Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 April 2005 The Parasite Behavior Hypothesis and the Use of Sleeping Sites by Black Howler Monkeys (Alouatta caraya) in a Discontinuous Forest
Martin Kowalewski, Gabriel E. Zunino
Author Affiliations +

Introduction

Primates are particularly susceptible to parasitic infections because they live in social groups that facilitate their transmission (Stoner, 1996). The costs and benefits of living in smaller and larger social groups have been examined in a number of primate field studies (Struhsaker, 1969; Eisenberg et al., 1972; Clutton-Brock and Harvey, 1977; Van Schaik, 1989; Janson, 1992; Sterck et al., 1997; Chapman and Chapman, 2000; Kappeler and van Schaik, 2002). Several factors influence social group living: 1) the availability, abundance, and distribution of food resources — limiting the number of animals that an area can support or influencing foraging efficiency in groups of different sizes (Wrangham, 1980; Chapman, 1990); 2) historical and social traits, including changes in group size or population size (Stevenson et al., 1998); 3) life history traits such as birth rates, sex ratios, mortality, and dispersal patterns (Altmann, 1980; Dunbar, 1988; Crockett, 1996); 4) predation pressure (Stanford, 2002); 5) cooperation and affiliation among individuals (Sussman and Garber, 2004); and 6) traits related to social organization that are phylogenetically conservative and do not change in different environments (DiFiore and Rendall, 1994). There are few studies that consider parasite transmission as a factor in limiting group size or affecting group structure in social primates (Freeland, 1976; Janson, 2000).

Parasitism has density-dependent costs related to disease transmission; therefore, it may play an important role in increasing the fitness of individuals living in smaller social groups, who benefit from an enhanced amount of grooming. Because parasitic infections can cause a fitness decrease in animals, some parasite-avoidance behaviors (e.g., mammals licking their own fur, auto- and allogrooming, mud wallows, and dust baths) can be expected (Alexander, 1974; Pulliam and Caraco, 1984; Mooring and Hart, 1992; Loehle, 1995). Parasites can directly affect host survival by increasing predation risk or decreasing competitive abilities (Scott, 1988). In addition, if parasite loads affect health and physical appearance, they can influence patterns of female mate choice or the ability of individuals to compete directly for access to sexual partners (Freeland, 1981; Hamilton and Zuk, 1982).

Due to their use and re-use of a limited ranging area, primates living in small forest patches with restricted home ranges will be more exposed to infection and re-infection (increasing the amount of the re-infecting dose) with parasite ova and larvae (Freeland, 1976, 1980; Gilbert, 1997). Behaviors that avoid and/or reduce parasite infections (Freeland, 1980; Hausfater and Meade, 1982) constitute an alternative to physiological immunity (Keymer and Read, 1991) and may contribute to the survival of some individuals. Howler monkeys (Alouatta) host a number of intestinal parasites that are eliminated in their feces (Stuart et al., 1998; Santa Cruz et al., 2000; Muller et al., 2000). This study examines the black howler monkey's (Alouatta caraya) use of defecation and night resting sites as a strategy to avoid parasite re-infection in a forest fragment in northern Argentina.

A number of hypotheses have been offered to explain the selection of sleeping sites in primates (Anderson, 1984; Di Bitetti et al., 2000): 1) Parasite hypothesis: different trees are chosen every night to avoid recontamination with parasites; 2) Predation hypothesis: a) different and inaccessible trees are used so that predators cannot predict the locations of the sleeping sites, or b) contrariwise, the persistent use of the same trees that provide the most effective escape routes from predators; 3) Thermoregulatory hypothesis: energy behavconservation associated with individuals huddling together when it is cold; 4) Social hypothesis: monkeys choose sleeping sites that allow social contact and social bonding; 5) Safety hypothesis: howlers select trees that offer secure and sheltered platforms to sleep in relaxed positions or to avoid severe weather; and 6) Feeding site hypothesis (von Hippel, 1998): monkeys prefer to sleep near or in feeding trees. If howlers select sleeping sites to reduce the chances of parasitic infections then they will defecate in places different from those where they sleep (avoiding the contamination of sites they use often). They should also defecate from low branches in order to avoid sullying supports used as potential traveling routes or sleeping sites.

