R. 1997. Observations on the biology of Agalychnis spurrelli from the Caribbean lowlands of Costa Rica. Journal of the International Herpetological Society 22:61–70. Google Scholar

28.

Hertz A., Lotzkat S. 2010. Notes on vocalization and natural history of Hylomantis medinae (Funkhouser, 1962)(Anura, Hylidae) from Northern Venezuela. Salamandra 46:121–124. Google Scholar

29.

Heyer W.R., Donnelly M.A., Mc Diarmid R.W., Hayek L.C., Foster M.S. 2001. Medición y monitoreo de la diversidad biológica: Métodos estandarizados para anfibios. Editorial Universitaria de La Patagonia, Chubut. Google Scholar

30.

IBM Corporation. 2012. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows. Version 21.0. IBM Corp., Armonk, NY. Google Scholar

31.

Ibáñez R., Rand S.A., Jaramillo C.A. 1999. Los Anfibios del Monumento Natural Barro Colorado, Parque Nacional Soberanía y Areas Adyacentes. Editorial Mizrachi and Pujol S.A., Panama. Google Scholar

32.

Jungfer K.H., Weygoldt P. 1994. The reproductive biology of the leaf frog Phyllomedusa lemur Boulenger, 1882, and a comparison with other members of the Phyllomedusinae (Anura: Hylidae). Revue Française d'Aquariologie 21:57–64. Google Scholar

33.

Köhler J., Jansen M., Rodríguez A., Kok P.J.R., Toledo L.F., Emmrich M., … Vences M. 2017. The use of bioacoustics in anuran taxonomy: theory, terminology, methods and recommendations for best practice. Zootaxa 425:1–124.  DOI  Google Scholar

34.

Kubicki B. 2004. Leaf-frogs of Costa Rica. Editorial INBio, Costa Rica. Google Scholar

35.

Lee J.C. 1996. The Amphibian and Reptiles of the Yucatán Peninsula. Cornell University Press, New York. Google Scholar

36.

Leigh E.G. 1999. Tropical Forest Ecology: A View from Barro Colorado Island. Oxford University Press on Demand, Oxford. Google Scholar

37.

Littlejohn M.J. 1977. Long-range acoustic communication in anurans: an integrated and evolutionary approach. Pp. 263–294, in Taylor D.H., Guttman S.I. (Eds.), The Reproductive Biology of Amphibians. Plenum, New York.  DOI  Google Scholar

38.

Martins M., Pombal J.P. Jr., Haddad C.F.B. 1998. Escalated aggressive behaviour and facultative parental care in the nest building gladiator frog, Hyla faber . Amphibia-Reptilia 19:65–73.  DOI  Google Scholar

39.

Oliveira F.F.R., Nogueira P.A.G., Eterovick P.C. 2012. Natural history of Phyllomedusa megacephala (Miranda-Ribeiro, 1926) (Anura: Hylidae) in southeastern Brazil, with descriptions of its breeding biology and male territorial behaviour. Journal of Natural History 46:117–129.  DOI  Google Scholar

40.

Ortega-Andrade H.M. 2008. Agalychnis spurrelli Boulenger (Anura, Hylidae): variación, distribución y sinonimia. Papéis Avulsos de Zoologia 48:103–117.  DOI  Google Scholar

41.

Ortega-Andrade H.M., Tobar-Suárez C., Arellano M. 2011. Tamaño poblacional, uso del hábitat y relaciones interespecíficas de Agalychnis spurrelli (Anura: Hylidae) en un bosque húmedo tropical remanente del Noroccidente de Ecuador. Papéis Avulsos de Zoologia 51:1–19.  DOI  Google Scholar

42.

Peters W.C.H. 1873. Über eine, zwei neue Gattungen enthaltende, Sammlung von Batrachiern des Hrn. Dr. O. Wucherer aus Bahia, so wie über einge neue oder weniger bekannte Saurier. Monatsberi-chte der Königlichen Preussische Akademie des Wissenschaften zu Berlin 1872:768–772. Google Scholar

43.

Pimenta B.V.S., Nunes I., Gonçalves C.A. 2007. Notes on the poorly known Phyllomedusine frog Hylomantis aspera Peters, 1872 (Anura, Hylidae). South American Journal of Herpetology 2:206–214.  DOI  Google Scholar

44.

Protázio A.S., Albuquerque R.L., Falkenberg L.M., Mesquita D.O. 2015. Acoustic ecology of an anuran assemblage in the arid Caatinga of northeastern Brazil. Journal of Natural History 49:15–16.  DOI  Google Scholar

45.

Pröhl H. 1997. Territorial behaviour of the strawberry poison-dart frog, Dendrobates pumilio . Amphibia-Reptilia 18:437–442.  DOI  Google Scholar

46.

Pyburn W.F. 1970. Breeding behavior of the leaf-frogs Phyllomedusa callidryas and Phyllomedusa dacnicolor in Mexico. Copeia 1970:209–218.  DOI  Google Scholar

47.

Rand A.S., Ryan M.J. 1981. The adaptive significance of a complex vocal repertoire in a Neotropical frog. Zeitschrift Für Tierpsychologie 57:209–214.  DOI  Google Scholar

48.

Roberts W.E. 1994. Explosive breeding aggregations and parachuting in a Neotropical frog, Agalychnis saltator (Hylidae). Journal of Herpetology 28:193–199.  DOI  Google Scholar

49.

