Open Access
How to translate text using browser tools
1 December 2009 Using PIT-Tag Technology to Target Supplementary Feeding Studies
Scott Newey, Peter Allison, Simon J. Thirgood, A. Adam Smith, Isla M. Graham
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

The role of food in limiting or regulating populations of mammalian herbivores remains a central question in ecology with great relevance to wildlife and livestock management. Supplementary feeding studies have been widely used to assess the potentially limiting role of food availability, and supplementary feeding is also a common management technique. In both contexts there is an assumption that all individuals in the target population have access to food. There are, however, questions as to whether supplementary feed reaches the target population and how benefits are translated into individual and population level effects. We describe and use a technique using Passive Induced Transponder (PIT) tags to monitor individual use of supplementary feed in wild mountain hare Lepus timidus populations and test the assumption that supplementary feed reaches the target population. Over the course of one winter only 50% of the target hare population used supplementary feed and there was considerable individual variation in the time spent feeding among those individuals that fed. Neither age, sex nor an index of body condition were significant in explaining which individuals visited feeding stations or how long individuals spent feeding. The method and results described here suggest that, at least for the mountain hare, the central assumption that all target individuals have access to and use supplementary feed is invalid. Great care is thus needed in designing and interpreting the results of supplementary feeding studies or management programmes that include supplementary feeding.

Identifying the factors that limit vertebrate populations is a central question in ecology and critical to the management and conservation of wild populations (Krebs 2002). While there is little doubt that food is ultimately limiting for most vertebrates, other proximate factors may limit population growth before food becomes a limiting factor. The role of food availability as a driver of vertebrate population dynamics remains unclear, primarily because of the difficulties in isolating the impacts of food from other confounding factors (Boutin 1990, Isaac et al. 2004, Wirsing & Murray 2007). Supplementary feeding experiments, where individuals or populations are provided with additional food, have been widely used to study the effects of food on vertebrate population dynamics, demography and behaviour (Karels et al. 2000, Predavec 2000, Thomas & Cuthill 2002, Boonstra & Krebs 2006, Boutin et al. 2006). The use of supplementary food by wild vertebrates is also a widely used technique in wildlife management, particularly to increase animal numbers, density, or local distribution (Hoodless et al. 1999, Putman & Staines 2004, Gonzalez et al. 2006).

Despite the proliferation of supplementary feeding studies, there is little consensus on the role of food availability in vertebrate population dynamics. In a review of 138 studies, Boutin (1990) found that while supplementary feeding had been associated with an increase in population density, earlier breeding, higher birth mass, greater juvenile survival and increased growth rates, overall the results of food-addition experiments had been inconclusive. Furthermore, while there was evidence that many populations were food limited, food addition alone failed to alter the population dynamics in any of the studies reviewed. A number of hypotheses have been suggested to account for the inconclusiveness of food addition studies (Boutin 1990, Wirsing & Murray 2007): 1) factors other than food become limiting after an increase in density; 2) even though food is provided ad libitum, it becomes limiting after an increase in density because the way it is distributed does not allow all individuals access; 3) the true response to food addition is less than the observed increase because most of the increase is due to immigration; 4) food addition studies have not been conducted over long enough periods or large enough areas to have much impact at the population level.

While behavioural studies employing food addition typically use the individual as the study unit (e.g. Thomas & Cuthill 2002), most studies that have investigated the role of food in population dynamics have assessed the effects of food addition at the population level (Boutin 1990). These studies have only considered the average response of the population and have not investigated how behavioural interactions between individuals over food affect how resources are partitioned, how food addition affects individual behaviour, or how the effects of supplementary feeding translate into individual life-history parameters (Dobson & Kjelgaard 1985, Boutin 1990, Klenner & Krebs 1991, Isaac et al. 2004). Concentrating on population level effects is a short-coming of many supplementary feeding experiments as behavioural factors may act to concentrate the effects of food shortage on certain individuals (Clutton-Brock & Albon 1985). Furthermore, few studies have assessed individual variation in the use of supplementary food and generally assume that all individuals have access to and use the food equally (but see Isaac et al. 2004 and Kenward et al. 2005). The logistical difficulty of assessing individual responses to supplementary food, particularly for cryptic or nocturnal species, has almost certainly contributed to the inconclusiveness of supplementary feeding studies.

