Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
4 November 2015 Competition and aggression for nest cavities between Striated Pardalotes and endangered Forty-spotted Pardalotes
Amanda B. Edworthy
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

The Forty-spotted Pardalote (Pardalotus quadragintus) is an endangered songbird with specialized habitat requirements, including Eucalyptus viminalis trees for foraging and tree cavities for nesting. The species was originally distributed throughout eastern Tasmania, Australia, but habitat loss and fragmentation resulted in the contraction of its range to just 3 islands and several remnant mainland patches, primarily in the southeast of the state. The species' remaining habitat is exclusively second-growth forest, with reduced nest-cavity availability, and it competes for cavities with a common generalist, the Striated Pardalote (Pardalotus substriatus). This study documents the frequency of cavity takeover across the major populations of Forty-spotted Pardalotes on Maria Island, Bruny Island, and mainland Tasmania. Additionally, the intensity of interspecific aggression by pardalotes toward conspecific and heterospecific competitors at nest sites was assessed using a model presentation experiment. Striated Pardalotes usurped ~10% of Forty-spotted Pardalote nest sites across all study areas, and up to 17% of cavities within a single region (mainland Tasmania). Conversely, Forty-spotted Pardalotes never usurped Striated Pardalote nests. Most takeovers (79%) occurred during the nest-building stage, although Striated Pardalotes removed or crushed Forty-spotted Pardalote eggs in 21% of takeovers (4 nests). However, there was no change in nest defense aggression across nest stages. Striated Pardalotes displayed equal aggression toward conspecific and heterospecific models, whereas Forty-spotted Pardalotes were more aggressive toward conspecifics. These results show that Striated Pardalotes are the dominant competitor for nest cavities, and reduce the breeding success of Forty-spotted Pardalotes by usurping their nest sites. Nest boxes are a promising option for restoring the availability of nest sites for Forty-spotted Pardalotes, but given the competition from Striated Pardalotes for nest sites, nest-box placement should take advantage of species differences in nest-site selection to minimize conflict.

INTRODUCTION

The Forty-spotted Pardalote (Pardalotus quadragintus) is an endangered songbird found in forests and woodlands of eastern Tasmania, Australia, where it is currently experiencing unexplained decline within existing habitat. These birds are specialists of Eucalyptus viminalis (white gum trees). They rely strongly on manna, a sugary exudate of E. viminalis foliage, for food. Within E. viminalis habitat, they also usually use tree cavities for nesting (Woinarski and Rounsevell 1983). Historically, Forty-spotted Pardalotes were found throughout eastern Tasmania, but their distribution contracted with forest clearing and fragmentation following European settlement. Currently, they are limited to an area <4,500 ha in size, with major populations in southeastern Tasmania on Maria Island (~1,000 individuals), Bruny Island (~500 individuals), and mainland Tasmania on Tinderbox Peninsula near Bruny Island (<100 individuals), as well as a small population in the northeast on Flinders Island (Bryant 2010). Although much of their habitat is protected, within the past 18 yr their population has declined by 60%, to ~1,500 birds (Bryant 2010). Remaining Forty-spotted Pardalote habitat is exclusively second-growth forest, which typically has limited nest-cavity availability (Newton 1994, Wiebe 2011). Strong competition for cavities often arises in cavity-limited systems (Heinsohn et al. 2003, Dhondt 2011), and competition with Striated Pardalotes (Pardalotus substriatus) is a potential threat to Forty-spotted Pardalotes (Woinarski and Rounsevell 1983).

Competition for nest cavities is common within communities of cavity-nesting birds (Martin and Eadie 1999, Dhondt 2011). The combination of limited cavity availability and strong selection pressure toward predator-safe cavities (e.g., those with small entrances, deep chambers, and high above the ground) results in direct competition for high-quality cavities (Nilsson 1984, Newton 1994). Although competition often excludes weaker species or individuals from high-quality resources, sustained competition for nest cavities is relatively common among closely matched cavity-nesting birds (Martin et al. 2004, Dhondt 2011). Weaker competitors for nest cavities may be forced to use poor-quality cavities (Kempanaers and Dhondt 1991), be prevented from acquiring a cavity (Dhondt and Adriaenson 1999), have their cavities usurped (Brazill-Boast et al. 2011), or may even be killed in conflict (Merilä and Wiggins 1995). These impacts can have population-level consequences, and competition for nest sites is a major threat to some endangered birds (Minot and Perrins 1986, Cade and Temple 1995, Cooper et al. 2007).

In competitive interactions, larger species usually dominate because body size is related to strength, weaponry, and cost of conflict (Persson 1985, Jennions and Backwell 1996). But in closely matched competitors, size differences may be overcome by aggressiveness, residency advantage, or shifts in the value of the resource to either competitor caused by parental investment or the difficulty of evicting eggs or nestlings (Slagsvold 1975, 1978, Gowaty 1981, Krist 2004). For example, Ingold (1998) found that an aggressive species, the European Starling (Sturnus vulgaris; 60–96 g), was able to evict a species twice its size, the Northern Flicker (Colaptes auratus; 110–160 g), from 27 of 40 nests at a site in Ohio, USA. For several cavity-nesting birds, the nest-building and egg-laying stages experience the highest takeover rates compared with later stages (Slagsvold 1975, Knight and Temple 1986). Possibly, the presence of nestlings complicates nest takeover because of the need for intruders to eject or build over the top of moving nestlings, which creates a strong discrepancy in the value of the resource between competitors (Krist 2004). Thus, the nesting stage may influence vulnerability to takeover for some species.