Methods

Alouatta caraya is an arboreal folivore-frugivore. Its range in northern Argentina marks the extreme southern distribution of the genus (Brown and Zunino, 1994). The study was carried out in a fragment of semideciduous gallery forest in northern Argentina (27°30′s and 58°41′W) in the basin of the Río Riachuelo, a tributary of the Río Paraná (Fig. 1). The area is between 50 and 60 m above sea level. The climate is subtropical, with an annual average temperature of 21.7° C and annual average precipitation of 1230 mm (Servicio Meteorológico Nacional, from 1901–1950). Rains are frequent year-round, but decrease considerably in July and August. The vegetation forms a mosaic of tall and low forests, savannas with palms, grasslands and lowland zones with lagoons and “esteros” (marshes). The primary forest has been and is currently being logged intensively.

Figure 1.

Location of the study site.

i1413-4705-13-1-22-f01.gif

A group of black howlers was followed for 15 days in August 1994 (winter) and 15 days in February 1995 (summer) from sunrise till sunset. There were 10 individuals in the group in the winter period (1 adult male, 2 subadult males, 3 adult females, 1 subadult female, 2 juveniles and 1 infant) and 9 individuals in the summer (1 adult male, 3 subadult males, 2 adult females, 1 subadult female, 1 juvenile and 1 infant). The forest fragment of 8.5 ha was subdivided into 212 quadrates of 20 x 20 m. We recorded the quadrates where the group defecated and slept at night, and then compared the frequencies of quadrate use with a G-test (Sokal and Rohlf, 1995). We also recorded the height and species of trees in which the monkeys defecated and slept. The height differences were analyzed with a Mann-Whitney test. The floristic composition and vegetation structure were taken from Zunino (1986) and Rumiz et al. (1986).

Results

Black howler monkeys defecate 2.63 times a day (sd = 0.49, n = 205 [total number of defecations of all individuals, excluding infants]), generally after resting (when they wake up in the morning and after an afternoon nap) and before going to sleep at night. In 60% of the 205 defecations recorded, the entire group defecated at about the same time. In 21%, all of the individuals but one defecated, and in 19% all but two defecated. The distribution and the frequency of quadrates used for night resting showed that the howlers were selective in the areas used for sleeping sites (site fidelity). They used different quadrates and locations in the forest to sleep and to defecate (GWilliams = 112.36, df =1, p < 0.001). The heights at which they defecated (8.33 ± 2.97 m) and at which they slept (18.07 ± 4.88 m) were significantly different (U = 189, N1 = 110, N2 = 205, p < 0.001). They slept in the crowns of the trees and defecated from the lower branches directly onto the ground. The group used six trees as night resting sites: five Ficus monckii trees (90.1%) and a Tabebuia ipe (9.9%). The troop defecated in 23 trees of nine different species.

Ficus monckii trees were commonly used as both sleeping (90.1%) and defecating sites (35.12%) (Table 1). These fig trees are the largest in this semideciduous forest (Rumiz et al., 1986). The importance of F. monckii may also be its asynchrony in leafing and fruiting phenology, as it thus provides a year-round source of fruits and leaves (Zunino, 1987, 1989). The monkeys fed in these trees before going to sleep at night and when they woke up in the morning. In total, they used six different sleeping sites on the 20 nights of our two study periods, suggesting site fidelity. All group members slept together each night. Three times they used the same tree for three consecutive nights, and three times they used the same tree on two consecutive nights, again indicating site fidelity.

Table 1.

Use of different tree species by A. caraya for defecation, sleeping, and eating.

i1413-4705-13-1-22-t01.gif

Discussion

The differences in the frequency of quadrate use and the heights at which the howlers defecated and slept may well reflect their attempts to diminish contact with feces in areas where they carry out much of their daily activity. Defecating in specific areas without understorey vegetation (low heights) could diminish the individual's chance of infection and re-infection by parasite ova or larvae on sullied branches or the leaves they may later come to eat. Following the hypotheses proposed above, however, the choice of sleeping trees did not appear to be related to parasite avoidance, at least as stated (Hypothesis 1: they used the same trees on consecutive nights), but instead could be related to behaviors designed to decrease predation risk. Braza et al. (1981) described a behavior in A. seniculus in which the monkeys rubbed their anus on a tree branch after defecating, behavior that could expose other group members to parasites. We observed a similar behavior in A. caraya. These howler behaviors do not appear to be consistent with avoiding exposure to parasites.

Sleeping high up in the trees is a common pattern in primates (Anderson, 1984). In this study howlers slept in tall trees characterized by a closed crown. This may be related to reducing predation risk from terrestrial predators while the closed crown minimizes risk from aerial predators. Although black howler monkeys do not have many predators at this site, potential predators include the jaguarondi (Herpailurus yagouaroundi) and dogs (Canis domesticus). We did not observe any predation or predator attacks, and reports of predation on atelines are rare anyway (Di Fiore, 2002). The relationship between the selection of sleeping trees and predation avoidance remains unclear. Selectivity in the trees used as sleeping sites was evident in that they were not the most abundant trees in the forest. As such, the selection of sleeping trees was consistent with a predator avoidance hypothesis: Ficus and Tabeuia trees were the tallest in the forest and possibly provided protection against predators.