Passive Induced Transponders

Passive Induced Transponders (PIT) tags consist of an electronic microchip encased in biocompatible glass, are typically 10–25 mm in length and 2–4 mm in diameter, and have been widely used in fisheries and wildlife research to assess habitat use (Greenberg & Giller 2001), movements (Harper & Batzli 1996), feeding behaviour (Isaac et al. 2004, Kenward et al. 2005) and individual life histories (Becker & Wendeln 1997). The application of PIT tags in wildlife research is reviewed by Gibbons & Andrews (2004). The tags are passive in that the tag is dormant until activated by a reader. If a tag is present, the reader generates a close-range, electromagnetic field that causes the tag to transmit a unique alphanumeric code, which is read and stored by the reader. PIT tags are typically injected or surgically implanted into the animal necessitating the use of small tags and consequently limiting read range.

Population dynamics in mountain hares

Throughout their range, mountain hare Lepus timidus populations show regular, large fluctuations in density; in particular Scottish mountain hare populations associated with moorland managed for red grouse Lagopus lagopus scoticus shooting show regular, high amplitude fluctuations in density with a mean 9-year periodicity (Newey et al. 2007). The reasons for these dynamics remain unclear and our current work is investigating the effects of nutrition and parasites on mountain hare population dynamics (Newey & Thirgood 2004, Newey et al. 2005).

Faced with the challenge of assessing individual use of supplementary feed we developed a simple, robust, automated technique to monitor individual use of supplementary food using PIT-tag technology. Here we describe the system used and present results on individual variation in use of supplementary feed.

Material and methods

Our study was undertaken on two private estates managed for red grouse shooting in the Central Highlands of Scotland. The experimental design consisted of four study areas, two on each of the estates, each measuring 500 × 500 m. Hares in two of the study areas, one on each estate, were provisioned at feeding stations. These comprised a covered trough equipped with a sensor array (with three sensors/ array) along each side of the trough, a reader data logger (6-channel HDX Multi-point Decoder) and a 26 Ah, 12 v sealed lead acid (SLA) battery (Wyre Micro Designs Ltd, Lancashire, UK). The other two study areas served as controls. Mountain hares in Scotland have ranges of 10–12 ha (Hulbert et al. 1996, Rao et al. 2003), which equates to a circular area with a radius of 180–190 m. The four feeding stations per fed study area were spaced 200 m apart giving all hares in each study area access to at least one feeding station. Study areas were >1 km from each other to minimise movement between fed and control areas.

Hares were live-trapped in cages and long-nets between September and December 2005. Traps were placed throughout each study area on active hare runs, baited with apple and set at dusk, and checked at first light the following day. Each hare was sexed, aged (juvenile or adult), weighed and fitted with a small uniquely numbered ear-tag (Monel #1, National Band & Tag Co., Kentucky, USA). We equipped 119 hares with a collar-mounted 23 × 4 mm Half Duplex (HDX) PIT tag (Texas Instruments Inc., USA). The use of large externally mounted HDX tags used in this study provided a read range of 4–5 cm ensuring that feeding hares were detected, and at the same time reduced power consumption, allowing one battery to power two sensor arrays, reader and data logger for 8–9 days. Of the 119 PIT-tagged hares (Table 1), 48 were also equipped with collar-mounted radio-transmitters (TW3, Biotrack Ltd, UK). From September 2005 until the end of the study in July 2006, radio-collared hares were located fortnightly to monitor survival and to assess possible movement between study areas. Hares were also live-trapped during March-July 2006.

Feeding stations were replenished with commercial rabbit chow and rolled oats coated with molasses weekly from September 2005 to April 2006. The PIT-tag reader fitted to each feeding station polled each sensor every second and logged the presence of any PIT tag along with date and time. Once a PIT tag was detected by the reader its presence was initially logged every 20 seconds, but due to the high usage of the feeding stations and the initially limited data storage capacity (1,024 entries, this was subsequently increased to >7,000 by using a larger memory module), data were logged at 60-second intervals for the majority of the study. The data were downloaded weekly to a handheld computer.