Striated Pardalotes (13.6 ± 0.7 g [SD]) compete with Forty-spotted Pardalotes (11.2 ± 1.2 g) for both nest sites and food (Rounsevell and Woinarski 1983, A. B. Edworthy personal observation). In contrast to Forty-spotted Pardalotes, Striated Pardalotes are common generalists, distributed throughout much of Australia (Woinarski and Bulman 1985, Birdlife International 2012). Woinarski and Bulman (1985) found that Forty-spotted Pardalotes were more frequently the aggressor in competitive interactions within their foraging territories, and concluded that Forty-spotted Pardalotes were dominant over Striated Pardalotes. However, I observed Striated Pardalotes (Striateds) evicting Forty-spotted Pardalotes (Forty-spotteds) from nest cavities. Because nest cavities are a discrete, limited resource, the outcome of contests for this resource can be easily assessed and offers the opportunity to assess dominance between pardalote species more clearly. The 2 species compete for similar cavities, though Striateds more frequently use cavities in living wood, which provide a more stable microclimate than those in dead wood, and Forty-spotteds are more likely to use cavities with entrances inclined above the vertical, which increases their exposure to rain (Woinarski and Bulman 1985, Wiebe 2001). Both species are socially monogamous, raise 1–2 broods per year, and primarily nest in tree cavities (or nest boxes), but occasionally use ground burrows (Higgins and Peter 2002). Conflict for nest cavities (Figure 1A) arises during the beginning of the breeding season in mid-August and can last through to early December when the last of the Striateds initiate breeding (A. B. Edworthy personal observation). Intruding Striateds inspect a cavity, perch at the entrance (or sit on top of the box), and sometimes call (Figure 1B). Forty-spotteds defend their nests by chasing off intruders repeatedly, with both members of the pair involved in nest and territory defense. Both conspecific and heterospecific conflict occur, and may result in energy loss, delayed breeding, nest takeover, and loss of eggs (Figure 1C). Forty-spotteds are resident in Tasmania and initiate breeding up to 3 weeks earlier than Striateds, which are at least partially migratory (Woinarski and Bulman 1985, Higgins and Peter 2002). As a result, competition from Striateds often occurs after Forty-spotteds have established nests, and the influence of breeding stage on competitive interactions may have important consequences for Forty-spotted Pardalote populations.

FIGURE 1.

Examples and consequences of competition for nest cavities between Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes in southeastern Tasmania, Australia, 2013–2015. (A) Forty-spotted Pardalotes in aerial combat on Bruny Island, Tasmania, Australia (Photo credit: Alfred Schulte). (B) Striated Pardalote defending a recently usurped Forty-spotted Pardalote nest cavity (Photo credit: Mick Brown). (C) Forty-spotted Pardalote eggs punctured and removed from a cavity by Striated Pardalotes (Photo credit: Amanda Edworthy).

i0010-5422-118-1-1-f01.tif

I use a 2-yr study of pardalote breeding biology to investigate the intensity and impact of interspecific competition for nest cavities on Forty-spotted Pardalotes. Study sites were selected in areas containing the 2 major island populations and largest remaining mainland Tasmanian population of Forty-spotted Pardalotes, and thus allow assessment of competition as a threat across much of their distribution. In this paper, I: (1) report the frequency and timing of Forty-spotted Pardalote nest takeovers by Striated Pardalotes, (2) use a model presentation experiment to investigate relative levels of nest-defense aggression by Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes in response to heterospecific and conspecific competitors, and (3) test whether nest defense by pardalotes increases during the egg and nestling periods.

METHODS

Study Sites

I conducted fieldwork in dry coastal forests and woodlands of Maria Island (5 patches, 2–19 ha; 42.65°S, 148.05°E), Bruny Island (8 patches, 4–15 ha; 43.10°S, 147.36°E), and Tinderbox Peninsula (4 patches, 5–20 ha; 43.04°S, 147.32°E), Tasmania, Australia. Patches were sections of forest containing Eucalyptus viminalis trees, surrounded by either native forest or cleared farmland. Some patches were dominated by E. viminalis, while others contained a mix of E. viminalis, E. pulchella, E. globulus, E. obliqua, E. ovata, and E. amygdalina trees (Table 1, Figure 2). For this study, patches were grouped into sites, which reflected their geographical proximity and forest type (Table 1). There were 2 sites on Maria Island: Isthmus, a single patch of E. viminalis–dominated forest; and North Maria Island, composed of 4 patches of mixed forest in the north of the island. There were 2 sites on Bruny Island: Waterview Hill, composed of 2 patches of E. viminalis–dominated forest on opposite sides of Waterview Hill; and Murrayfield, composed of 5 patches of mixed forest on Murrayfield Farm. Finally, there were 2 sites on mainland Tasmania: Howden, a single patch of E. viminalis within the Peter Murrell Nature Reserve, at the base of Tinderbox Peninsula; and Tinderbox Ridge, a string of small clumps or individual E. viminalis trees spread across the length of the ridge. Bruny Island and Tinderbox Peninsula are separated by a 1.4-km channel, and Forty-spotted Pardalotes may disperse from the larger population on Bruny Island to Tinderbox Peninsula, which supports 1 of the 2 remaining mainland Tasmanian populations. Maria Island is 65 km NE of Bruny Island, and separated from mainland Tasmania by a 4-km channel. Sixty percent of Forty-spotted Pardalote habitat is protected in reserves, including Maria Island National Park, which protects the whole of Maria Island; 2 state reserves on Tinderbox Peninsula, including the Howden study site (Peter Murrell Nature Reserve) and Magazine Reserve on Tinderbox Ridge; and Pierson's Point, a municipal reserve at the tip of Tinderbox Peninsula (Kingborough Council; Bryant 2010). On Bruny Island, the Murrayfield sites are the property of the Indigenous Land Corporation, and the Waterview sites are privately owned.

TABLE 1.