We also found evidence in support of the thermoregulation hypothesis: they always slept huddled as a group. The social hypothesis was supported because the large crowns of the trees allowed the group members to sleep together. The safety hypothesis could not be discounted because the selected trees offered large branches and crowns to accommodate the individuals (pers. obs.).

Lastly, the feeding site hypothesis fits because Ficus trees were the major source of food in the howlers' diet (Table 1). Ficus monckii was the most frequent tree used as a sleeping site and the most important species in the black howler monkey diet (Zunino, 1987, 1989), representing 45.8% of the feeding time (Zunino, 1989). The leaves and fruits of F. monckii were available during almost all the year owing to the asynchronous phenology of this species (Zunino, 1986, 1987, 1989). During the winter when other species such as Celtis sp. and Tabebuia ipe (Zunino, 1987, 1989) increase in dietary importance (depending on their phenology), these species also were used as sleeping sites. Although we cannot discount a social function for sleeping site selectivity, the selection of large feeding trees as sleeping sites might best represent a foraging strategy.

Although the selection of sleeping trees was consistent with several alternative hypotheses, it was not consistent with parasite avoidance in so far as they used the same trees for defecating and sleeping on consecutive nights and only six sites during the 20 days of the study. Their tendency to move down in the forest to defecate, and do so in areas with sparse understoreys, however, might well be adaptive in terms of avoiding parasitism.

Other howler species such as Alouatta palliata (v. Dudley and Milton, 1990; Stuart et al., 1990; Stoner, 1996), A. seniculus (v. Braza et al., 1981; Gilbert, 1994, 1997) and A. guariba (v. Stuart et al., 1993) have been recorded showing similar behavior in terms of selectivity of sleeping trees and defecation sites. Braza et al. (1981) reported that A. seniculus defecated directly over the ground as a way to avoid contaminating possible foraging routes. Gilbert (1997) showed that A. seniculus used specific trees to defecate from, defecating from lower branches and avoiding contact with underlying vegetation. Gilbert (1997) argued that defecation site choice may represent a parasite avoidance behavior in red howlers, and that this behavior could contribute to the relatively low abundance of endoparasite infection in howlers (Thatcher and Porter, 1968; Stuart et al., 1990; Gilbert, 1994). Finally, a number of howler species, including black howlers, are reported to show behaviors associated with the reduction of disease transmission, such as the selection of defecation sites near the ground. Phylogeny may play an important role in the evolution of this behavior.

Acknowledgments

We thank Paul A. Garber for thoughtful discussions on the subject and valuable comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. We gratefully acknowledge the support of the Argentine Museum of Natural Sciences (MACN) and the Estación Biológica Corrientes (EBCo). We also thank Miguel Blanco and Ramon Romero for helping us in the field.

References

1.

R. D. Alexander 1974. The evolution of social behavior. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst 5:325–383. Google Scholar

2.

J. Altmann 1980. Baboon Mothers and Infants. Harvard University Press. Cambridge, Massachusetts. Google Scholar

3.

J. R. Anderson 1984. Ethology and ecology of sleep in monkeys and apes. Adv. Stud. Behav 14:165–229. Google Scholar

4.

F. Braza, F. Alvarez, and T. Azcarate . 1981. Behaviour of the red howler monkey (Alouatta seniculus) in the Llanos of Venezuela. Primates 22:4459–473. Google Scholar

5.

A. D. Brown and G. E. Zunino . 1994. Hábitat, distribución y problemas de conservación de los primates de la Argentina. Vida Silvestre Neotropical 3:130–40. Google Scholar

6.

C. A. Chapman 1990. Association patterns of spider monkeys: The influence of ecology and sex on social organization. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 26:409–414. Google Scholar

7.

C. A. Chapman and L. J. Chapman . 2000. Determinants of group size in primates: The importance of travel costs. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups. S. Boinski and P. A. Garber , editors. (eds.), pp. 24–42.The University of Chicago Press. Chicago. Google Scholar

8.

T. H. Clutton-Brock and P. H. Harvey . 1977. Primate ecology and social organization. J. Zool., Lond 183:1–39. Google Scholar

9.

C. M. Crockett 1996. The relation between red howler monkey (Alouatta seniculus) troop size and population growth in two habitats. In Adaptive Radiations of Neotropical Primates. M. A. Norconk, A. L. Rosenberger, and P. A. Garber , editors. (eds.), pp. 489–510.Plenum Press. New York. Google Scholar

10.