Assessing body condition of live animals under field conditions is difficult and the subject of considerable debate on the validity of different measures and indices (Krebs & Singleton 1993, Green 2001, Schulte-Hostedde et al. 2001, 2005). Here we use the residuals of a linear regression of body mass against body size indexed by hind foot length as an index of body condition. While this method has been criticised (Green 2001), more recent assessment suggests that the residuals from a standard regression of body mass: size represent a valid index of body condition (Schulte-Hostedde et al. 2001, 2005).

Statistical analysis was conducted in R 2.7.0 (R Development Core Team 2007) following Crawley (2005). The effects of age, sex and body condition on whether individuals used feeding stations or not, and the time individuals spent feeding were assessed using Generalised Linear Models (GLM). Age, sex and body condition index were included in the full model which was then subject to backwards stepwise deletion of the least non-significant terms until only the significant terms remained in the model. There were only two replicates of each treatment, therefore prohibiting the use of random effects models or any meaningful analysis of interaction terms. Study site (i.e. replicate) was therefore included in each analysis as a fixed factor and retained in the model as a ‘nuisance’ variable during model selection, and the analysis was confined to assessing the main effects only.

Table 1.

Details of the number of individuals fitted with PIT tags and radio collars in each of the four study areas, along with the total number of visits and time spent at feeding stations. * indicates that one animal was not included in the analysis, ** that two animals were excluded from the analysis due to missing data, and *** that one animal was excluded from the analysis due to missing data.

t01_405.gif

Those animals that had been logged at a feeding station at least once during the study were classified as having used supplementary feed. We used a GLM with a binomial error term and logit link to assess the effects of sex, age and body condition index on whether or not animals used feeding stations.

The distribution of the total time each animal spent feeding was highly overdispersed, where the variance greatly exceeded the mean. Consequently, to examine the effects of age, sex and body condition on the total time individuals were recorded at a feeding station, we used a negative binomial GLM (Venables & Ripley 1997). To allow for the staggered entry of PIT-tagged individuals into the study and for the death or loss of individuals we used the natural logarithm of the number of weeks an animal was known to be present on a fed site as an offset in the model, thereby essentially analysing the mean weekly time spent feeding. Where there was no telemetry or trapping data to confirm whether an individual was present and alive, we took the last date an animal was recorded at a feeding station to be the last known date to be alive and rounded this up to the nearest whole week.

Results

Of the 70 PIT-tagged hares released in the two areas with supplementary feed, 35 (50%) visited a feeding station at least once (see Table 1). In addition, two hares from control sites visited a feeding station indicating that there was movement between areas despite the distance between the sites (see Table 1). These two animals, along with two animals with missing morphometric data, were excluded from the analyses. On both fed study areas, 50% of both adult and juvenile PIT-tagged males used feeding stations, whereas adult females were consistently more likely to use feeding stations than were juvenile females (Figure 1A). However neither age, sex or body condition index had any significant effect on whether individual hares used supplementary feed (sex: χ2 1 = 0.004, P = 0.95; body condition index: χ2 1 = 0.383, P = 0.54; age: χ2 1=0.259, P = 0.61). There was considerable individual and site variation in the time animals were logged at feeding stations (Fig. 1B). Again, neither age, sex or body condition index had any significant effect on time spent feeding (sex: logratio = 0.07, df = 1,29, P = 0.79; age: log-ratio = 0.60, df = 1, 30, P = 0.44; body condition index: log-ratio = 0.57, df = 1, 31, P = 0.45).

Figure 1.