Summary of site characteristics, number of pairs located at each site, and number of takeovers per nesting attempt (including only attempts that were monitored starting before eggs were laid) of Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes in southeastern Tasmania, Australia, 2013–2015. Forest types included “viminalis,” which was dominated by Eucalyptus viminalis (white gum trees), the main forage tree for Forty-spotted Pardalotes, and “mixed,” which included a mix of Eucalyptus tree species including E. viminalis, E. globulus, E. pulchella, E. obliqua, E. amygdalina, and/or E. ovata. Striated Pardalotes were the usurpers in all Forty-spotted Pardalote nest takeover events, except for a single takeover at Maria Island Isthmus by Tree Martins (not shown).

i0010-5422-118-1-1-t01.eps

FIGURE 2.

Habitat in southeastern Tasmania, Australia, at (A) the Isthmus site on Maria Island, dominated by Eucalyptus viminalis (Photo credit: Amanda Edworthy), and (B) a Murrayfield patch on Bruny Island, composed of mixed Eucalyptus species (Photo credit: Linda Edworthy).

i0010-5422-118-1-1-f02.tif

Both pardalote species were common in the study sites. Although these species occasionally nest in ground burrows, only 3 Striated Pardalote pairs nested in burrows, with all other nests in natural tree cavities or nest boxes (Table 1). Most nests in boxes were in the Waterview Hill study site, where 100 nest boxes were installed in 2008 (Table 1). However, there were also 2 existing nest boxes on Tinderbox Peninsula, 1 of which was used. In July 2013, I installed an additional 153 boxes (46 on Maria Island, 51 on Bruny Island, and 56 on mainland Tasmania), but the time lag for box uptake by pardalotes is >2 yr. Only 8 of these new boxes were used by pardalotes, all within a single patch in the Murrayfield site.

Nest Monitoring for Breeding and Takeover Rates

I located nests of Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes in both nest boxes and natural cavities by searching potential nest sites and by following birds to their cavities during August to January in 2013–2015. To provide an estimate of pardalote breeding density at each site, I report all pairs located, regardless of whether they were found early enough in the nesting cycle (prelaying) to use in the takeover analysis (Table 1). However, the number of Striated Pardalotes was likely underestimated in the Murrayfield, Howden, and Tinderbox Ridge sites due to large site areas and lower search effort for Striated Pardalote nests.

To assess nesting stage, identity of adult birds defending the nest, and interactions between Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes at nest sites, nests were monitored every 4 days using ladders for cavities and nest boxes up to 7 m high, climbing ropes for cavities up to 30 m high, or behavioral observations from the ground for inaccessible cavities. Where possible, nest contents were inspected using a video camera on a flexible stalk (Burrowcam; Faunatech, Mount Taylor, Victoria, Australia). Nest takeovers were defined as cases in which: (1) a nest site was occupied by a pardalote pair (building, defending, laying, incubating, or feeding nestlings); (2) aggressive interaction (e.g., chasing) was observed between the resident pair and an intruder of a different species; and (3) on subsequent checks, the intruder species had ownership of the nest site (e.g., building, defending, or laying). Forty-spotted Pardalotes were color-banded to identify individuals; 124 of 156 pairs (79%) had at least 1 banded member during the study. Because Forty-spotted Pardalotes are socially monogamous during the breeding season, I could generally distinguish between pairs. Fourteen of the 20 pairs who had their cavities usurped had at least 1 banded member, and I continued to monitor their activity following takeover to determine whether they renested elsewhere.

Model Presentation Experiment

I conducted a model presentation experiment to assess relative levels of aggression toward heterospecific and conspecific intruders by Forty-spotted Pardalotes (n = 15 nests; 12 boxes and 3 natural cavities) and Striated Pardalotes (n = 16 nests; 15 boxes and 1 natural cavity). At each nest I conducted 3 trials, presenting freeze-dried specimens (“dummies” or “models”) of a Forty-spotted Pardalote, a Striated Pardalote, and a noncompetitor control. The models were perched in a life-like position with wings folded. Intruding competitors typically land at the cavity entrance, display (Striated only), and often call. To approximate this behavior, at nest boxes I used a telescoping pole to place the model (perched on a small wire stand) on top of the box, and at natural cavities I perched the model on a wire hanger and placed it within 1 m of the cavity entrance. To reduce the response of target pairs to my presence, I placed the model while both members of the pair were foraging away from the nest tree. For conspecific and heterospecific models I used 2 specimens of each pardalote species, and for noncompetitor models I used 1 of either Grey Fantail (Rhipidura albiscapa) or Silvereye (Zosterops lateralis), which are open-cup nesters and forage in the understory. To randomize trial order, I used the “sample” function in the statistical program R, generating a random list of numbers from 1 to 6 for the 6 possible trial orders, with each trial order represented 2 or 3 times for both Forty-spotted and Striated pardalote nests (blocked randomization; R Development Core Team 2012). Subsequent trials were conducted within 1–3 days of each other.

I started the trial when at least 1 member of the resident pair returned to the nest tree. Trials ended after 10 min, or when the pardalotes pecked the model repeatedly or knocked it to the ground. During each trial, I estimated the distance of the resident birds to the model and recorded their behavior, including: (1) no response; (2) agitation: short hops and rapid wing extensions; (3) aggressive approach: swoop toward or hover at the model; (4) display: wings and tail fanned, crest raised; and (5) contact: direct attack with feet or bill. I could not consistently distinguish between sexes or individuals because their bands were too small to see during the experiment and both species lack obvious sexual dimorphism, so I used the strongest response (closest approach and most aggressive behavior) from each pair for the analysis. The trials were conducted within an 8-week period (September 28–November 18, 2013), using pairs that were nesting simultaneously in different locations, so I was confident that each of the pairs used in the experiment was unique; where possible, this was confirmed by identification of color-banded birds.