M. S. Di Bitetti, E. M. Luengos-Vidal, M. C. Baldovino, and V. Benesovsky . 2000. Sleeping site preferences in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus). Am. J. Primatol 50:257–274. Google Scholar

11.

A. Di Fiore 2002. Predator sensitive foraging in ateline primates. In Eat or Be Eaten: Predator Sensitive Foraging Among Primates. L. E. Miller , editor. (ed.), pp. 242–267.Cambridge University Press. New York. Google Scholar

12.

A. Di Fiore and D. Rendall . 1994. Evolution of social organization: A reappraisal for primates by using phylogenetic methods. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci 91:9941–9945. Google Scholar

13.

R. Dudley and K. Milton . 1990. Parasite deterrence and the energetic costs of slapping in howler monkeys, Alouatta palliata. J. Mammal 71:3463–465. Google Scholar

14.

R. I M. Dunbar 1988. Primate Social Systems. Cornell University Press. Ithaca, NY. Google Scholar

15.

J. F. Eisenberg, N. A. Muckenhirn, and R. Rudran . 1972. The relation between ecology and social structure in primates. Science 176:863–874. Google Scholar

16.

W. J. Freeland 1976. Pathogens and the evolution of primate sociality. Biotropica 8:12–24. Google Scholar

17.

W. J. Freeland 1980. Mangabey (Cercocebus albigena) movement patterns in relation to food availability and fecal contamination. Ecology 61:61297–1303. Google Scholar

18.

W. J. Freeland 1981. Functional aspects of primate grooming. Ohio J. Sci 81:173–177. Google Scholar

19.

K. A. Gilbert 1994. Endoparasitic infection in red howling monkeys (Alouatta seniculus) in the Central Amazonian basin: A cost of sociality. Doctoral thesis. State University of New Jersey. New Brunswick, Rutgers. Google Scholar

20.

K. A. Gilbert 1997. Red howling monkey use of specific defecation sites as a parasite avoidance strategy. Anim. Behav 54:451–455. Google Scholar

21.

W. D. Hamilton and M. Zuk . 1982. Heritable true fitness and bright birds: A role for parasites. Science 218:384–387. Google Scholar

22.

G. Hausfater and B. J. Meade . 1982. Alternation of sleeping groves by yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) as a strategy for parasite avoidance. Primates 23:2287–297. Google Scholar

23.

C. H. Janson 1992. Evolutionary ecology of primate social structure. In Evolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior. E. A. Smith and B. Winterhalder , editors. (eds.), pp. 95–130.De Gruyter. New York. Google Scholar

24.

C. H. Janson 2000. Primate socio-ecology: The end of a golden age. Evol. Anthropol 9:273–86. Google Scholar

25.

P. M. Kappeler and C. P. van Schaik . 2002. Evolution of primate social systems. Int. J. Primatol 23:4707–740. Google Scholar

26.

A. E. Keymer and A. F. Read . 1991. Behavioural ecology: The impact of parasitism. In Parasite-Host Associations, Coexistence or Conflict. C. A. Toft, A. Aeschliman, and L. Bolis , editors. (eds.), pp. 37–61.Oxford University Press. Oxford, UK. Google Scholar

27.

C. Loehle 1995. Social barriers to pathogen transmission in wild animal populations. Ecology 76:2326–335. Google Scholar

28.

M. S. Mooring and B. L. Hart . 1992. Animal grouping for protection from parasites: Selfish herd and encounter-dilution effects. Behaviour 123:3–4173–193. Google Scholar

29.

G. C K. Müller, A. Krambeck, Z. M B. Hirano, and H. H da Silva Filho . 2000. Levantamento preliminar de endoparasitas do tubo digestivo de bugios Alouatta guariba clamitans. Neotrop. Primates 8:3107–108. Google Scholar

30.

H. R. Pulliam and T. Caraco . 1984. Living in groups: Is there an optimal group size. In Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. 2nd edition J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies , editors. (eds.), pp. 122–147.Sinauer Associates. MA. Google Scholar

31.

D. I. Rumiz, G. E. Zunino, M. L. Obregozo, and J. C. Ruiz . 1986. Alouatta caraya: Habitat and resource utilization in northern Argentina. In Current Perspectives in Primate Social Dynamics. D. M. Taub and F. A. King , editors. (eds.), pp. 175–193.Van Nostrand Reinhold. New York. Google Scholar

32.