Proportion of PIT-tagged animals that visited feeding stations (A) and mean weekly (± SE) time (in seconds; B) logged at feeding stations by sex, age and study area. 1 and 2 indicate study sites, Ad = adult and Juv = juvenile.

f01_405.eps

Discussion

Supplementary feeding experiments have been widely used to investigate the role of food in limiting vertebrate populations on the assumption that food addition will increase survival and/or fecundity. However, despite the proliferation of such studies, the role of food availability in vertebrate population dynamics remains unclear. Supplementary feeding experiments have been criticised at a number of levels, in particular the question of whether supplementary feed actually reaches the target population and the difficulty of relating both population and individual responses directly to food addition (Boutin 1990, Wirsing & Murray 2007). The PIT-tag system described here offers a simple, robust and automated method to assess individual use and potentially the individual-level effects of food supplementation and quantifies the proportion of a target population that actually used supplementary feed. The results of our study on mountain hares demonstrate that only a subset of the target population uses the food and that there is large individual variation in the use of food, reinforcing concerns voiced by Boutin (1990) and Wirsing & Murray (2007) and highlighting the importance of understanding food supplementation at the individual level.

We found no evidence for any significant role of sex, age or body condition in explaining why only half of the PIT-tagged hares used supplementary feed. Based on published range sizes of Scottish mountain hares (Hulbert et al. 1996, Rao et al. 2003), feeding stations were spaced 200 m apart to ensure that each hare's range included at least one feeder. Given that some individuals may have been trapped at the edge of their range, the observation that only half of the PIT-tagged hares used feeding stations suggests that some hares either did not find, were prevented from or chose not to use feeders. Some hares used more than one feeder in a study plot which, along with the observation that two PIT-tagged hares from the control plots also found and used feeders, suggests that hare ranging behaviour was greater than previously described, hares were able to find feeders even if located outside of their presumed core range, and finally that hares were not averse to using feeders. The use of feeding stations may have been influenced by the position of the feeder relative to an individual's home range. The radio-telemetry data collected in our study did not allow a meaningful assessment of this hypothesis, and we suggest future work should assess the effect of feeder placement relative to home range on feeder use through radio-telemetry or by increasing feeder density. There is no evidence that mountain hares are territorial or defend resources, and the PIT data show instances of two animals feeding simultaneously at the same feeder (S. Newey, unpubl. data), and thus social exclusion seems unlikely but cannot be ruled out and warrants investigation.

Home range sizes of mountain hares and snowshoe hares Lepus americanus are correlated with food availability (Boutin 1984, Hulbert et al. 1996), which supports the hypothesis that hares forage to minimise risk (Boonstra et al. 1998, Murray 2002). Individual hares that had adequate resources in their home range may have been averse to the risk of using novel resources such as artificial feeding stations. However, without detailed data on forage quality within each hare's home range this remains speculation and there is considerable potential for further work exploring habitat correlates with use of supplementary feed.

Given the large variation in the time that individuals spent using feeding stations, it is perhaps not surprising that our analysis failed to identify any significant explanatory variables. The results do, however, suggest that some individual trait other than age, sex or body condition is more important in determining time spent feeding. Abundance and quality of browse in individual home ranges may account for the individual variation in the time spent at feeding stations. Differences in habitat quality between the two sites may also explain why individuals on Site 1 appear to use feeders more than those on Site 2; we are, however, unable to explore this question further in the current study. There is also a growing awareness that animal personality, which refers to consistent individual differences in behaviour, may play an important role in wild vertebrates influencing among other things boldness and ranging behaviour (Sih et al. 2004, Boon et al. 2007). This could explain why some individuals did not use feeders and the variation in time spent feeding.

In summary, we have described a simple, robust, affordable and practical system to monitor the individual use of supplementary food in vertebrates, and in the mountain hare system at least have demonstrated that there is considerable individual variation in the use of supplementary food. At present this variation cannot be explained by age, sex or body condition. The occurrence of such individual variation means that the common assumption that supplementary feed reaches the entire target population is questionable, requiring care to be exercised in the design and interpretation of supplementary feeding experiments and supplementary feeding programmes for wildlife management and conservation.

Acknowledgements

we thank the sporting estates and gamekeepers for their cooperation, and Colin Beale, Lucy Gilbert, Emily Mockford, Katja Neumann, Mose Nodari and Phil Rycroft for their assistance or advice. This work was funded by the Natural Environment Research Council, The Game & Wildlife Conservation Trust and the Scottish Government Rural and Environment Research and Analysis Directorate.