Statistical Analysis

I used a linear mixed-effects model to estimate relative aggression by Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes toward competitor dummies and across nest stages. All variables of interest and their potential interactions were included in a single model. Minimum distance of approach to the competitor dummy was used as the measure of aggression (response variable), and was transformed by (ln(x + 1 cm)) to normalize the distribution. Fixed effects included species of nest defender (Forty-spotted Pardalote or Striated Pardalote), competitor dummy type (heterospecific, conspecific, or noncompetitor [Grey Fantail or Silvereye]), and nest stage (prelaying, or eggs or nestlings present), as well as the 2-way interactions of species of nest defender with competitor dummy type and nest stage, and the 3-way interaction of species of nest defender, competitor dummy type, and nest stage. The “prelaying” category included the period from the beginning of nest building to the start of laying. Nest ID was included in the model as a random effect to account for repeated sampling of individual pairs (3 competitor dummies presented once each per pair; Laird and Ware 1982). After log-transforming the dependent variable, both the assumptions of homogeneity of variance (Levene's test: F = 1.29, P = 0.24) and normality (Shapiro-Wilk test: W = 0.98, P = 0.30) were met and verified by examining residual vs. fitted values and Q-Q plots. Model fit was assessed using marginal R2 to describe the proportion of variance explained by the fixed factors alone, and conditional R2 to estimate the proportion of variance described by the fixed and random effects together (Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2013).

In addition to using the closest distance of approach to the competitor dummy as a measure of aggression, I calculated the frequency of the maximum level of aggression displayed in each trial type, ordered from least to most aggressive as (1) no response, (2) aggressive approach, including swoop or hover, and (3) display or contact. Display and contact were grouped together because displays were used only by Striated Pardalotes, and were a highly aggressive response (usually within 5–15 cm of the model). I used chi-square tests to assess differences in the frequencies of responses to competitor dummies between the 2 pardalote species. Values are reported as mean ± standard error (SE). All analyses were done using R, including the nlme package (R Development Core Team 2012, Pinheiro et al. 2013).

RESULTS

Frequency and Timing of Nest Takeovers

Striated Pardalotes usurped the nests of Forty-spotted Pardalotes in 19 of 192 (~10%) nesting attempts (Table 1). There were no cases of Forty-spotted Pardalotes taking over Striated Pardalote nests (n = 91 nests). Takeover rates were highest on mainland Tasmania (~17%, n = 18 nests) and Bruny Island (~15%, n = 104 nests). On Maria Island, there were no takeovers by Striated Pardalotes, but Tree Martins (Petrochelidon nigricans) took over a single Forty-spotted Pardalote nest (n = 70 nests; Table 1). Eighty percent of takeovers by Striated Pardalotes (and the single takeover by a Tree Martin) took place while Forty-spotted Pardalotes were nest building, prior to egg laying (16 of 20 takeovers); in the remaining 20% (4 cases), Striated Pardalotes punctured and removed Forty-spotted Pardalote eggs (Figure 1C), or crushed eggs in the nest and then removed shell fragments and replaced some of the nest material. Four of the 14 banded Forty-spotted Pardalote pairs (29%) that experienced takeovers by Striated Pardalotes or Tree Martins nevertheless managed to breed successfully in an earlier or later attempt within the same breeding season. Five pairs had their nests usurped during their second nesting attempt. Four of 14 pairs (29%) with banded individuals renested in new locations: 2 pairs moved from nest boxes into natural cavities nearby, 1 pair moved between natural cavities, and the last pair moved from one nest box to a second box on the same tree (2 of these attempts were successful).

During all takeovers, I observed Forty-spotted Pardalotes chasing intruding Striated Pardalotes prior to takeover. During 1 takeover, Striated and Forty-spotted pardalotes were observed engaging in frequent chases and displays (e.g., 20 chases hr−1) over a period of 2 days.

Model Presentation Experiment

The closest distance of approach to the competitor dummy was influenced by both species of nest defender and competitor type, but there was no effect of nest stage (Table 2, Figures 3 and 4). The fixed effects explained 22% of the variance (marginal R2 = 0.221), and, by taking pair ID into account as a random effect, the full model explained 43% of the variation (conditional R2 = 0.433).

TABLE 2.

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) results for a generalized linear mixed-effects model predicting distance approached to the competitor dummy (transformed by ln(x + 1)) as a function of species of nest defender (Forty-spotted Pardalote, n = 15; or Striated Pardalote, n = 16), competitor dummy type (conspecific, heterospecific, and noncompetitor control [Grey Fantail or Silvereye]), nest stage (prelaying [n = 33], or eggs or nestlings present [n = 29]), and all relevant interactions for Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes in southeastern Tasmania, Australia, 2013. Nest ID was included in the model as a random factor to account for repeated trials at individual nests. Bold font denotes statistical significance (P < 0.05).

i0010-5422-118-1-1-t02.eps

FIGURE 3.

Mean closest approach distance (m) of Forty-spotted Pardalotes (light gray bars; n = 15 trials per model) and Striated Pardalotes (dark gray bars; n = 16 trials per model) to competitor dummies (heterospecific, conspecific, and noncompetitor control [Grey Fantail or Silvereye]) placed at nest sites in southeastern Tasmania, Australia, 2013. Means were averaged across nest stages (prelaying, eggs present, and nestlings present). The means and standard errors (error bars) were back-transformed from log-transformed values of approach distance (ln(x + 1 cm)). Smaller values of approach distance (lower on the y-axis) were interpreted as higher levels of aggression. Asterisks denote significant differences (P < 0.05) between responses to competitor dummy types within each species of nest defender.

i0010-5422-118-1-1-f03.tif

FIGURE 4.