A. C M. Santa Cruz, J. T. Borda, E. M. Patino, L. Gomez, and G. E. Zunino . 2000. Habitat fragmentation and parasitism in howler monkeys (Alouatta caraya). Neotrop. Primates 8:4146–148. Google Scholar

33.

M. E. Scott 1998. The impact of infection and disease on animal populations: Implications for conservation biology. Conserv. Biol 2:140–56. Google Scholar

34.

R. R. Sokal and F. J. Rohlf . 1995. Biometry. 3rd editionFreeman and Company. New York. Google Scholar

35.

C. B. Stanford 2002. Avoiding predators: Expectations and evidence in primate antipredator behavior. Int. J. Primatol 23:741–757. Google Scholar

36.

E. H M. Sterck, D. P. Watts, and C. P. van Schaik . 1997. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol 41:5291–309. Google Scholar

37.

P. R. Stevenson, M. J. Quiñones, and J. A. Ahumada . 1998. Effects of fruit patch availability on feeding subgroup size and spacing patterns in four primate species at Tinigua National Park, Colombia. Int. J. Primatol 19:2313–324. Google Scholar

38.

K. E. Stoner 1996. Prevalence and intensity of intestinal parasites in mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in northeastern Costa Rica: Implications for conservation biology. Conserv. Biol 10:2539–546. Google Scholar

39.

T. T. Struhsaker 1969. Correlates of ecology and social organization among African cercopithecines. Folia Primatol 11:80–118. Google Scholar

40.

M. D. Stuart, L. L. Greenspan, K. E. Glander, and M. Clarke . 1990. A coprological survey of parasites of wild mantled howling monkeys, Alouatta palliata palliata. J. Wildl. Diseases 26:547–549. Google Scholar

41.

M. D. Stuart, K. B. Strier, and S. M. Pierberg . 1993. A coprological survey of parasites of wild muriquis, Brachyteles arachnoides, and brown howling monkeys, Alouatta fusca. J. Helminthol. Soc. Wash 60:111–115. Google Scholar

42.

M. D. Stuart, V. Pendergast, S. Rumfelt, S. Pierberg, L. Greenspan, K. E. Glander, and M. Clarke . 1998. Parasites of wild howlers (Alouatta spp.). Int. J. Primatol 19:3493–512. Google Scholar

43.

R. W. Sussman and P. A. Garber . 2004. Rethinking sociality: Cooperation and aggression among primates. In Origins and Nature of Sociality. R. W. Sussman and A. R. Chapman , editors. (eds.), pp. 161–190.Aldine de Gruyter. Hawthorne, NY. Google Scholar

44.

V. E. Thatcher and J. A. Porter . 1968. Some helminth parasites of Panamanian primates. Trans. Am. Microscopical Soc 87:186–196. Google Scholar

45.

C. P. van Schaik 1989. The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals. V. Standen and R. A. Foley , editors. (eds.), pp. 195–218.Blackwell Scientific Publications. Oxford. Google Scholar

46.

F. A. von Hippel 1998. Use of sleeping trees by black and white colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza) in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. Am. J. Primatol 43:3281–290. Google Scholar

47.

R. W. Wrangham 1980. An ecological model of femalebonded primate groups. Behaviour 75:262–300. Google Scholar

48.

G. E. Zunino 1986. Observaciones sobre el comportamiento territorial del mono aullador negro (Alouatta caraya). Bol. Primatol. Arg 4:136–52. Google Scholar

49.

G. E. Zunino 1987. Nutrición en primates folívoros: La dieta de Alouatta caraya en vida silvestre. Bol. Primatol. Arg 5:1–278–90. Google Scholar

50.

G. E. Zunino 1989. Hábitat, dieta y actividad del mono aullador negro (Alouatta caraya) en el noreste de la Argentina. Bol. Primatol. Latinoam 1:174–97. Google Scholar

Notes

[1] Martin Kowalewski Department of Anthropology, University of Illinois, 109 Davenport Hall, 607 S. Mathews Avenue, Urbana, Illinois 61801, USA

[2] Martin Kowalewski and Gabriel E. Zunino, Estación Biológica Corrientes – Museo Argentino de Ciencias Naturales (MACN), Av. Angel Gallardo 470, 1405, Buenos Aires, Argentina. E-mail: <mkowalew@uiuc.edu>.

Martin Kowalewski and Gabriel E. Zunino "The Parasite Behavior Hypothesis and the Use of Sleeping Sites by Black Howler Monkeys (Alouatta caraya) in a Discontinuous Forest," Neotropical Primates 13(1), 22-26, (1 April 2005). https://doi.org/10.1896/1413-4705.13.1.22
Published: 1 April 2005
Back to Top