References

1.

P.H. Becker & H. Wendeln 1997: A new application for transponders in population ecology of the common tern. - Condor 99: 534–538. Google Scholar

2.

A.K. Boon , D. Reale & S. Boutin 2007: The interaction between personality, offspring fitness and food abundance in North American red squirrels. - Ecology Letters 10: 1094–1104. Google Scholar

3.

R. Boonstra , D.S. Hik , G.R. Singleton & A. Tinnikov 1998: The impact of predator-induced stress on the snowshoe hare cycle. - Ecological Monographs 68: 371– 394. Google Scholar

4.

R. Boonstra & C.J. Krebs 2006: Population limitation of the northern red-backed vole in the boreal forests of northern Canada. - Journal of Animal Ecology 75: 1269–1284. Google Scholar

5.

S. Boutin 1984: Effect of Late Winter Food Addition on Numbers and Movements of Snowshoe Hares. Oecologia 62: 393–400. Google Scholar

6.

S. Boutin 1990: Food supplementation experiments with terrestrial vertebrates: patterns, problems and future. Canadian Journal of Zoology 60: 203–220. Google Scholar

7.

S. Boutin , L.A. Wauters , A.G. McAdam , M.M. Humphries , G. Tosi & A.A. Dhondt 2006: Anticipatory reproduction and population growth in seed predators. - Science 314: 1928–1930. Google Scholar

8.

T.H. Clutton-Brock & S.D. Albon 1985: Competition and population regulation in social animals. - In: R.M. Sibly & R.H. Smith (Eds.); Behavioural ecology, ecological consequences of adaptive behaviour. Blackwell Scientific Publications, pp. 557–575. Google Scholar

9.

M.J. Crawley 2005: Statistics: an introduction using R. Wiley, 342 pp. Google Scholar

10.

F.S. Dobson & J.D. Kjelgaard 1985: The influence of food resources on population-dynamics in Columbian ground-squirrels. - Canadian Journal of Zoology 63: 2095–2104. Google Scholar

11.

J.W. Gibbons & K.M. Andrews 2004: PIT tagging: simple technology at its best. - BioScience 54: 447–454. Google Scholar

12.

L. Gonzalez , A. Margalida , R. Sanchez & J. Oria 2006: Supplementary feeding as an effective tool for improving breeding success in the Spanish imperial eagle (Aquila adalberti). - Biological Conservation 129: 477– 486. Google Scholar

13.

A.J. Green 2001: Mass/length residuals: measures of body condition or generators of spurious results? Ecology 82: 1473–1483. Google Scholar

14.

L.A. Greenberg & P.S. Giller 2001 : Individual variation in habitat use and growth of male and female brown trout. - Ecography 24: 212–224. Google Scholar

15.

S.J. Harper & G.O. Batzli 1996: Monitoring use of runways by voles with passive integrated transponders. Journal of Mammalogy 77: 364–369. Google Scholar

16.

A.N. Hoodless , R.A.H. Draycott , M.N. Ludiman & P.A. Robertson 1999: Effects of supplementary feeding on territoriality, breeding success and survival of pheasants. - Journal of Applied Ecology 36: 147–156. Google Scholar

17.

I.A.R. Hulbert , G.R. Iason , D.A. Elston & P.A. Racey 1996: Home-range sizes in a stratified upland of two lagomorphs with different feeding strategies. - Journal of Applied Ecology 33: 1479–1488. Google Scholar

18.

J.L. Isaac , C.N. Johnson , P.J. Grabau & A.K. Krockenberger 2004: Automated feeders: new technology for food supplementation experiments with mammals. Wildlife Research 31: 437–441. Google Scholar

19.

T.J. Karels , A.E. Byrom , R. Boonstra & C.J. Krebs 2000: The interactive effects of food and predators on reproduction and overwinter survival of arctic ground squirrels. - Journal of Animal Ecology 69: 235. Google Scholar

20.

B. Kenward , R.E. Kenward & A. Kacelnik 2005: An automatic technique for selective feeding and logging of individual wild squirrels. - Ethology, Ecology & Evolution 17: 271–277. Google Scholar

21.