Effects of nesting stage (prelaying [n = 33], or eggs or nestlings present [n = 29]) on mean closest distance of approach (m) by Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes to competitor dummies (set to heterospecific in this graph) placed at nest sites in southeastern Tasmania, Australia, 2013. The means and standard errors (error bars) were back-transformed from log-transformed values of approach distance (ln(x + 1 cm)). Smaller values of approach distance (lower on the y-axis) were interpreted as higher levels of aggression. The effect of nest stage on approach distance was nonsignificant (F = 1.60, P = 0.21), as was the interaction between species and nest stage (F = 0.01, P = 0.93) and the 3-way interaction of species, nest stage, and competitor type (F = 0.16, P = 0.85).

i0010-5422-118-1-1-f04.tif

Striated Pardalotes approached to within similar distances of heterospecific and conspecific competitor dummies (0.05 ± 0.03 m and 0.04 ± 0.03 m, respectively; F1,15 = 0.02, P = 0.88; Figure 3). Forty-spotted Pardalotes approached conspecific models more closely than heterospecific dummies (0.01 ± 0.00 m vs. 0.06 ± 0.04 m; F1,14 = 6.86, P = 0.02), which suggests that Striated Pardalotes were seen as the more threatening opponent by Forty-spotted Pardalotes (Figure 3). Noncompetitor controls were not approached as closely as heterospecific (F1,26 = 5.48, P = 0.03) or conspecific dummies (F1,25 = 28.30, P < 0.001) by either species of defending pardalote (Figure 3). The effect of nest stage was nonsignificant across species and competitor dummy type, indicating no change in nest defense aggression toward any competitor after eggs were laid for either Forty-spotted or Striated pardalotes (Table 2, Figure 4).

Both Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes made contact with intruding competitor dummies by pecking or grabbing with their claws, but only Striated Pardalotes used displays (wings and tail fanned, crest raised), typically within 5–15 cm of the model. Patterns in the most aggressive behavior displayed mirrored patterns of aggression seen in closest approach distance: Striated Pardalotes displayed or made contact with both conspecifics and heterospecifics in the same percentage of trials (75%; n = 16 trials, χ2 = 0.00, P = 1.00), whereas Forty-spotted Pardalotes made contact more frequently with conspecifics (87%) than with heterospecifics (40%; n = 15 trials, χ2 = 3.90, P = 0.048; Figure 5). Both Forty-spotted and Striated pardalotes were less aggressive toward noncompetitor dummies in their use of displays and/or contact (20% vs. 63%, respectively, in Forty-spotted Pardalotes [n = 15 trials, χ2 = 7.52, P = 0.006], and 19% vs. 75% in Striated Pardalotes [n = 16 trials, χ2 = 13.71, P < 0.001]; Figure 5).

FIGURE 5.

Relative frequencies of the most aggressive behavior displayed by Forty-spotted Pardalotes (n = 15 trials per competitor dummy type) and Striated Pardalotes (n = 16 trials per competitor) in response to presentations of 3 competitor dummies (heterospecific, conspecific, and noncompetitor control [Grey Fantail or Silvereye]) placed at nest sites in southeastern Tasmania, Australia, 2013. Circles represent frequencies of zero.

i0010-5422-118-1-1-f05.tif

DISCUSSION

The results of this study show that competition for nest cavities reduces breeding opportunities for Forty-spotted Pardalotes. Competition for cavities is an important factor to consider in conservation management of the species. Striated Pardalotes (Striateds) were the dominant competitor for nest cavities, displaying greater aggression toward heterospecifics than Forty-spotted Pardalotes (Forty-spotteds), and evicting ~10% of Forty-spotted breeding pairs across all study sites. However, there was strong variation in takeover rates among regions, with no usurpations on Maria Island and high rates on Bruny Island and mainland Tasmania. Takeovers had a substantial impact on Forty-spotted breeding success. Sixteen of 20 pairs that had their nests usurped produced no offspring for the year; just 4 pairs nested successfully in an earlier or subsequent attempt in a new location. While these results show a clear negative impact on Forty-spotted Pardalotes, the impact of Forty-spotteds on Striated Pardalotes is uncertain. If Striated usurpers save nest-building time and energy by taking over Forty-spotted nests (both species add and reuse nest material in cavities), then their interaction might be kleptoparasitism rather than competition (Kappes 1997). However, probable costs to Striateds include energy spent usurping Forty-spotted nests (e.g., frequent chases over up to 2 days), and likely reduced or delayed access to cavities that are occupied early in the breeding season by Forty-spotteds. These impacts are forms of interference and exploitative competition, respectively.

All remaining Forty-spotted Pardalote habitat is second-growth forest, and limited cavity availability in these forests may intensify competition for nest sites (e.g., Brazill-Boast et al. 2013). However, relative population densities of the 2 pardalote species also appeared to have a strong impact on takeover rates. Takeovers were most frequent in the Bruny Island (where nests were in boxes and natural cavities) and mainland Tasmania regions (all nests in natural cavities, except 1), where Striateds usurped ~15–17% of nests. These areas had greater abundances of Striated Pardalotes than Maria Island, where all nests were in natural cavities and there were no takeovers by Striateds. At the site level, Waterview, on Bruny Island, had the highest frequency of takeovers (15 of 88 nesting attempts) and also had the highest densities of both Forty-spotted (1.20–1.24 pairs ha−1) and Striated pardalotes (0.84–0.96 pairs ha−1). Waterview was also the only site in which most nests were in boxes, and increased densities of both species in response to nest boxes may have resulted in increased competition. Further research is needed to examine the interacting effects of relative population densities, nest-site availability, and habitat quality on competition. Nest box experiments that monitor breeding density, nest success, and interspecific takeovers before and after occupation of nest boxes, and across sites that vary in forest type and cavity availability, are needed to examine the effects of nest boxes on population density and competition.