Rose G.J., Brenowitz E.A. 1991. Aggressive thresholds of male Pacific treefrogs for advertisement calls vary with amplitude of neighbors' calls. Ethology 89:244–252.  DOI  Google Scholar

50.

Schwartz J.J., Bee M.A. 2013. Anuran acoustic signal production in noisy environments. Pp. 91–132, in Brumm H. (Ed.), Animal Communication and Noise. Springer, New York.  DOI  Google Scholar

51.

Schwartz J.J., Buchanan B.W., Gerhardt H. 2002. Acoustic interactions among male gray treefrogs, Hyla versicolor, in a chorus setting. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 53:9–19.  DOI  Google Scholar

52.

Schwartz J.J., Wells K.D. 1984. Interspecific acoustic interactions of the Neotropical treefrog Hyla ebraccata . Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 14:211–224.  DOI  Google Scholar

53.

Schwartz J.J., Wells K.D. 1985. Intra-and interspecific vocal behavior of the Neotropical treefrog Hyla Microcephala . Copeia 1985:27–38.  DOI  Google Scholar

54.

Scott N.J., Starret A. 1974. An unusual breeding aggregation of frogs, with notes on the ecology of Agalychnis spurrelli (Anura: Hylidae). Bulletin of the Southern California Academy of Sciences 73:86–94. Google Scholar

55.

Serrano M., Cruz J.J., Villatoro-Castañeda M., Ariano-Sánchez D. 2018. Tremulation display in male agonistic behavior of the black-eyed leaf frog (Agalychnis moreletii: Hylidae). Animal Behavior and Cognition 5:222–228.  DOI  Google Scholar

56.

Taylor E.H. 1955. Additions to the Known Herpetological Fauna of Costa Rica with Comments on Other Species. No. II. The University of Kansas Science Bulletin 37:499–575.  DOI  Google Scholar

57.

Toledo L.F., Martins I.A., Bruschi D.P., Passos M.A., Alexandre C., Haddad C.F.B. 2015. The anuran calling repertoire in the light of social context. Acta Ethologica 18:87–99.  DOI  Google Scholar

58.

Vargas F., Bolaños M.E., Berrío-B H. 2000. Notas sobre la ecología reproductiva de Agalychnis spurrelli (Anura: Hylidae) en una población de Anchicayá, Pacífico Colombiano. Revista de La Academia Colombiana de Ciencias Exactas, Físicas Y Naturales 24:85–99. Google Scholar

59.

Venâncio N.M., Melo-Sampaio P.R. 2010. Reproductive behavior of the giant leaf frog Phyllomedusa bicolor (Anura: Hylidae) in the Western Amazon. Phyllomedusa 9:63–67.  DOI  Google Scholar

60.

Vilela B., Lisboa B.S., Nascimento F.A.C. 2015. Reproduction of Agalychnis granulosa Cruz, 1989 (Anura: Hylidae). Journal of Natural History 49:709–717.  DOI  Google Scholar

61.

Wells K.D. 1977. The social behaviour of anuran amphibians. Animal Behaviour 25:666–693.  DOI  Google Scholar

62.

Wells K.D. 1988. The effect of social interactions on anuran vocal behavior. Pp. 433–454, in Fritszch B., Ryan M.J., Wilczynski W., Hetherington T.E., Walkowiak W. (Eds.), The Evolution of the Amphibian Auditory System. Wiley, New York. Google Scholar

63.

Wells K.D. 2007. The Ecology and Behavior of Amphibians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.  DOI  Google Scholar

64.

Wells K.D., Greer B.J. 1981. Vocal responses to conspecific calls in a Neotropical hylid frog, Hyla ebraccata . Copeia 1981:615–624.  DOI  Google Scholar

65.

Wells K.D., Schwartz J.J. 1984a. Vocal communication in a Neotropical treefrog, Hyla ebraccata: advertisement calls. Animal Behaviour 32:405–420.  DOI  Google Scholar

66.

Wells K.D., Schwartz J.J. 1984b. Vocal communication in a Neotropical treefrog, Hyla ebraccata: aggressive calls. Behaviour 91:128–145.  DOI  Google Scholar

67.

Wells K.D., Schwartz J.J. 2006. The behavioral ecology of anuran communication. Pp. 44–86, in Narins P.M., Feng A.S., Fay R.R., Popper A.N. (Eds.), Hearing and Sound Communication in Amphibians. Springer, New York.  DOI  Google Scholar

68.

Wogel H., Abrunhosa P.A., Pombal J.P. Jr. 2005. Breeding behaviour and mating success of Phyllomedusa rohdei (Anura, Hylidae) in southeastern Brazil. Journal of Natural History 39:2035–2045.  DOI  Google Scholar
© 2020 Brazilian Society of Herpetology
Ricardo Cossio and Eduardo Medina-Barcenas "Acoustic Repertoire and Calling Behavior of the Gliding Treefrog, Agalychnis spurrelli (Anura: Hylidae)," South American Journal of Herpetology 17(1), 71-78, (31 August 2020). https://doi.org/10.2994/SAJH-D-18-00040.1
Received: 6 June 2018; Accepted: 27 November 2018; Published: 31 August 2020
KEYWORDS
Agonistic encounters
Chorus activity
reproductive behavior
vocalization
Back to Top