W. Klenner & C.J. Krebs 1991: Red squirrel population dynamics. 1. The effect of supplemental food on demography. - Journal of Animal Ecology 60: 961–978. Google Scholar

22.

C.J. Krebs 2002: Two complementary paradigms for analysing population dynamics. - Philosophical Transactions Royal Society of London, Series B 357: 1211–1219. Google Scholar

23.

C.J. Krebs & G.R. Singleton 1993: Indexes of condition for small mammals. - Australian Journal of Zoology 41 : 317–323. Google Scholar

24.

D.L. Murray 2002: Differential body condition and vulnerability to predation in snowshoe hares. - Journal of Animal Ecology 71: 614–625. Google Scholar

25.

S. Newey , D.J. Shaw , A.D. Kirby , P. Montieth , P.J. Hudson & S.J. Thirgood 2005: Prevalence, intensity and aggregation of intestinal parasites in mountain hares and their potential impact on population dynamics. - International Journal for Parasitology 35: 367– 373. Google Scholar

26.

S. Newey & S.J Thirgood 2004: Parasite-mediated reduction in fecundity of mountain hares. - Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 271: S413–S415. Google Scholar

27.

S. Newey , T. Willebrand , D.T. Haydon , F. Dahl , N.J. Aebischer , A.A. Smith & S.J. Thirgood 2007: Do mountain hare populations cycle? - Oikos 116: 1547– 1557. Google Scholar

28.

M. Predavec 2000: Food limitation in Australian desert rodents: experiments using supplementary feeding. Oikos 91: 512–522. Google Scholar

29.

R.J. Putman & B.W. Staines 2004: Supplementary winter feeding of wild red deer Cervus elaphus in Europe and North America: justifications, feeding practice and effectiveness. - Mammal Review 34: 285–306. Google Scholar

30.

S.J. Rao , G.R. Iason , I.A.R. Hulbert & P.A. Racey 2003: The effect of establishing native woodland on habitat selection and ranging of moorland mountain hares (Lepus timidus), a flexible forager. - Journal of Zoology (London) 260: 1–9. Google Scholar

31.

R Development Core Team 2007: R: A language and environment for statistical computing. - R Foundation for statistical computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at:  http://www.r-project.org (Last accessed on 22 April 2008). Google Scholar

32.

A.I. Schulte-Hostedde , J.S. Millar & G.J. Hickling 2001: Evaluating body condition in small mammals. Canadian Journal of Zoology 79: 1021–1029. Google Scholar

33.

A.I. Schulte-Hostedde , B. Zinner , J.S. Millar & G.J. Hickling 2005: Restitution of mass-size residuals: Validating body condition indices. - Ecology 86: 155–163. Google Scholar

34.

A. Sih , A. Bell , J.C. Johnson & R. Ziemba 2004: Behavioural syndromes: an integrative overview. - The Quarterly Review of Biology 79: 241–277. Google Scholar

35.

R.J. Thomas & L.C. Cuthill 2002 Body mass regulation and the daily singing routines of European robins. Animal Behaviour 63: 285–292. Google Scholar

36.

W.N. Venables & B.D. Ripley 1997: Modern applied statistics with S-Plus. - Springer-Verlag, 512 pp. Google Scholar

37.

A.J. Wirsing & D.L. Murray 2007: Food supplementation experiments revisited: verifying that supplemental food is used by its intended recipients. - Canadian Journal of Zoology 85: 679–685. Google Scholar
© Wildlife Biology, NKV www.wildlifebiology.com
Scott Newey, Peter Allison, Simon J. Thirgood, A. Adam Smith, and Isla M. Graham "Using PIT-Tag Technology to Target Supplementary Feeding Studies," Wildlife Biology 15(4), 405-411, (1 December 2009). https://doi.org/10.2981/08-083
Received: 10 December 2008; Accepted: 1 April 2009; Published: 1 December 2009
KEYWORDS
food supplementation
Lepus timidus
mountain hare
PIT tags
resource manipulation
Back to Top