Striated Pardalotes displayed equally aggressive responses toward both heterospecific and conspecific dummies, suggesting that the perceived relative threat or risk of conflict was similar between both competitor types. Conversely, aggressive responses by Forty-spotted Pardalotes were weaker toward heterospecific dummies (40% of trials involved contact, n = 15) than toward conspecific dummies (87% involved contact, n = 16). I interpret the weaker response of Forty-spotted Pardalotes toward Striated Pardalotes as caution in the face of a relatively threatening competitor. Alternatively, Striated Pardalotes may present less risk to Forty-spotted Pardalotes (i.e. Striated Pardalotes may not be a significant competitor for nest sites, or may be easily deterred) and thus may require less response. However, my observations of takeovers and extensive interspecific chasing and displays centered on nest cavities confirm that there is real contest between the two pardalote species for nest sites. A similar study of interspecific aggression between endangered Gouldian Finches (Chloebia gouldiae) and Long-tailed Finches (Poephila acuticauda) showed that the subordinate Gouldian Finch was more reluctant to approach Long-tailed Finch dummies than Gouldian Finch dummies (Pearce et al. 2011). In this case, interspecific competition resulted in reduced fledging rates and breeding density of Gouldian Finches (Brazill-Boast et al. 2011, 2013).

Although differences in competitive strength can result in the exclusion of weaker competitors from high-quality habitat (Kempenaers and Dhondt 1991, Robinson and Terborgh 1995, Martin and Eadie 1999), sustained competition for cavity resources appears to be relatively common among cavity-nesters with similar body sizes (e.g., Slagsvold 1975, Meek and Robertson 1994). Competitors for nest cavities employ strategies to reduce the degree of competition or to change the balance of competitive strength through subtle shifts in cavity selection (e.g., Robles and Martin 2013) or early timing of breeding (Wiebe 2003). To some extent, Forty-spotted Pardalotes appear to use lower-quality cavities than Striated Pardalotes—for example, those in dead rather than living wood—although the effects of cavity characteristics on breeding success have not been tested (Woinarski and Bulman 1985). As residents, Forty-spotteds are also able to start breeding earlier than Striateds, and they may defend their nests more aggressively with greater investment into eggs and nestlings, or may gain a residency advantage independent of nest defense aggression.

Seventy-nine percent of takeovers of Forty-spotted Pardalote cavities by Striated Pardalotes (n = 19) occurred before the Forty-spotted Pardalotes started laying (during the nest-building period or between nest-building and laying). While some species are capable of usurping the nests of their competitors at any stage of the nesting period by removing or building over the top of eggs or nestlings (e.g., House Wrens [Troglodytes aedon]; Doherty and Grubb 2002), for many species the majority of takeovers appear to occur before eggs are laid or during the laying period (Slagsvold 1975). Forty-spotted Pardalotes start nesting earlier on average than Striated Pardalotes, so differences in takeover risk among nest stages may affect cavity acquisition at the population level in these species. Increasing aggression across nest stages (e.g., due to increased parental investment) is one possible explanation for the decreased takeover rates following egg laying (Trivers 1972). However, there was no change in nest-defense aggression by either pardalote species after egg laying. Thus, the declining risk of takeover was likely due to other factors related to residency advantage. For example, the presence of eggs or nestlings may increase the difficulty of takeover because of the need to eject nest contents or build over the top of nestlings (Krist 2004). Regardless of the mechanisms involved, early onset of breeding seems to force Forty-spotted Pardalotes into frequent conflict with Striated Pardalotes searching for nest sites. It is uncertain whether Forty-spotteds benefit from early cavity acquisition by successfully defending at least some nest sites that would otherwise be claimed by Striated Pardalotes, or whether they suffer high costs of nest building and egg laying in cavities that will inevitably be usurped.

Management of competitors for nest sites can be a key conservation strategy for endangered cavity- and burrow-nesting species (Cade and Temple 1995). Butchart et al. (2006) found that, of 7 obligate cavity-nesters on the brink of extinction during 1994–2004, 4 were likely saved because of the control or removal of competitors for nest sites, or the provision of nest boxes to reduce competition. While removal of Striated Pardalotes is not an option because they are native species, restoration of nest sites through habitat protection and nest box addition is a promising conservation strategy for Forty-spotted Pardalotes. Over multiple decades or centuries, regeneration of mature, complex forests will help to restore the availability of cavities with a range of characteristics to allow partitioning of cavities among competitors (Lindenmayer and Franklin 1997, Robles and Martin 2013). Over shorter time periods, provision of nest boxes may alleviate interspecific competition or create alternative nesting sites for displaced pairs.

However, my results suggest that increased densities at sites with nest boxes may also result in increased interspecific competition. In some cases, increased competition outweighs the benefits of nest boxes, and can have negative effects on the breeding success of the target species (e.g., Finch 1990). In the United States, the decline of Bewick's Wrens (Thryomanes bewickii) was linked to increasing densities of House Wrens, which destroy multiple nests of competitors in their territories, including those with eggs or nestlings (Kennedy and White 1996). Where nest boxes result in increased densities of aggressive competitors such as House Wrens, the net breeding success of their competitors may suffer (Finch 1990). But, unlike House Wrens, Striated Pardalotes do not destroy multiple nests of other species in their territories and are not considered an unusually aggressive species. Rather, the asymmetric tendency for Striateds to be the usurpers is likely due in part to the earlier commencement of breeding and occupation of nest sites by Forty-spotteds. Nonetheless, it will be important to assess the net effects of nest box addition on Forty-spotted Pardalotes, considering the benefits of increased breeding density and nest success relative to the costs of increased competition.

Nest-box addition has had a clearly positive effect for many threatened cavity-nesting birds, and, in systems in which cavity limitation motivates interspecific competition, we would expect nest-box addition to reduce competition for cavities (e.g., Brazill-Boast et al. 2013). However, given the possibility of increased competition with increasing population densities, nest-box design and placement should minimize competition by taking the ecology and behavior of individual species into account (Finch 1990). Distribution of boxes across mixed species and Eucalyptus viminalis–dominated forests might allow for greater habitat partitioning among pardalote species (Woinarski and Rounsevell 1983). Fine-scale cavity-selection preferences such as height or entrance diameter can also be used to reduce competition for nest boxes (Robertson and Rendell 1990, Aitken and Martin 2008). A 6-mm difference in nest-box entrance diameter excludes Eurasian Great Tits (Parus major) and allows access by their subordinate competitors, Blue Tits (Cyanistes caeruleus; Dhondt and Adriaensen 1999). Nest boxes currently in use for pardalotes allow access by both species (entrance diameter = 28–30 mm), but smaller entrance diameters may exclude Striated Pardalotes. Further experiments are needed to assess the impacts of nest-box design, entrance diameter, and forest context on densities and competition between pardalote species.

In general, competition is an important factor to consider in evaluating the effects of habitat management strategies on pardalotes. But the impact of competition must also be considered in the broader context of demographic rates and other threats to Forty-spotted Pardalotes to determine its relevance to their population trajectory.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank Naomi Langmore and Rob Heinsohn for discussions of this study and comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. I also thank Marika Van der Pol, Charlie Governali, George Cummins, Ryan Steiner, Sam Case, Marissa Buschow, and Sean MacDonald for their assistance with fieldwork, and Linda Edworthy, Virginia Abernathy, and Cat Young for their editing and comments on the manuscript. Matt Webb (Department of Primary Industries, Parks, Water, and the Environment, Threatened Species Section), Margaret Vandenberg, and Miriam Fokker provided freeze-dried bird specimens.

Funding statement: This research was funded by Birdlife Australia (Prof Allen Keast Student Research, Emu–Austral Ornithology, and Stuart Leslie Bird Research awards); the Australian Academy of Science (Margaret Middleton Fund for Endangered Australian Vertebrates); the Paddy Pallin Foundation (Terrestrial Conservation Grant); the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (Postgraduate Scholarship); the Australian Federal Government (Australian Postgraduate Award), Sigma Xi (Grant-in-Aid of Research), Ecological Society of Australia (Jill Landsberg Trust Fund Scholarship), and the Holsworth Wildlife Research Fund. None of the funders had any influence on the content of the manuscript, nor did they require approval of the final manuscript prior to publication.

Ethics statement: All methods were approved by the Australian National University's Animal Ethics committee (A2012/34) and the Tasmania Department of Primary Industries, Parks, Water, and the Environment (TFA 13908, TFA 14295).

LITERATURE CITED

1.

K. E. H. Aitkenand K. Martin (2008). Resource selection plasticity and community responses to experimental reduction of a critical resource. Ecology 89:971–980. Google Scholar

2.

BirdLife International (2012). Pardalotus quadragintus. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2013.2.  http://www.iucnredlist.org Google Scholar

3.

J. Brazill-Boast S. R. Prykeand S. C. Griffith (2013). Provisioning habitat with custom-designed nest-boxes increases reproductive success in an endangered finch. Austral Ecology 38:405–412. Google Scholar

4.

J. Brazill-Boast E. van Rooij S. R. Prykeand S. C. Griffith (2011). Interference from Long-tailed Finches constrains reproduction in the endangered Gouldian Finch. Journal of Animal Ecology 80:39–48. Google Scholar

5.

S. Bryant (2010). Conservation assessment of the endangered Forty-spotted Pardalote 2009–2010. Report to Threatened Species DPIPWE Sectionand NRM South Hobart, Tasmania. Google Scholar

6.

S. H. M. Butchart A. J. Stattersfieldand N. J. Collar (2006). How many bird extinctions have we prevented? Oryx 40:266–278. Google Scholar

7.

T. J. Cadeand S. A. Temple (1995). Management of threatened bird species: Evaluation of the hands-on approach. Ibis 137:S161–S172. Google Scholar

8.

C. Cooper W. M. Hochachkaand A. A. Dhondt (2007). Two contrasting natural experiments confirm interspecific competition between House Finches on House Sparrows. Ecology 88:864–870. Google Scholar

9.

A. A. Dhondt (2011). Interspecific Competition in Birds. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. Google Scholar

10.

A. A. Dhondtand F. Adriaensen (1999). Experiments on competition between Great and Blue tit: Effects on Blue Tit reproductive success and population processes. Ostrich 70:39–48. Google Scholar

11.

P. F. Doherty Jrand T. C. Grubb Jr (2002). Nest usurpation is an ‘edge effect' for Carolina Chickadees Poecile carolinensis. Journal of Avian Biology 33:77–82. Google Scholar

12.

D. M. Finch (1990). Effects of predation and competitor interference on nesting success of House Wrens and Tree Swallows. The Condor 92:674–687. Google Scholar

13.

P. A. Gowaty (1981). Aggression of breeding Eastern Bluebirds (Sialia sialis) toward their mates and models of intra- and interspecific intruders. Animal Behaviour 29:1013–1027. Google Scholar

14.

R. Heinsohn S. Murphyand S. Legge (2003). Overlap and competition for nest holes among Eclectus Parrots, Palm Cockatoos and Sulphur-crested Cockatoos. Australian Journal of Zoology 51:81–94. Google Scholar

15.

P. J. Higginsand J. M. Peter (Editors) (2002). Handbook of Australian, New Zealand and Antarctic Birds. Volume 6: Pardalotes to Shrike-Thrushes. Oxford University Press, Melbourne, Australia. Google Scholar

16.

D. J. Ingold (1998). The influence of starlings on flicker reproduction when both naturally excavated cavities and artificial nest boxes are available. Wilson Bulletin 110:218–225. Google Scholar

17.

M. D. Jennionsand P. R. Y. Backwell (1996). Residency and size affect fight duration and outcome in the fiddler crab Uca annulipes. Biological Journal of the Linnaean Society 57:293–306. Google Scholar

18.

J. J. Kappes Jr (1997). Defining cavity-associated interactions between Red-cockaded Woodpeckers and other cavity-dependent species: Interspecific competition or cavity kleptoparasitism? The Auk 114:778–780. Google Scholar

19.

B. Kempenaersand A. A. Dhondt (1991). Competition between Blue and Great tit for roosting sites in winter: An aviary experiment. Ornis Scandinavica 22:73–75. Google Scholar

20.

E. D. Kennedyand D. W. White (1996). Interference competition from House Wrens as a factor in the decline of Bewick's Wrens. Conservation Biology 10:281–284. Google Scholar

21.

R. L. Knightand S. A. Temple (1986). Why does intensity of avian nest defense increase during the nesting cycle? The Auk 103:318–327. Google Scholar

22.

M. Krist (2004). Importance of competition for food and nest-sites in aggressive behaviour of Collared Flycatcher Ficedula albicollis. Bird Study 51:41–47. Google Scholar

23.

N. M. Lairdand J. H. Ware (1982). Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics 38:963–974. Google Scholar

24.

D. B. Lindenmayerand J. F. Franklin (1997). Managing stand structure as part of ecologically sustainable forest management in Australian mountain ash forests. Conservation Biology 11:1053–1068. Google Scholar

25.

K. Martinand J. M. Eadie (1999). Nest webs: A community-wide approach to the management and conservation of cavity-nesting birds. Forest Ecology and Management 115:243–257. Google Scholar

26.

K. Martin K. E. H. Aitkenand K. L. Wiebe (2004). Nest sites and nest webs for cavity-nesting communities in interior British Columbia, Canada: Nest characteristics and niche partitioning. The Condor 106:5–19. Google Scholar

27.

S. B. Meekand R. J. Robertson (1994). Interspecific competition for nestboxes affects mate guarding in Eastern Bluebirds, Sialia sialis. Animal Behaviour 47:295–302. Google Scholar

28.

J. Meriläand D. A. Wiggins (1995). Interspecific competition for nest holes causes adult mortality in the Collared Flycatcher. The Condor 97:445–450. Google Scholar

29.

E. O. Minotand C. M. Perrins (1986). Interspecific interference competition—Nest sites for Blue and Great tits. Journal of Animal Ecology 55:331–350. Google Scholar

30.

S. Nakagawaand H. Schielzeth (2013). A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 42:133–142. Google Scholar

31.

I. Newton (1994). The role of nest sites in limiting the numbers of hole-nesting birds: A review. Biological Conservation 70:265–276. Google Scholar

32.

S. G. Nilsson (1984). The evolution of nest-site selection among hole-nesting birds: The importance of nest predation and competition. Ornis Scandinavica 15:167–175. Google Scholar

33.

D. Pearce S. R. Prykeand S. C. Griffith (2011). Interspecific aggression for nest sites: Model experiments with Long-tailed Finches (Poephila acuticauda) and endangered Gouldian Finches (Erythrura gouldiae). The Auk 128:497–505. Google Scholar

34.

L. Persson (1985). Asymmetrical competition: Are larger animals competitively superior? American Naturalist 126:261–266. Google Scholar

35.

J. Pinheiro D. Bates S. DebRoy D. Sarkar and the R Development Core Team (2013). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-111.  http://www.inside-r.org/packages/nlme Google Scholar

36.

R Development Core Team (2012). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.  http://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar

37.

R. J. Robertsonand W. B. Rendell (1990). A comparison of the breeding ecology of a secondary cavity nesting bird, the Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolor), in nest boxes and natural cavities. Canadian Journal of Zoology 68:1046–1052. Google Scholar

38.

S. K. Robinsonand J. Terborgh (1995). Interspecific aggression and habitat selection by Amazonian birds. Journal of Animal Ecology 64:1–11. Google Scholar

39.

H. Roblesand K. Martin (2013). Resource quantity and quality determine the inter-specific associations between ecosystem engineers and resource users in a cavity-nest web. PLOS One 8:e74694. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074694 Google Scholar

40.

D. E. Rounsevelland J. C. Z. Woinarski (1983). Status and conservation of the Forty-spotted Pardalotes, Pardalotus quadragintus (Aves: Pardalotidae). Australian Wildlife Research 10:343–349. Google Scholar

41.

T. Slagsvold (1975). Competition between the Great Tit Parus major and the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca in the breeding season. Ornis Scandinavica 6:179–190. Google Scholar

42.

T. Slagsvold (1978). Competition between the Great Tit Parus major and the Pied Flycatcher Ficedula hypoleuca: An experiment. Ornis Scandinavica 6:179–190. Google Scholar

43.

R. L. Trivers (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man 1871–1971 ( B. Campbell Editor). Aldine, Chicago, IL, USA. pp. 136–179. Google Scholar

44.

K. L. Wiebe (2001). Microclimate of tree cavity nests: Is it important for reproductive success in Northern Flickers? The Auk 118:412–421. Google Scholar

45.

K. L. Wiebe (2003). Delayed timing as a strategy to avoid nest-site competition: Testing a model using data from starlings and flickers. Oikos 100:291–298. Google Scholar

46.

K. L. Wiebe (2011). Nest sites as limiting resources for cavity-nesting birds in mature forest ecosystems: A review of the evidence. Journal of Field Ornithology 83:239–248. Google Scholar

47.

J. C. Z. Woinarskiand C. Bulman (1985). Ecology and breeding biology of the Forty-spotted Pardalote and other pardalotes on north Bruny Island. Emu 85:106–120. Google Scholar

48.

J. C. Z. Woinarskiand D. E. Rounsevell (1983). Comparative ecology of pardalotes, including the Forty-spotted Pardalote, Pardalotus quadragintus (Aves: Pardalotidae) in south-eastern Tasmania. Australian Wildlife Research 10:351–361. Google Scholar
© 2016 Cooper Ornithological Society.
Amanda B. Edworthy "Competition and aggression for nest cavities between Striated Pardalotes and endangered Forty-spotted Pardalotes," The Condor 118(1), 1-11, (4 November 2015). https://doi.org/10.1650/CONDOR-15-87.1
Received: 20 May 2015; Accepted: 1 August 2015; Published: 4 November 2015
KEYWORDS
cavity-nesting bird
competition
endangered species
interspecific aggression
nest defense
pardalote
tree cavities
Back to Top